Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 22 March 2023
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Well-Being and Psychological Compensation in an Unstable World View all 7 articles

Effect of Yoga involvement on mental health in times of crisis: A cross-sectional study

  • 1Institute of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria
  • 2Center for Integrative Addiction Research (CIAR), Grüner Kreis Society, Vienna, Austria
  • 3Department of Philosophy, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapeutic Medicine, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria
  • 5Department of Religious Studies, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
  • 6Faculty of Psychotherapy Science, Sigmund Freud University, Vienna, Austria

Background: Yoga involvement can be understood as a degree of immersion in the philosophical/spiritual teachings of Yoga. Previous research has shown a positive association between Yoga involvement and mental health. This study further investigates the effect of Yoga involvement on several parameters of psychological well-being and distress amidst a global crisis precipitated by the COVID-19 pandemic.

Methods: A total sample of 246 participants (216 females; 118 Yoga versus 128 age-matched Pilates practitioners) were included in the study. Via an online-survey the following questionnaires were completed: the WHO-Five Well-Being Index, the Multidimensional Inventory for Religious/Spiritual Well-Being short version, the Leipzig Short Scale of Sense of Coherence, the Brief Symptom Inventory and the Yoga Immersion Scale which was adapted for the control group to Pilates Immersion Scale.

Results: When controlling for occupation and psychiatric disorder, involvement in general was higher for Yoga practitioners than for Pilates controls. Furthermore, Yoga practice was associated with increased religious/spiritual well-being and decreased sense of coherence, but neither with psychological well-being nor with psychological distress. Involvement, in general, positively predicted psychological well-being, religious/spiritual well-being and sense of coherence, but there was no connection with psychological distress. Lastly, involvement mediated the positive relationship between Yoga practice and religious/spiritual well-being and suppressed the negative effect of Yoga practice on sense of coherence.

Conclusion: Our findings suggest that involvement in a certain relaxation practice has a positive impact and might be a key component in understanding Yoga’s influence on mental health. Further randomized controlled research, including clinical groups, is necessary to explain how involvement changes and how it effects well-being more specifically.

Introduction

“The world is upside down, and so am I” is how one participant of this study described their current experience during a headstand.1 The global outbreak of the respiratory coronavirus disease COVID-19 in March 2020, has led to an international public health emergency with profound political, sociocultural and economic implications. The measures taken to contain the pandemic—lockdowns and restrictions in occupational, social, recreational and cultural life—are related to increased chronic stress, sleep disturbances, loneliness, depression, and anxiety (Röhr et al., 2020; Traunmüller et al., 2020). Life satisfaction and the experience of positive feelings are diminished (Zacher and Rudolph, 2021). The pandemic is thus not only a global health crisis but a mental health crisis. The length and scope of the ongoing situation can be understood as a collective trauma (Holman et al., 2022) that is further exacerbated by the economic recession, the recent Russian invasion of Ukraine in February 2022 and consequences of climate change that are “approaching irreversibility” (IPCC, 2022, p. 9). A pressing issue is, therefore, to help people find strategies to cope with the enduring stress throughout challenging times. A discipline that combines exercise with relaxation and could, hence, serve as a suitable coping strategy is the practice of Yoga.

According to Yoga tradition, practitioners strive for greater balance, reduction of suffering and spiritual development (Michelis, 2005). This pursuit is facilitated through a holistic system of techniques, including postures, breathing methods, meditation, self-reflection, and ethical guidelines (Taimni, 2005). Yoga techniques support and build upon each other to achieve a deeper level of consciousness (Taimni, 2005).

One of the underlying mechanisms that encompasses this deeper level of consciousness, with which Yoga practice might mitigate the psychological impact of the crisis, could be the level at which people immerse themselves in their practice—their involvement. Yoga involvement is a measure of how central the identity of as Yoga practitioner is to an individual’s self-concept (Gaiswinkler and Unterrainer, 2016). It is derived from Huber’s (2004) centrality scale for measuring religiosity that is based on Kelly’s (1955) classic Personal Construct Theory. Accordingly, a high level of involvement means that the identity as a Yoga practitioner plays a central role in their personal construct system, which corresponds to an increased intrinsic motivation (Gaiswinkler et al., 2015). This depth of involvement into the practice might help to explain why and how yoga practice fosters different aspects of mental health.

An ever-growing body of research attests to Yoga’s psychological benefits, including an increase of overall well-being (Woodyard, 2011; Trulson and Vernon, 2019), possibly because different aspects of the practice (e.g., physical exertion, breathwork, meditation) strengthen various psychological resources such as mindfulness, body consciousness, self-transcendence, social connectedness and spirituality which in turn foster positive emotional experiences (Park et al., 2020). Despite these associations, research is not as extensive as one might expect (Hendriks et al., 2017) and especially research concerning the exact mechanisms of Yoga is limited. Additionally, there have been methodological concerns, including a shortage of randomized controlled trials, small sample sizes, and difficulty in fully capturing the complexity of the practice (Jeter et al., 2015). A systematic review found that Yoga practice increased positive affect in some studies but had no significant influence on positive affect in others (Domingues, 2018). Throughout COVID-19 lockdown, Yoga practitioners experienced higher well-being than both non-practitioners and practitioners of other spiritual practices (Sahni et al., 2021) and an online Yoga intervention was able to increase participant’s well-being when compared to a waitlist control group (Wadhen and Cartwright, 2021). Aside from the general effect of Yoga, Yoga involvement is associated with feelings of happiness (Gaiswinkler et al., 2015) and with harmonious passion (Domingues and Carmo, 2021).

Religious/spiritual well-being (RSWB) has been defined as “the ability to experience and integrate meaning and purpose in existence through a connectedness with self, others, or a power greater than oneself” (Unterrainer et al., 2011, p. 360). A systematic review discovered that Yoga seems to be associated with spirituality and more specifically with spiritual aspirations, a search for wisdom, an integrative worldview, a sense of meaning and peace, faith, hope and compassion (Csala et al., 2021). Moreover, It has been found that higher involvement in the Yoga practice is related to an increased overall RSWB and to all of its subscales (Gaiswinkler and Unterrainer, 2016). Correspondingly, an intense practice might positively influence a Yoga practitioner’s spirituality, mood and mindfulness (Büssing et al., 2012a) and a regular practice might be necessary to reap the spiritual benefits of Yoga (Csala et al., 2021).

Increasing and maintaining well-being is also the subject of Antonovsky’s Salutogenesis model (1987, 1991) that encompasses preconditions and promotion of health Sense of coherence (SOC) is the core concept of Salutogenesis and refers to the feeling that there is predictability and meaning in life, that things will turn out as they should (Antonovsky, 1988). It comprises three components: Understandability, Manageability and Meaningfulness. A pronounced SOC is strongly related to various predictors of health (specifically mental health) and of life satisfaction (Eriksson and Lindström, 2006, 2007). Originally, Antonovsky proposed that SOC does not change after it has been developed in youth, however, current research suggests that SOC can shift over time (Feldt et al., 2003) and probably even be modified by religious/spiritual interventions (Jeserich, 2013). Since the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic, people reported feeling a loss of control, freedom and predictability (Zhai and Du, 2020; Statz et al., 2022). Since SOC captures these states, it would be useful to know whether Yoga involvement has an impact on them. Although literature on the relationship between SOC and Yoga is limited, promising findings are emerging. A small study (N = 15) was able to show that Hatha-Yoga practitioners have a higher SOC than non-practitioners (Gwiaździński et al., 2017). Furthermore, a 2-week Mindfulness-based meditation therapy (which included breathing exercises, Yoga, exercise, and meditation) significantly increased SOC and, particularly, sense of purpose in a group of nurses (Ando et al., 2011). The association between Yoga involvement and SOC has yet to be investigated.

The relationship between Yoga practice and psychological distress is complex. Yoga practice has been found to alleviate psychological symptoms (Büssing et al., 2012b). However, Yoga practitioners also displayed higher rates of depression and anxiety than a representative sample (Birdee et al., 2008) and had more frequent contact with mental health services (Fouladbakhsh and Stommel, 2010). An possible explanation could be that particularly vulnerable people might be more likely to pursue Yoga to soothe their symptoms (Orlygsdottir et al., 2021) because treatment of mental conditions is often given as a reason to why people practice Yoga (Birdee et al., 2008). COVID-19 containment measures are associated with chronic stress, sleep disturbance, loneliness, depression and anxiety (Röhr et al., 2020). Causes of these symptoms include social isolation, financial hardships and loss of control (Pietrabissa and Simpson, 2020). People in quarantine are often plagued by insomnia (Röhr et al., 2020). Yoga may provide relief, as a correlation has been found between regular Yoga practice and improved quality of sleep (Wang et al., 2020). This may be due to the exertion during Yoga postures along with improved tissue blood flow and oxygenation through breathing exercises (Bankar et al., 2013). Besides sleep problems, stress levels increased under COVID-19 related restrictions. Yoga reduces stress by regulating the sympathetic nervous system (Pascoe et al., 2017). In correspondence to this, a meta-analysis found a positive effect of Yoga on both depressive symptoms and depressive disorders (Cramer et al., 2013). Other meta-analyses showed evidence of Yoga easing anxiety (Li and Goldsmith, 2012; Cramer et al., 2018). In addition, not only Yoga but also a high Yoga involvement has been found to predict lower levels of psychological distress, especially for anxiety and depression symptoms (Gaiswinkler and Unterrainer, 2016), and may help to buffer the negative effects of the current crisis and alleviate psychological symptoms.

Research aims and hypotheses

The present study seeks to assess the role of involvement in the effect of Yoga on various aspects on mental health: psychological well-being, RSWB, SOC and psychological distress. For this purpose, we conducted a cross-sectional online survey that compares Yoga practitioners to Pilates controls. Due to the spiritual and meditative nature of Yoga, we expect (1) that the involvement in the Yoga group is higher than in the Pilates group. In line with previous research, we assume a practice with a spiritual and meditative dimension should be more deeply anchored in the personality construct of the practitioners than a practice that is mainly physical (Gaiswinkler and Unterrainer, 2016).

Additionally, we hypothesize that (2) a higher involvement in the respective practice is associated with an increased general and spiritual well-being (Gaiswinkler and Unterrainer, 2016), an increased SOC, and decreased psychiatric distress. Given the expected positive association between group membership (Yoga versus Pilates group) with involvement and the expected positive association between involvement and psychological well-being, RSWB and SOC and the expected negative association between involvement and psychological distress, we hypothesize (3) a mediation effect in which the positive association of Yoga with psychological well-being, RSWB and SOC and the negative association of Yoga with psychological distress occurs via increased involvement. In a nutshell, we propose that involvement might be a key mechanism through which Yoga practice promotes mental health.

The current study also has an exploratory component that investigates potential correlates for increased involvement. Since increased Yoga involvement corresponds to a high centrality of Yoga in the self-concept (Gaiswinkler and Unterrainer, 2016), motivations for Yoga practice that are more central to the practitioner’s identity should also lead to an increased involvement in the Yoga practice. For example, people who practice Yoga for spiritual reasons or for self-discovery should be more immersed in their practice than people who want to lose weight.

Methods

Participants and procedure

In total, 424 participants were recruited online via convenience sampling (required sample size estimation based on Fritz and MacKinnon, 2007). For this purpose, a link to the study was posted to practice-specific message boards, e-mailed to Yoga and Pilates studios, and distributed via the e-mail list of the University of Graz over 4 weeks. To increase the validity of the results, only subjects who did their practice of choice a total of at least five times and the practice of the opposing condition no more than five times since the beginning of the pandemic could partake (“Have you practiced Pilates/Yoga five or more times since the beginning of the COVID-19 Pandemic?”). In addition, only participants located in either Germany, Austria, or Switzerland were included, since these countries’ epidemiological demands throughout the COVID-19 pandemic and their outcomes were comparable (Desson et al., 2020).

Participants were assigned to one of two conditions determined by their preferred type of practice (Yoga or Pilates). The study itself comprised an online questionnaire in which informed consent was obtained first, followed by screen-out questions, socio-demographic questions, crisis-, health-, and practice-related questions and questionnaires. The survey took about 10 min to complete. As compensation, participants could enter a raffle for one gift certificate (€50). In addition, psychology students at the University of Graz could receive class credit for their participation. Approval for the study was granted by the Ethics Committee of the University of Graz.

Thus far, Yoga involvement has only been examined with a heterogeneous control group of various gymnastic styles (Aerobics, pelvic floor exercises, gymnastics for the spinal column and Pilates) in which the Yoga group was split into three subgroups: low, medium, and high involvement (Gaiswinkler and Unterrainer, 2016). Although Yoga involvement has been defined in the same study as the “extent of personal involvement into the teachings of Yoga running on a continuum” (p. 124), no research so far investigated whether involvement predicts mental health outcomes when it is treated as a linear variable. Additionally, a thorough comparison with a more homogenous control has not been attempted yet. Pilates was chosen as the control group because active control groups are recommended (Park et al., 2014) and because Pilates is a physically similar, yet non-spiritual practice (Yashika and Vasanthi, 2019). Previous research by Gaiswinkler and Unterrainer (2016) compared the effect of Yoga involvement on different indicators of mental health but did not extend the concept of involvement to the control group. This study, therefore, compares levels of involvement between Yoga and Pilates participants.

Psychometric assessment

Socio-demographic information such as age, gender, marital status, and religious affiliation was obtained. To determine whether the two groups were similar across all relevant contexts, we asked about participants’ circumstances during the pandemic, their health and their practice. Pandemic-related questions included system relevance of occupation, demands during the pandemic and the extent of urbanization of the participant’s residence. Health-related questions addressed overall health status, presence of physical or mental illness and additional exercise. Practice-related questions assessed length, frequency and the main reason for starting the practice.

The Yoga Immersion Scale (YI-S; Gaiswinkler et al., 2015) is a self-report measure that assesses the degree of involvement in the Yoga practice. The test includes 10 items answered on a 6-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 6 = strongly agree). The scale displays high internal consistency (α = 0.91; Gaiswinkler et al., 2015). For the control group, the scale was renamed to Pilates Immersion Scale (PI-S), the word “Yoga” was replaced by “Pilates,” and two of the items were secularized: “The theoretical concepts of Pilates [wisdoms of Yoga] also influence my other ways of looking at things in everyday life” and “Studying the historical sources of Pilates [wisdoms of Yoga] helps me a lot.” (see supplementary materials for the full list of items; original German language and English translation).

The WHO-Five Well-Being Index (WHO-5; Version II; Bech et al., 2003; German version by Singer and Brähler, 2007) is a unidimensional scale that uses five items to estimate well-being within the last 2 weeks including mood, vitality, and general interests. Items are scored on a 6-point Likert scale (0 = at no time to 5 = all of the time), with a higher total score indicating greater psychological well-being. Items include: “I have felt cheerful and in good spirits” and “My daily life has been filled with things that interest me.” The measurement instrument is characterized by very good internal consistency (α = 0.92) and high construct validity (Brähler et al., 2007).

The short version of the Multidimensional Inventory for Religious/Spiritual Well-Being (MI-RSWB 12; Unterrainer and Kapfhammer, 2014, see also Vuzic et al., 2022) is based on the MI-RSWB long version with 48 items (Unterrainer et al., 2010; see Unterrainer et al., 2011 for the full list of English items) and measures the total of RSWB. It contains 12 items which are measured on a 6-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 6 = strongly agree). Four subscales (General Religiosity, Hope, Forgiveness, Connectedness,) as well as a total RSWB score can be calculated. Example items are “I have experienced the feeling of being absorbed into something greater” and “I view the future with optimism.” The total score as well as the sub-scales were found to be sufficiently reliable (total score α = 0.76, ω = 0.77; sub-scales α > 0.6; Unterrainer and Kapfhammer, 2014; Fuchshuber and Unterrainer, 2021).

The Leipzig Short Scale (SOC-L9; Schumacher et al., 2000) based on the Sense of Coherence Scale (SOC; Antonovsky, 1993) measures SOC. The questionnaire contains nine items that are answered on a 7-point Likert scale. The ends of the scale are each anchored with different verbal response options. A higher score indicates a more pronounced SOC. Example items: “Do you feel that you are in an unfamiliar situation and do not know what to do?” (1 = very often to 7 = very rarely or never) and “You expect in the future that your own life will be” (1 = without any meaning or purpose to 7 = full of meaning and purpose). Satisfying internal consistency was found for the instrument (α = 0.84; Schumacher et al., 2000).

The short form (BSI-18; Derogatis, 2000) of the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Derogatis and Melisaratos, 1983; German version by Spitzer et al., 2011) is a highly established instrument to assess psychological distress within the past 7 days. The inventory includes three subscales, Somatization, Depressiveness, and Anxiety, and has a total amount of 18 items that are rated using a 5-point Likert scale (0 = not at all to 4 = very much). In addition to the three subscales, a total score Global Severity Index (GSI) of all items can be calculated, which signifies the overall level of psychological distress. Examples of symptoms include “Feeling lonely” and “Spells of terror or panic.” The BSI-18 is a reliable and valid instrument (α = 0.93; Franke et al., 2017).

Statistical analysis

Data were analyzed with IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 28). The items of the different scales were pooled into a total score for each measurement (involvement, psychological well-being, RSWB, SOC, psychological distress). Group differences in the potential control variables were analyzed with t-tests for independent groups for metric variables, Mann–Whitney-U tests for ordinal variables and chi-square tests of independence for categorical variables. Pearson correlations between the main variables were calculated and Bonferroni correction was applied to correct for multiple testing. Prior to conducting the mediation analyses, assumptions were checked including linearity, normality, homoscedasticity, multicollinearity, uncorrelatedness of residuals and absence of extreme outliers. To detect mediation effects of involvement on the relationship between practice type and psychological well-being, RSWB, SOC and psychological distress, the fourth model of the PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2022) was used, which calculates confidence intervals with a bootstrap method. The number of bootstrap samples was set at 5,000. For the exploratory analysis, t-tests for different reasons and Tukey-HSD for post-hoc comparisons were conducted. The rejection level for all analyses was set at α = 0.05. Tests were two-tailed.

A comparison of the two groups revealed a large age gap between the Yoga (M = 34.1, SD = 15.6) and Pilates group (M = 47.1, SD = 13.2), t(401.7) = −9.16, p < 0.001, d = −0.88, probably due to sampling bias because of convenience sampling. As the SOC-L9 is age dependent (Schumacher et al., 2000) and because age influences all dependent variables (Nilsson et al., 2010; López Ulloa et al., 2013; Moleiro et al., 2013; Dadfar et al., 2021) participants were matched by age to within 5 years. Therewith, we could ensure that both groups are comparable and avoid spurious effects due to age (Bland and Altman, 1994).

After matching, the Yoga and Pilates groups differed only in the potential control variables occupation (χ2 [7, N = 256] = 0.184, p = 0.01), psychiatric disorder (χ2 [1, N = 255] = 0.246, p = 0.003) and sport frequency (U = 7,011, z = −2.47, p = 0.01). Of those three, only occupation and psychiatric disorder were included as covariates because they seemed to be confounders, since they had the highest impact on most dependent variables. Occupation was related to SOC (Volanen et al., 2004), and to psychological well-being and psychological distress, especially during COVID-19 (OECD, 2021). Psychiatric disorders have a negative impact on psychological well-being, RSWB and SOC (Larsson and Kallenberg, 1996; Ristkari et al., 2008; Bartels et al., 2013; Unterrainer et al., 2014). They are closely intertwined with psychological distress, as psychological distress may signal disorders like depression or anxiety (Viertiö et al., 2021). However, one can experience psychological distress without the diagnosis of a psychiatric disorder. Because psychiatric diagnoses are more common among Yoga practitioners (Birdee et al., 2008) and because they often use Yoga to alleviate symptoms, we deemed it essential to include it as a covariate to reveal the effects of the different practices on psychological distress without the influence of preconditions.

Sport frequency was not accounted for, as higher levels in the Pilates group (compared to the Yoga group) could be an effect of practicing Pilates and, therefore, a downstream effect, not a confounder (Rosenbaum, 1984). Nine participants were excluded because of missing data in the covariates. After analyzing univariate and multivariate outliers, five more participants were excluded because they either appeared to be in a state of acute crisis (e.g., suicidal thoughts, panic attacks) or because they faced extreme demands (death and care of relatives as well as job loss). Normality was violated for psychological well-being (wide, left-skewed) and psychological distress (narrow and right-skewed). However, confidence intervals generated by bootstrapping do not require normality (Haukoos and Lewis, 2005; Pek et al., 2018). The scatter plot of standardized indicated a violation of the homoscedasticity assumption for psychological distress. Consequently, we applied the heteroscedasticity-robust standard error introduced by Davidson and MacKinnon to calculate that mediation (HC3; 1993), as it is the most reliable (Cribari-Neto et al., 2005).

Results

Sample and descriptive statistics

The final sample comprised 246 participants (216 women) whose ages ranged from 19 to 78 years (M = 45.0, SD = 15.0). Of those, 118 participants were in the Yoga group and 128 in the Pilates group. Table 1 displays socio-demographic variables for each condition and the differences between the groups. Pearson correlations between the main variables are displayed in Table 2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sociodemographic data with group differences.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlations between study variables (N = 249).

Mediation analyses

The study was conducted to examine the effect of Yoga practice on different indicators of mental health as mediated by involvement in the practice. We hypothesized that practicing Yoga would positively affect involvement, which in turn positively affects psychological well-being, RSWB and SOC and negatively affects psychological distress. Table 3 and Figure 1 display the different mediation models.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mediation analyses of study variables.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mediation Models of the Influence of Involvement on the Relationship between Practice Group (Yoga or Pilates) and Psychological Well-Being, Religious/Spiritual Well-Being, Sense of Coherence and Psychological Distress. Standardized regression coefficients for the relationship between practice group and psychological well-being, religious/spiritual well-being, sense of coherence, psychological distress as mediated by involvement. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001.

When controlling for presence of a psychiatric disorder and occupational status, Yoga practice positively predicted RSWB when compared to Pilates practice (b = −3.47, t(237) = −2.62, p = 0.01). However, contrary to expectations, it negatively predicted SOC (b = 3.11, t(236) = 2.79, p = 0.01). Yoga practice per se did neither predict psychological well-being nor psychological distress. Being part of the Yoga group predicted higher levels of involvement (b = −12,25, t(237) = −7,92, p < 0.001) than being part of the Pilates group. Involvement positively affected psychological well-being (b = 0.05, t(236) = 2.13 p = 0.03), RSWB (b = 0.48, t(236) = 10.35, p < 0.001) and SOC (b = 0.15, t(236) = 3.53, p < 0.001). It did not predict psychological distress.

When analyzing the mediation of involvement on the relationship between practice group and psychological well-being, neither the total (b = −0.31, t(237) = −0.52 p = 0.61) nor the direct effect were significant (b = 0.35, t(236) = 0.51 p = 0.61). However, the indirect effect was significant (−0.66, SE = 0.07, 95% CI [−0.28, −0.02]), which would indicate a mediation. Involvement mediated the relationship between group membership and RSWB (ab = −5.86, SE = −0.96, 95% CI [−7.95, −4.14]). The effect of Yoga practice on RSWB was not significant anymore when involvement was included in the model (b = 2.39, t(236) = 1.93, p = 0.55). This means that the positive relationship between Yoga practice and RSWB was fully mediated by involvement in the practice. For the putative relationship between practice group and SOC, the total effect was nonsignificant (b = 1.30, t(237) = 1.02, p = 0.20) but the mediated effect was significant (ab = −1.80, SE = −0.56, 95% CI [−3.02, −0.78]), which suggests an inconsistent mediation or suppression model (MacKinnon et al., 2000). According to that, Yoga practice would negatively influence SOC if involvement would not suppress its effect. Finally, there was no mediation of involvement for the relationship between practice group and psychological distress (ab = 0.24, SE = 0.42, 95% CI [−0.53, 1.12]).

Exploratory analyses

In order to explore the cause for higher involvement in the Yoga group more deeply, we examined the participants’ reasons for starting the practice. Groups differed significantly in their primary motivation for starting the practice (χ2[12, N = 246] = 99.22, p < 0.001). Hereby, we found that Yoga practitioners were more likely to practice for spiritual reasons and for reasons more central to the practitioner’s self-construct (such as spirituality and finding oneself) when compared to the Pilates control group. Seven participants in the Yoga group stated that their primary motivation for starting the practice was spiritual, while none in the Pilates group did. Moreover, the reason for practice was significantly associated with levels of involvement (F(12, 246) = 4.99, p < 0.001). Post-hoc analysis revealed that individuals who practiced for spiritual reasons reported higher levels of involvement than people who practiced to better deal with health problems (p = 0.01), for flexibility (p = 0.004), movement (p = 0.03), fitness (p < 0.001), or fun (p =< 0.001). Individuals who practiced for self-discovery reported higher levels of involvement than people who practiced to better deal with health problems (p = 0.02), for flexibility (p = 0.01), movement (p = 0.05), or fitness (p < 0.001). The remaining differences were not significant.

Discussion

This study was conducted to determine whether and how involvement mediates the effects of Yoga on different aspects of mental health, including psychological well-being, RSWB, psychological distress and SOC when compared to a Pilates control group. We expected more deeply involved individuals to experience increased mental health benefits. As predicted, the Yoga group was more involved in their practice than the Pilates control group, which endorses the assumption that a spiritual practice might lead to a deeper embeddedness than a non-spiritual practice (Gaiswinkler et al., 2015). Accordingly, participants in the Yoga group were more likely to start practicing for spirituality or self-discovery than participants in the Pilates group and practicing for these reasons was generally related to higher levels of involvement. Yoga as a practice, therefore, appears to be a more identity-forming than Pilates and seems to profoundly shape how a practitioner experiences the world.

The relationship between yoga and mental health

In contrast to our assumptions, the Yoga group did not experience higher levels of psychological well-being than the Pilates group. However, Yoga practitioners were more immersed in their practice, which in turn was associated with an increased well-being. This is in line with general findings that Yoga facilitates well-being in general (Woodyard, 2011), and in the pandemic especially (Sahni et al., 2021). One explanation for the insignificant overall effect might be that the study did not have enough power to detect an overall effect, and a larger sample would have yielded a mediation with a significant overall effect (Kenny and Judd, 2014).

As expected, involvement mediated the positive relationship between Yoga practice and RSWB. We even found a full mediation, explaining indicating that involvement is the key factor in explaining the higher levels of RSWB in the Yoga group. Consistent with our model and prior research, these results suggest that involved Yoga practitioners experience higher levels of RSWB (Gaiswinkler and Unterrainer, 2016) than Pilates practitioners. This is probably because they engage in a spiritual practice resulting in higher involvement, which in turn elevates RSWB. Consistent with this are also the findings that an intense practice deepens the spirituality of Yoga practitioners (Büssing et al., 2012a). In the secularized West, where religious practice and identification have waned (Molteni and Biolcati, 2018), a spiritual practice such as Yoga may provide similar mental health benefits (see Csala et al., 2021 for further discussion). Contrary to expectations, Yoga practice was associated with a lower SOC than Pilates practice but with higher levels of involvement, and with because involvement increased SOC, the potential negative effect of Yoga practice on SOC was suppressed by involvement. Our exploratory findings provide some insight into these unexpected results, since we discovered that yogis and yoginis practice more frequently with the intention of fostering spirituality and finding themselves. Yoga practitioners are more likely to suffer from depression and anxiety (Birdee et al., 2008), which are linked to a low sense of coherence (Carstens and Spangenberg, 1997). People who start to practice Yoga for these reasons may lack sense of coherence and wish to compensate for it. Perhaps then, because Yoga practice has been associated with an increasing increased sense of coherence (Ando et al., 2011; Gwiaździński et al., 2017), Yoga’s propensity for higher involvement may have buffered preexisting negative levels of sense of coherence. On the other hand, true causal effects of practice differences may be reflected. Hereby, either Pilates improves sense of coherence more than Yoga does or Yoga diminishes sense of coherence, since meditative states achieved by Yoga practice might mirror some aspects of dissociative states (Waelde et al., 2004), which could impair a sense of comprehensibility or meaningfulness.

Contrary to previous research, Yoga practice was not significantly associated with lower levels of psychological distress. Yoga was associated with increased levels of involvement, but higher involvement did not lead to lower levels of psychological distress. Although we controlled for psychiatric disorders, it is entirely possible that yoga practitioners may have higher levels of psychological distress without meeting the threshold for a diagnosis, as vulnerable individuals may choose yoga to soothe their symptoms (Orlygsdottir et al., 2021). If Yoga practice and, more specifically, Yoga involvement alleviates symptoms and anxiety as expected (Li and Goldsmith, 2012; Cramer et al., 2013; Gaiswinkler and Unterrainer, 2016; Cramer et al., 2018), both effects might cancel each other out. Possibly, as suggested by Gaiswinkler and Unterrainer (2016), only highly involved practitioners experience relief of psychological distress.

Limitations and future research

The biggest limitation of the study is the correlative, cross-sectional design, which restricts conclusions regarding causal relationships (van der Stede, 2014). It would be possible, too, that people who experience higher levels of well-being find it easier to involve themselves in their practice, not vice versa (Gaiswinkler and Unterrainer, 2016). We did, however, compare the Yoga group to an age-matched control group and examined several possible confounders to have higher confidence in the directionality of our findings. To obtain more accurate measures, future studies could additionally consider the different styles of Yoga and Pilates, as well as status as a teacher or student.

Furthermore, data were obtained solely through self-assessment, allowing participants to partake from home with minimal time commitment and risk of being exposed to COVID-19 but possibly inducing common-method bias. For a follow-up study, a longitudinal design where biological parameters (e.g., cardiovascular activity or imaging techniques) are considered would be highly desirable (Balasubramaniam et al., 2012). Randomly assigned groups instead of convenience sampling would increase internal validity and ensure that groups do not differ as greatly in age (Joshi and Singh, 2020). To establish a causal relationship, it would be vital to obtain two measurements (pre-and post-treatment) and calculate the difference between them. The Pilates control group was more homogenous than in previous research, single-blinded intervention involving some sort of “quasi-Yoga,” that does neither include spirituality, nor breath control might help more carefully analyze specific aspects of Yoga. However, it might be challenging to isolate basic elements of the practice (Balasubramaniam et al., 2012).

Finally, future research could explore what exactly causes higher levels of involvement besides the reason for starting the practice. The relationship with the instructor, a sense of community, private versus public practice, different personality traits (especially conscientiousness, openness, and neuroticism), self-compassion, and/or mindfulness may be of particular interest (Gaiswinkler et al., 2020). Since involvement predicts various indicators of well-being, further research could alter these possible predictors of involvement to intervene and heighten involvement. Involvement might even be useful in a clinical setting, where facilitating an involved practice could promote established positive effects of Yoga on depression, anxiety, trauma, somatic symptom disorders and schizophrenia (Forfylow, 2011; Balasubramaniam et al., 2012; Nguyen-Feng et al., 2019; Simon et al., 2021; Tibbitts et al., 2021), thereby serving as a therapeutic adjunct in line with traditional psychotropic medication and psychotherapy.

Conclusion

In this study, Yoga involvement could be further confirmed as a useful construct to explain how Yoga practice influences various states of mental health and illness. As expected, involvement was higher in the Yoga group and predicted several indicators of well-being. A full mediation was only found for RSWB, while involvement buffered potential negative effects of practicing Yoga on SOC. Yoga and involvement may not be the panacea for all varieties of psychological difficulties caused by current stressors. However, both could provide an important piece of the puzzle, as part of a holistic clinical treatment approach.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of the University of Graz. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

CB and H-FU conceptualized the study. CB collected, analyzed and interpreted the data and drafted the first version of the manuscript. H-FU supervised the writing of the manuscript. JF, LG, and AS critically reviewed the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^The participant’s consent to publish their statement was obtained.

References

Ando, M., Natsume, T., Kukihara, H., Shibata, H., and Ito, S. (2011). Efficacy of mindfulness-based meditation therapy on the sense of coherence and mental health of nurses. Health 03, 118–122. doi: 10.4236/health.2011.32022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonovsky, A. (1987). “Health promoting factors at work” in Psychosocial factors at work and their relation to health. eds. C. L. Cooper, R. Kalimo, and M. El-Batawi (Geneva: WHO), 153–167.

Google Scholar

Antonovsky, A. (1988). Unraveling the mystery of health: How people manage stress and stay well. 2nd Edn. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Google Scholar

Antonovsky, A. (1991). Health, stress, and coping. The Jossey-Bass social and behavioral science series. 1st Edn. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Google Scholar

Antonovsky, A. (1993). The structure and properties of the sense of coherence scale. Soc. Sci. Med. 36, 725–733. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(93)90033-Z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balasubramaniam, M., Telles, S., and Doraiswamy, P. M. (2012). Yoga on our minds: a systematic review of yoga for neuropsychiatric disorders. Front. Psych. 3:117. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bankar, M. A., Chaudhari, S. K., and Chaudhari, K. D. (2013). Impact of long term yoga practice on sleep quality and quality of life in the elderly. J. Ayurveda Integr. Med. 4, 28–32. doi: 10.4103/0975-9476.109548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartels, M., Cacioppo, J. T., van Beijsterveldt, T. C. E. M., and Boomsma, D. I. (2013). Exploring the association between well-being and psychopathology in adolescents. Behav. Genet. 43, 177–190. doi: 10.1007/s10519-013-9589-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bech, P., Olsen, L. R., Kjoller, M., and Rasmussen, N. K. (2003). Measuring well-being rather than the absence of distress symptoms: a comparison of the SF-36 mental health subscale and the WHO-five well-being scale. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 12, 85–91. doi: 10.1002/mpr.145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birdee, G. S., Legedza, A. T., Saper, R. B., Bertisch, S. M., Eisenberg, D. M., and Phillips, R. S. (2008). Characteristics of yoga users: results of a national survey. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 23, 1653–1658. doi: 10.1007/s11606-008-0735-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bland, J. M., and Altman, D. G. (1994). Matching. BMJ 309:1128. doi: 10.1136/bmj.309.6962.1128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brähler, E., Mühlan, H., Albani, C., and Schmidt, S. (2007). Teststatistische Prüfung und Normierung der deutschen Versionen des EUROHIS-QOL Lebensqualität-index und des WHO-5 Wohlbefindens-index [testing and standardization of the German version of the EUROHIS-QOL and WHO-5 quality-of life-indices]. Diagnostica 53, 83–96. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924.53.2.83

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Büssing, A., Hedtstück, A., Khalsa, S. B. S., Ostermann, T., and Heusser, P. (2012a). Development of specific aspects of spirituality during a 6-month intensive yoga practice. Evid. Based Complement. Alternat. Med. 2012:981523, 1–7. doi: 10.1155/2012/981523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Büssing, A., Michalsen, A., Khalsa, S. B. S., Telles, S., and Sherman, K. J. (2012b). Effects of yoga on mental and physical health: a short summary of reviews. Evid. Based Complement. Alternat. Med. 2012:165410. doi: 10.1155/2012/165410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carstens, J. A., and Spangenberg, J. J. (1997). Major depression: a breakdown in sense of coherence?. Psychol. Report. 80, 1211–1220. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1997.80.3c.1211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cramer, H., Lauche, R., Anheyer, D., Pilkington, K., Manincor, M. D., Dobos, G., et al. (2018). Yoga for anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Depress. Anxiety 35, 830–843. doi: 10.1002/da.22762

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cramer, H., Lauche, R., Langhorst, J., and Dobos, G. (2013). Yoga for depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Depress. Anxiety 30, 1068–1083. doi: 10.1002/da.22166

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cribari-Neto, F., Ferrari, S. L. P., and Oliveira, W. A. S. C. (2005). Numerical evaluation of tests based on different heteroskedasticity-consistent covariance matrix estimators. J. Stat. Comput. Simul. 75, 611–628. doi: 10.1080/00949650410001729427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csala, B., Springinsfeld, C. M., and Köteles, F. (2021). The relationship between yoga and spirituality: a systematic review of empirical research. Front. Psychol. 12:695939. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.695939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dadfar, M., Lester, D., Turan, Y., Beshai, J. A., and Unterrainer, H.-F. (2021). Religious spiritual well-being: results from Muslim Iranian clinical and non-clinical samples by age, sex and group. J. Relig. Spiritual. Aging 33, 16–37. doi: 10.1080/15528030.2020.1818161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, R., and MacKinnon, J. G. (1993). Estimation and inference in econometrics. New York: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Derogatis, L. R. (2000). BSI 18 brief symptom inventory 18: administration, scoring and procedures manual. London: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Derogatis, L. R., and Melisaratos, N. (1983). The brief symptom inventory: an introductory report. Psychol. Med. 13, 595–605. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700048017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desson, Z., Lambertz, L., Peters, J. W., Falkenbach, M., and Kauer, L. (2020). Europe’s Covid-19 outliers: German, Austrian and Swiss policy responses during the early stages of the 2020 pandemic. Health Policy Technol. 9, 405–418. doi: 10.1016/j.hlpt.2020.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domingues, R. B. (2018). Modern postural yoga as a mental health promoting tool: a systematic review. Complement. Ther. Clin. Pract. 31, 248–255. doi: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2018.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domingues, R. B., and Carmo, C. (2021). Orthorexia nervosa in yoga practitioners: relationship with personality, attitudes about appearance, and yoga engagement. Eat. Weight Disord. 26, 789–795. doi: 10.1007/s40519-020-00911-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksson, M., and Lindström, B. (2006). Antonovsky’s sense of coherence scale and the relation with health: a systematic review. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 60, 376–381. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.041616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksson, M., and Lindström, B. (2007). Antonovsky’s sense of coherence scale and its relation with quality of life: a systematic review. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 61, 938–944. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.056028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldt, T., Leskinen, E., Kinnunen, U., and Ruoppila, I. (2003). The stability of sense of coherence: comparing two age groups in a 5-year follow-up study. Personal. Individ. Differ. 35, 1151–1165. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00325-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forfylow (2011). Integrating yoga with psychotherapy: a complementary treatment for anxiety and depression. Can. J. Couns. Psychother. 45:132.

Google Scholar

Fouladbakhsh, J. M., and Stommel, M. (2010). Gender, symptom experience, and use of complementary and alternative medicine practices among cancer survivors in the U.S. Cancer population. Oncol. Nurs. Forum 37, E7–E15. doi: 10.1188/10.ONF.E7-E15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franke, G. H., Jaeger, S., Glaesmer, H., Barkmann, C., Petrowski, K., and Braehler, E. (2017). Psychometric analysis of the brief symptom inventory 18 (BSI-18) in a representative German sample. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 17:14. doi: 10.1186/s12874-016-0283-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritz, M. S., and MacKinnon, D. (2007). Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychol. Sci. 18, 233–239. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01882.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuchshuber, J., and Unterrainer, H. F. (2021). “test your spirituality in one minute or less” structural validity of the multidimensional inventory for religious/spiritual well-being short version (MI-RSWB 12). Front. Psychol. 12:597565. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.597565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaiswinkler, L., Kaufmann, P., Pollheimer, E., Ackermann, A., Holasek, S., Kapfhammer, H. P., et al. (2020). Mindfulness and self-compassion in clinical psychiatric rehabilitation: a clinical trial. Mindfulness 11, 374–383. doi: 10.1007/s12671-019-01171-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaiswinkler, L., and Unterrainer, H.-F. (2016). The relationship between yoga involvement, mindfulness and psychological well-being. Complement. Ther. Med. 26, 123–127. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2016.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaiswinkler, L., Unterrainer, H.-F., Fink, A., and Kapfhammer, H.-P. (2015). Die Beziehung zwischen yoga immersion, psychologischem Wohlbefinden und psychiatrischer Symptomatik [the relationship between yoga immersion, psychological well-being and psychiatric symptoms]. Neuropsychiatr 29, 29–35. doi: 10.1007/s40211-015-0139-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gwiaździński, P., Fedyk, O., Krawczyk, M., and Szymański, M. (2017). Practicing hatha-yoga, sense of coherence and sense of agency. Neurophenomenological approach. Psychiatr. Danub. 29, 530–535.

Google Scholar

Haukoos, J. S., and Lewis, R. J. (2005). Advanced statistics: bootstrapping confidence intervals for statistics with “difficult” distributions. Acad. Emerg. Med. 12, 360–365. doi: 10.1197/j.aem.2004.11.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2022). Methodology in the social sciences. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional Process analysis: A regression-based approach. 3rd Edn. New York: The Guilford Press Available at: https://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/kxp/detail.action?docID=6809031.

Google Scholar

Hendriks, T., Jong, J. D., and Cramer, H. (2017). The effects of yoga on positive mental health among healthy adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 23, 505–517. doi: 10.1089/acm.2016.0334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holman, E. A., Jones, N. M., Garfin, D. R., and Silver, R. C. (2022). Distortions in time perception during collective trauma: Insights from a national longitudinal study during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy. doi: 10.1037/tra0001326. [E-pub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, S. (2004). “Zentralität und multidimensionale Struktur der Religiosität: Eine Synthese der theoretischen Ansätze von Allport und Glock zur Messung der Religiosität [centrality and multidimensional structure of religiosity: a synthesis of Allport and Glock’s theoretical approaches to measuring religiosity.]” in Religiosität: Messverfahren und Studien zu Gesundheit und Lebensbewältigung. eds. C. Zwingmann and H. Moosbrugger (Münster: Waxmann), 79–105.

Google Scholar

IPCC (Ed.) (2022). “Climate change 2022: impacts, adaptation and vulnerability” in Working group II contribution to the IPCC sixth assessment report (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press) Available at: https://www.ipcc.ch/report/ar6/wg2/downloads/report/IPCC_AR6_WGII_FullReport.pdf

Google Scholar

Jeserich, F. (2013). Can sense of coherence be modified by religious/spiritual interventions? A critical appraisal of previous research. Interdiscip. J. Res. Religion 9:Article 5 Available at: https://www.religjournal.com/pdf/ijrr09005.pdf

Google Scholar

Jeter, P. E., Slutsky, J., Singh, N., and Khalsa, S. B. S. (2015). Yoga as a therapeutic intervention: a bibliometric analysis of published research studies from 1967 to 2013. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 21, 586–592. doi: 10.1089/acm.2015.0057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joshi, N., and Singh, V. (2020). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs in yoga studies: review and suggestions for prospective yoga research scholars. Int. J. Yoga Allied Sci. 9, 152–155.

Google Scholar

Kelly, G. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs 1st ed New York: W. W. Norton: A Theory of Personality.

Google Scholar

Kenny, D. A., and Judd, C. M. (2014). Power anomalies in testing mediation. Psychol. Sci. 25, 334–339. doi: 10.1177/0956797613502676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsson, G., and Kallenberg, K. O. (1996). Sense of coherence, socioeconomic conditions and health: interrelationships in a nation-wide Swedish sample. Eur. J. Pub. Health 6, 175–180. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/6.3.175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, A. W., and Goldsmith, C. A. (2012). The effects of yoga on anxiety and stress. Altern. Med. Rev.: J. Clin. Ther. 17 Available at: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22502620/, 21–35.

Google Scholar

López Ulloa, B. F., Møller, V., and Sousa-Poza, A. (2013). How does subjective well-being evolve with age? A literature review. J. Popul. Ageing 6, 227–246. doi: 10.1007/s12062-013-9085-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., and Lockwood, C. M. (2000). Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prev. Sci. 1, 173–181. doi: 10.1023/A:1026595011371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michelis, E. D. (2005). A history of modern yoga: Patañjali and western esotericism. London, New York: Continuum.

Google Scholar

Moleiro, C., Pinto, N., and Freire, J. (2013). Effects of age on spiritual well-being and Homonegativity: religious identity and practices among LGB persons in Portugal. J. Relig. Spiritual. Aging 25, 93–111. doi: 10.1080/15528030.2012.741561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molteni, F., and Biolcati, F. (2018). Shifts in religiosity across cohorts in Europe: a multilevel and multidimensional analysis based on the European values study. Social Compass 65, 413–432. doi: 10.1177/0037768618772969

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen-Feng, V. N., Clark, C. J., and Butler, M. E. (2019). Yoga as an intervention for psychological symptoms following trauma: a systematic review and quantitative synthesis. Psychol. Serv. 16, 513–523. doi: 10.1037/ser0000191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilsson, K. W., Leppert, J., Simonsson, B., and Starrin, B. (2010). Sense of coherence and psychological well-being: improvement with age. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 64, 347–352. doi: 10.1136/jech.2008.081174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

OECD. (2021). COVID-19 and well-being: Life in the pandemic. OECD Publishing; Paris.

Google Scholar

Orlygsdottir, B., Gunnarsdottir, T. J., Thoroddsen, A., and Vilhjalmsson, R. (2021). The prevalence and characteristics of yoga or meditation users in Iceland: results of a national health survey. Complement. Ther. Clin. Pract. 45:101487. doi: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2021.101487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, C. L., Finkelstein-Fox, L., Groessl, E. J., Elwy, A. R., and Lee, S. Y. (2020). Exploring how different types of yoga change psychological resources and emotional well-being across a single session. Complement. Ther. Med. 49:102354. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2020.102354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, C. L., Groessl, E., Maiya, M., Sarkin, A., Eisen, S. V., Riley, K., et al. (2014). Comparison groups in yoga research: a systematic review and critical evaluation of the literature. Complement. Ther. Med. 22, 920–929. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2014.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascoe, M. C., Thompson, D. R., and Ski, C. F. (2017). Yoga, mindfulness-based stress reduction and stress-related physiological measures: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 86, 152–168. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pek, J., Wong, O., and Wong, A. C. M. (2018). How to address non-normality: a taxonomy of approaches, reviewed, and illustrated. Front. Psychol. 9:2104. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pietrabissa, G., and Simpson, S. G. (2020). Psychological consequences of social isolation during COVID-19 outbreak. Front. Psychol. 11:2201. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ristkari, T., Sourander, A., Rønning, J. A., Nikolakaros, G., and Helenius, H. (2008). Life events, self-reported psychopathology and sense of coherence among young men–a population-based study. Nord. J. Psychiatry 62, 464–471. doi: 10.1080/08039480801984313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Röhr, S., Müller, F., Jung, F., Apfelbacher, C., Seidler, A., and Riedel-Heller, S. G. (2020). Psychosoziale Folgen von Quarantänemaßnahmen bei schwerwiegenden Coronavirus-Ausbrüchen: ein Rapid Review [Psychosocial Impact of Quarantine Measures During Serious Coronavirus Outbreaks: A Rapid Review]. Psychiatr. Prax. 47, 179–189. doi: 10.1055/a-1159-5562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenbaum, P. R. (1984). The Consquences of adjustment for a concomitant variable that has been affected by the treatment. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. (General) 147:656. doi: 10.2307/2981697

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahni, P. S., Singh, K., Sharma, N., and Garg, R. (2021). Yoga an effective strategy for self-management of stress-related problems and wellbeing during COVID19 lockdown: a cross-sectional study. PLoS One 16:e0245214. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schumacher, J., Wilz, G., Gunzelmann, T., and Brähler, E. (2000). Die sense of coherence scale von Antonovsky: Teststatistische Überprüfung in einer repräsentativen Bevölkerungsstichprobe und Konstruktion einer Kurzskala [the Antonovsky sense of coherence scale. Test statistical evaluation of a representative population sample and construction of a brief scale]. Psychother. Psychosom. Med. Psychol. 50, 472–482. doi: 10.1055/s-2000-9207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, N. M., Hofmann, S. G., Rosenfield, D., Hoeppner, S. S., Hoge, E. A., Bui, E., et al. (2021). Efficacy of yoga vs cognitive behavioral therapy vs stress education for the treatment of generalized anxiety disorder: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiat. 78, 13–20. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.2496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, S., and Brähler, E. (2007). Die »Sense of Coherence Scale«: Testhandbuch zur deutschen Version (1. Aufl.). (Göttingen: Vandenhoeck Ruprecht). Available at: http://gbv.eblib.com/patron/FullRecord.aspx?p=1719327

Google Scholar

Spitzer, C., Hammer, S., Löwe, B., Grabe, H. J., Barnow, S., Rose, M., et al. (2011). Die Kurzform des brief symptom inventory (BSI-18): Erste Befunde zu den psychometrischen Kennwerten der deutschen version [the short version of the brief symptom inventory (BSI-18): preliminary psychometric properties of the German translation]. Fortschr. Neurol. Psychiatr. 79, 517–523. doi: 10.1055/s-0031-1281602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Statz, T. L., Kobayashi, L. C., and Finlay, J. M. (2022). ‘Losing the illusion of control and predictability of life’: experiences of grief and loss among ageing US adults during the COVID-19 pandemic. Ageing Soc., 1–24. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X21001872

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taimni, I. K. (2005). The science of yoga: The yoga-sutras of Patanjali. Adyar, Chennai, India • Wheaton, IL, USA: Theosophical Pub. House.

Google Scholar

Tibbitts, D. C., Aicher, S. A., Sugg, J., Handloser, K., Eisman, L., Booth, L. D., et al. (2021). Program evaluation of trauma-informed yoga for vulnerable populations. Eval. Program Plann. 88:101946. doi: 10.1016/j.evalprogplan.2021.101946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Traunmüller, C., Stefitz, R., Gaisbachgrabner, K., and Schwerdtfeger, A. (2020). Psychological correlates of COVID-19 pandemic in the Austrian population. BMC Public Health 20:1395. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-09489-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trulson, H. F., and Vernon, L. (2019). Yoga as an adjunct therapy through the lens of positive psychology theory and neuroscience. FAU Undergrad. Res. J. 8:64. Available at: https://journals.flvc.org/faurj/article/download/114576/109909

Google Scholar

Unterrainer, H.-F., Huber, H. P., Sorgo, I. M., Collicutt, J., and Fink, A. (2011). Dimensions of religious/spiritual well-being and schizotypal personality. Personal. Individ. Differ. 51, 360–364. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unterrainer, H.-F., and Kapfhammer, H.-P. (2014). Religiös/Spirituelles Befinden bei psychisch Kranken II: die Entwicklung einer Kurzskala und Vergleichswerte von klinisch-psychiatrischen Gruppen und gesunden Kontrollpersonen [religious/spiritual well-being in mentally ill persons II: the development of a short scale and comparison scores for clinical psychiatric groups and healthy controls]. Neuropsychiatr 28, 49–55. doi: 10.1007/s40211-013-0083-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unterrainer, H. F., Ladenhauf, K. H., Moazedi, M. L., Wallner-Liebmann, S. J., and Fink, A. (2010). Dimensions of religious/spiritual well-being and their relation to personality and psychological well-being. Pers. Individ. Differ. 49, 192–197. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unterrainer, H.-F., Lewis, A. J., and Fink, A. (2014). Religious/spiritual well-being, personality and mental health: a review of results and conceptual issues. J. Relig. Health 53, 382–392. doi: 10.1007/s10943-012-9642-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Stede, W. A. (2014). A manipulationist view of causality in cross-sectional survey research. Acc. Organ. Soc. 39, 567–574. doi: 10.1016/j.aos.2013.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viertiö, S., Kiviruusu, O., Piirtola, M., Kaprio, J., Korhonen, T., Marttunen, M., et al. (2021). Factors contributing to psychological distress in the working population, with a special reference to gender difference. BMC Public Health 21:611. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-10560-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volanen, S.-M., Lahelma, E., Silventoinen, K., and Suominen, S. (2004). Factors contributing to sense of coherence among men and women. Eur. J. Pub. Health 14, 322–330. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/14.3.322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuzic, X. D., Burgkart, P. L., Wenzl, M., Fuchshuber, J., and Unterrainer, H.-F. (2022). The relationship between religious/spiritual well-being and mental health among young adults during the COVID-19-pandemic. Front. Psychol. 13:942149. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.942149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wadhen, V., and Cartwright, T. (2021). Feasibility and outcome of an online streamed yoga intervention on stress and wellbeing of people working from home during COVID-19. Work (Reading, Mass.) 69, 331–349. doi: 10.3233/WOR-205325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waelde, L. C., Thompson, L., and Gallagher‐Thompson, D. (2004). A pilot study of a yoga and meditation intervention for dementia caregiver stress. J. Clin. Psychol. 60, 677–687. doi: 10.1002/jclp.10259

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, W.-L., Chen, K.-H., Pan, Y.-C., Yang, S.-N., and Chan, Y.-Y. (2020). The effect of yoga on sleep quality and insomnia in women with sleep problems: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry 20:195. doi: 10.1186/s12888-020-02566-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodyard, C. (2011). Exploring the therapeutic effects of yoga and its ability to increase quality of life. Int. J. Yoga 4, 49–54. doi: 10.4103/0973-6131.85485

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yashika, V., and Vasanthi, G. (2019). Effect of yoga and pilates on selected psychological variables on irregular menstrual cycle among college women. Int. J. Physiol. Nutr. Phys. Educ. 4, 853–855. Available at: https://www.journalofsports.com/pdf/2019/vol4issue1/parts/4-1-101-489.pdf

Google Scholar

Zacher, H., and Rudolph, C. W. (2021). Individual differences and changes in subjective wellbeing during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. Am. Psychol. 76, 50–62. doi: 10.1037/amp0000702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhai, Y., and Du, X. (2020). Loss and grief amidst COVID-19: a path to adaptation and resilience. Brain Behav. Immun. 87, 80–81. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.04.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Yoga, Yoga involvement, subjective well-being, psychological distress, sense of coherence, spirituality, COVID-19, crisis

Citation: Bös C, Gaiswinkler L, Fuchshuber J, Schwerdtfeger A and Unterrainer H-F (2023) Effect of Yoga involvement on mental health in times of crisis: A cross-sectional study. Front. Psychol. 14:1096848. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1096848

Received: 12 November 2022; Accepted: 28 February 2023;
Published: 22 March 2023.

Edited by:

Jie Li, Xi’an Jiaotong-Liverpool University, China

Reviewed by:

Nor Ba’yah Abdul Kadir, National University of Malaysia, Malaysia
Surabhi Gautam, School of Medicine, Emory University, United States

Copyright © 2023 Bös, Gaiswinkler, Fuchshuber, Schwerdtfeger and Unterrainer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jürgen Fuchshuber, a00913689@unet.univie.ac.at; Human-Friedrich Unterrainer, human.unterrainer@univie.ac.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.