Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 27 September 2023
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Towards an Understanding of the Cognitive Mechanisms Involved in Threat Processing and Perception View all 10 articles

The bigger the threat, the longer the gaze? A cross-cultural study of Somalis and Czechs

Iveta &#x;tolhoferov&#x;Iveta Štolhoferová1Daniel Frynta&#x;Daniel Frynta1Markta JanovcovMarkéta Janovcová1Veronika RudolfovVeronika Rudolfová1Hassan Sh Abdirahman Elmi,Hassan Sh Abdirahman Elmi1,2Kate&#x;ina RexovKateřina Rexová1Daniel Alex BertiDaniel Alex Berti1David KrlDavid Král1David SommerDavid Sommer1Eva LandovEva Landová1Petra Frýdlov
Petra Frýdlová1*
  • 1Department of Zoology, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Prague, Czechia
  • 2Department of Biology, Faculty of Education, Amoud University, Borama, Somalia

High fear reaction, preferential attention, or fast detection are only a few of the specific responses which snakes evoke in humans. Previous research has shown that these responses are shared amongst several distinct cultures suggesting the evolutionary origin of the response. However, populations from sub-Saharan Africa have been largely missing in experimental research focused on this issue. In this paper, we focus on the effect of snake threat display on human spontaneous attention. We performed an eye-tracking experiment with participants from Somaliland and the Czechia and investigated whether human attention is swayed towards snakes in a threatening posture. Seventy-one Somalis and 71 Czechs were tested; the samples were matched for gender and comparable in age structure and education level. We also investigated the effect of snake morphotype as snakes differ in their threat display. We found that snakes in a threatening posture were indeed gazed upon more than snakes in a relaxed (non-threatening) posture. Further, we found a large effect of snake morphotype as this was especially prominent in cobras, less in vipers, and mostly non-significant in other morphotypes. Finally, despite highly different cultural and environmental backgrounds, the overall pattern of reaction towards snakes was similar in Somalis and Czechs supporting the evolutionary origin of the phenomenon. We concluded that human attention is preferentially directed towards snakes, especially cobras and vipers, in threatening postures.

1. Introduction

Snakes seem to evoke a number of specific responses in humans, including a high fear reaction, preferential attention, and fast detection (Öhman and Mineka, 2003; Okon-Singer et al., 2011; Soares et al., 2014; Kawai and Qiu, 2020; Landová et al., 2020; Jensen and Caine, 2021). Each of these phenomena has been previously studied in detail and is described in its own terms and hypotheses. Taken altogether, however, the human mind seems to be specifically equipped to react to snakes in a certain manner (Isbell, 2006, 2009). Further, there are several compelling pieces of evidence that this reaction is at least partially innate (Tierney and Connolly, 2013; Kawai, 2019). First, it is shared amongst distinct cultures across the globe (Alves et al., 2014; Pandey et al., 2016; Landová et al., 2018; Onyishi et al., 2021), second, it can manifest itself very early in human ontogeny (Lobue and DeLoache, 2008; DeLoache and LoBue, 2009; Hayakawa et al., 2011; Borgi and Cirulli, 2015; Bertels et al., 2020), and third, we can observe a similar reaction in apes and other primates (Murray and King, 1973; Shibasaki and Kawai, 2009; Weiss et al., 2015; Kawai and Koda, 2016; Wombolt and Caine, 2016; Zhang et al., 2020). This body of evidence serves as the basis of what is now known as the Snake detection theory (Isbell, 2006 and references therein).

In the last two decades, the research of visual attention mainly focused on whether humans are able to detect a snake faster or more accurately than different types of stimuli in a challenging setup. In a visual detection task, snakes were detected faster than other animals (Lobue and DeLoache, 2008; Shibasaki and Kawai, 2011; Penkunas and Coss, 2013; Soares and Esteves, 2013), even under high perceptual load (Soares et al., 2014; Kawai and He, 2016; Kawai and Qiu, 2020) and regardless of their colouration (Prokop et al., 2018; Fančovičová et al., 2020). Similar results were obtained from a simulated virtual hike (Jensen and Caine, 2021), whilst LoBue (2014) and Kawai (2019) investigated which features facilitated the detection. Additional support for the Snake detection theory also comes from event-related potentials (ERP) studies (He et al., 2014; Grassini et al., 2016; Van Strien et al., 2016; Van Strien and Isbell, 2017) or neurobiological research (Van Le et al., 2013, 2014). Nonetheless, the issue seems more complex as, for example, guns (i.e., evolutionarily irrelevant inanimate objects) are detected as fast or even faster than snakes (Fox et al., 2007; Zsido et al., 2019a,b). Taken all together, there is a strong experimental support for primate brains being fine-tuned for snake detection, however, it seems that not all snakes are prioritised (see, e.g., Rádlová et al., 2019) and not under all circumstances (Subra et al., 2018; reviewed in Coelho et al., 2019).

Importantly, all these experiments assume – although sometimes inexplicitly – that humans and other primates pick on certain visual cues for snake detection provided by the snake’s appearance. It is worth pointing out that the snake does not provide these cues with the purpose of being detected; the primates rather take advantage of the cues the snake cannot conceal. In these scenarios, snakes are thought of as predators (Seligman, 1971; Öhman and Mineka, 2001; Isbell, 2006) and as such, they would not profit from being discovered. However, adult primates are rarely snake prey probably thanks to high vigilance, warning calls, and aggressive group defence (Seyfarth et al., 1980; Perry et al., 2003; Eberle and Kappeler, 2008; Etting et al., 2014; Teixeira et al., 2016). In fact, the roles might even reverse, and a snake might end up the prey itself (Headland and Greene, 2011; Falótico et al., 2018). What may have started as a clear predator–prey dynamics in evolutionary history, could now be seen rather as an equal-opponents situation.

Under these circumstances, it might be advantageous to signal one’s readiness to fight towards the opponent. This type of signalling is called a threat display. It is almost omnipresent in animals although it may take different forms in different species (e.g., chimpanzees – Nishida et al., 1999; frillneck lizards – Shine, 1990; cuttlefish – Langridge et al., 2007; pelicans – Gokula, 2011; tarantulas – Bennie et al., 2011). In snakes, the most famous example is the threatening posture of cobras – the animal puffs, its body front rises, and its neck-flack spreads (Greene, 1988). Another example comes from vipers – the animal puffs and coils its body in very tight loops with an elevated head held slightly back as if ready to strike (Greene, 1988). Both postures are quite conspicuous, and the animal often accompanies its display with hissing, which further facilitates its detection. The display is clearly intended to be seen by the opponent.

In this paper, we follow the line of thought previously introduced by Isbell’s Snake detection theory. We aim to explore whether the human mind is also fine-tuned for a snake’s intentional threat signalling rather than just unintentional cues of its presence. To this end, we employ an eye-tracking method utilising a simple design of spontaneous gaze preference when presented with two stimuli at once. We hypothesise that snakes in threatening postures will attract more attention than snakes in relaxed, non-threatening postures. In the past, it was demonstrated that emotions can guide visual attention (Vuilleumier, 2005), and, in particular, that fear-relevant animals are fixated faster, more often, or for longer time periods than fear-irrelevant animal targets (Öhman et al., 2001; Gerdes et al., 2009; LoBue, 2014). Moreover, the importance of snake posture for assessment of danger was previously shown in macaques (Etting and Isbell, 2014; Van Le et al., 2014), and humans (Masataka et al., 2010; Lobue and DeLoache, 2011).

To highlight the ecological aspect of our hypothesis, we focused on Somalis (specifically the population living in Somaliland). Whilst the culture is traditionally pastoral and therefore mobile, according to genetic and linguistic evidence they belong to the core populations of North and Northeast Africa, which have never left the African continent or the savanna environment. Somalis are thus characterised by the near-continuous presence in both the geographic region and the environment of human origin (Stringer, 2016; Gibbons, 2017). Moreover, evidence suggests that the snake species composition of the Horn of Africa has remained largely unchanged during the principal part of human evolution (Kelly et al., 2009; Barlow et al., 2019; Šmíd and Tolley, 2019; Zaher et al., 2019), and we previously found that Somalis consider snakes the most fear-eliciting animals amongst a wide variety of species (Frynta et al., 2023). This makes Somalis uniquely suited for research focusing on the possible co-evolution of snake signalling and human signal detection. In addition, we included participants from Czechia whose ancestors left Africa and, similarly to other Europeans, reached Europe about 30,000 years ago (Prüfer et al., 2021). As there have been virtually no dangerous snakes in Central Europe over the last 40,000 years (only mildly dangerous adder Vipera berus), Czechs seem a suitable match to Somalis for cross-cultural comparison. Similar responses across the participants, despite thousands of years of differential exposure to snakes, would suggest that the reaction is at least partially innate and a result of long coevolution between humans and snakes. Contrary, if cultural or more recent selection pressures are involved, clear differences between Somali and Czech participants should emerge. We know of no psychological study focusing on snakes and simultaneously utilising an eye-tracking experimental design in Sub-Saharan Africa.

To summarise, the aims of this study are as follows: (1) To test whether a snake in a threatening posture attracts more attention than one in a relaxed, non-threatening posture, (2) To investigate whether such phenomenon is universal or whether it is specific for certain snake morphotypes as those differ in their threatening postures, and (3) To compare the attention paid to snakes by Somalis and Czechs.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Selection and preparation of the stimuli

The experimental stimuli were photos of 20 snake species. The selected snakes could be divided into three morphotype groups: vipers (eight species), cobras (eight species), and others (two pythons and two colubrid species). Vipers and cobras, all venomous species, were chosen because of their presumptive relevance for human evolution. During the selection process, the threatening posture of each candidate stimulus species was considered as it had to be visually distinctive enough from the relaxed body posture. This was an especially important criterion for the selection of non-venomous species which tend to have less conspicuous threat display. Lastly, we chose species distributed in Africa or the Middle East (except for two Asian species), the key regions of human evolution. Amongst the included snakes were also some of the most venomous species of the African Horn region: two vipers (puff adder Bitis arietans and North-East African Carpet Viper Echis pyramidum), two elapids (black mamba Dendroaspis polylepis and Egyptian cobra Naja haje), and one colubrid (boomslang Dispholidus typus). The Somaliland local fauna is additionally represented by two non-venomous species in our set (a colubrid species Telescopus dhara and a rock python Python sebae). Most stimuli photos came from authors’ personal archives but 15 were sourced from the internet. For a complete list of experimental stimuli and their sources, see Supplementary Table S1.

Each experimental image (slide) consisted of a photo of a snake in a threatening posture and a photo of the same specimen in a relaxed (non-threatening) posture. The original photos’ backgrounds were cut-off, and the snakes were placed on a shared 20% grey background, each on one side of the image. They were adjusted to be similar in size, hue, and brightness, and positioned so they both were looking towards the image centre. When available, pictures of the same snake individual were used. Twenty images with the threatening posture on the left were supplemented with their horizontally flipped versions (i.e., the threatening posture on the right) accounting for a total of 40 experimental images. In addition, one practise image preceded the experimental ones in the task. The practise image consisted of a drawing of a squirrel on the left and a hyena on the right. For examples of experimental images, see Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Example of experimental slides, the snake in threatening posture is always on the right. (A) Cobra morphotype, (B–C) viper morphotype, (D) other morphotype.

2.2. The experimental procedure

Before the task itself, respondents were informed about the basic design of the experiment, and they gave written consent with their participation together with some personal information (name, gender, age, nationality). Next, participants were seated in front of the laptop (about 60 cm head to screen distance) with 1,366 × 768 pixels resolution, and they were asked to sit still but naturally and to look at the screen. This was followed by individual calibration of the myGaze eye tracking device. Immediately after a successful calibration, the experimental slide presentation followed. With the first slide (the practise image), they were instructed: “You will see two snakes at each slide, you may look at them as you wish, there is no particular task.”

The presentation consisted of 41 images (one practise image and forty experimental images), each displayed for 5 s. Between the images, there were slides with a black fixation cross on the same 20% grey background displayed for 2 s. The images were displayed in one of four pre-defined orders. Each order was a semi-random sequence of images where 10 stimuli (4 cobras, 4 vipers, 1 python, and 1 colubrid) were presented (both left and right versions) before the remaining 10 stimuli. The first and the second order were the same sequence of images, only displayed in reverse (the first image of order one was the last image of order two). The third and the fourth order were the same as order one and two, respectively, only the images were mirrored (left-versioned images were exchanged for right-versioned images and vice versa). Each respondent was assigned an order at random.

2.3. Participants

A total of 71 Somali and 71 Czech respondents participated in the experiment. In both samples, there were 25 women and 46 men. The mean age was 22.37 years (range 19–39) in the Somali sample and 24.63 years (range 18–44) in the Czech sample, the mean age did not significantly differ between the samples (non-parametric Man-Whitney test: Z-value = −1.26, value of p = 0.209). Most of the participants in both samples were undergraduate students of various fields. When asked by the investigator, no respondents expressed any extreme attitude towards snakes – neither positive (e.g., great fondness) nor negative (e.g., strong fear). The sample size was based on a similarly designed study (Rudolfová et al., 2022) where 136 participants in total were recommended by a priori power analysis using G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007). We chose a medium effect size (f = 0.15), adjusted the α error probability for multiple comparisons amongst categories (p = 0.0167), and corrected for a correlation amongst repeated measures (r = 0.25).

2.4. Data extraction and curation

MyGaze eye tracking device measures the position of the participants’ gaze and records approximately 30 samples per second. We developed our own processing software that converts the data into more intuitive variables which were defined as follows. “Number of sample measurements” is the total number of samples measured during the trial (i.e., approx. 150 in our case). A fixation was defined as all sample measurements that are no farther away than 23 pixels (0.5° visual angle) from a lead (reference) sample measurement. The lead sample measurement was defined as the first recorded measurement during each trial, and then each first measurement in the timeline that did not fall inside of the previous fixation (i.e., the next first measurement that was further them 23 pixels from the previous reference measurement). Moreover, each fixation had to consist of at least two consecutive sample measurements. Following these definitions, we computed the “Number of fixations.” Finally, “Fixation time” was defined as the total duration of the participant’s gaze. For the purpose of further analyses, we used only mean binocular metrics. Further, we custom-defined three interest areas (IAs) — the left side of the screen, the right side of the screen, and the central part (fixation cross) — and exported all variables separately for each IA. No IA overlapped any other. Left and right IAs covered the snake photos and their vicinity and were the same in size (each covering 37% of the screen). Central IA was very small in comparison (1% of the screen) and covered the area where the fixation cross would have been. Empty image parts too far from snake photos were not included in any of the IAs. To improve accuracy and precision, we used only averaged data from both eyes (Cui and Hondzinski, 2006).

For the subsequent statistical analysis, no participant was fully excluded, however, we eliminated defected measurements (trials) where the gaze was not tracked properly, or the participant was temporarily distracted and did not look at the screen. As a criterion, we chose to exclude observations with combined dwell time on the left and right AOIs under 2000 ms. Based on this criterion, 417 observations were excluded (7.34%). The final dataset, therefore, contained a total of 5,263 observations, 2,558 from Somali respondents and 2,705 from Czech respondents. To compensate for possible side preference, we averaged the data obtained from the horizontally flipped image pairs. In cases where only one slide of the pair was available (because the second one was excluded in the previous step), we used this data but assigned them observation weight “1.” Averaged data were assigned observation weight “2.” The original data associated with this manuscript are available in Supplementary Table S3.

2.5. Statistics

For the statistical analysis, we used linear mixed-effects models (LMM) as implemented in software R (R Development Core Team, 2022), packages nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2022), and emmeans (Lenth, 2022). Our prime focus was on the number of fixations supplemented by the analysis of dwell times (Orquin and Holmqvist, 2018). For investigation of the effect of threatening posture, we subtracted fixations on the snake in relaxed posture from fixations on the snake in threatening posture (always within the experimental slide) and thus prepared two new response variables – the difference in the number of fixations and the difference in the dwell time. We chose to investigate the difference rather than the absolute values on each posture because (1) it better reflected the pair manner of stimuli presentation and (2) represented specifically the effect of snake body position on participants’ spontaneous attention towards the stimuli. In full models, respondents’ gender, nationality, and age, and further group, gender-nationality interaction, and group-nationality interaction were used as fixed effects, whilst respondents’ ID was used as a random effect. To account for heteroscedasticity, we defined a custom variance structure combining the constant variance structure for respondents’ nationality and the fixed variance structure for observation weight. Fixed effects that did not prove significant (α = 0.05) were successively reduced. The reduced models and their respective full models were compared with the likelihood-ratio test and on the basis of the Akaike information criterion (AIC). The first method supported the same goodness of fit of both models (the full one and the reduced one), whilst the AIC suggested the reduced models were better because they were simpler (i.e., the full models were overfitted in comparison). Factor coefficients were computed using the restricted maximum likelihood method, for the purpose of full and reduced model comparison, we applied the maximum likelihood method.

2.6. Ethical note

All procedures performed in this study were carried out in accordance with the ethical standards of the appropriate institutional research committee (The Institutional Review Board of Charles University, Faculty of Science, approval no. 2019/2011, granted on 27 March 2019; and The Institutional Review Board of Amoud University, Borama, approval no. AU/AA/0012/2021, granted on 7 January 2021).

3. Results

In the linear mixed effect model for the difference in the number of fixations on threatening versus relaxed posture, only one factor proved significant: snake morphotype (F(2,2,593) = 24.42, p < 0.001). Other factors were successively taken out of the model since their effect did not prove significant (nationality: F(1,138) = 0.05, p = 0.830; gender: F(1,138) = 1.54; p = 0.217; age: F(1,138) = 0.20, p = 0.652; snake morphotype – nationality interaction: F(2,2,591) = 2.26, p = 0.078; nationality – gender interaction: F(1,138) = 0.05, p = 0.831). The odds ratio (assessed with likelihood-ratio test) between the full and reduced model was 7.00, p = 0.321. For the estimated differences, the following logic can be applied. When the difference is positive, participants looked more at the threatening posture, when negative, participants looked more at the relaxed posture, zero difference signifies equal attention on both. To this end, we tested each difference against zero. Cobras in threatening posture captured 1.85 more fixations than cobras in relaxed posture (95% confidence interval 1.45–2.25), this difference was significantly different from zero (t(2593) = 9.12, p < 0.001). Similarly, vipers in threatening posture captured 0.46 more fixations than vipers in relaxed posture (95% confidence interval 0.06–0.85). Whilst the difference was smaller than the one for cobras, it was also significantly different from zero (t(2593) = 2.28, p = 0.023). Lastly, other snake morphotypes captured 0.28 more fixations in threatening posture than in relaxed posture (95% confidence interval − 0.23 – 0.79), however, this difference was not significantly different from zero (t(2593) = 1.09, p = 0.276). For context, the average number of fixations per trial across all stimuli and participants was 18.07 for the left and right IAs combined. The results are shown in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean difference of the number of fixations on snakes in the threatening versus relaxed posture for three snake morphotype groups. When the difference is positive, participants looked more at the threatening posture, when negative, participants looked more at the relaxed posture, zero difference signifies exactly equal attention on both. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals, means are tested against zero with significances indicated by asterisks (ns – p ≥ 0.05; * – p < 0.05; *** – p < 0.001).

There was no difference in the goodness of fit of the full and reduced model for the difference in dwell time (p = 0.300), hence we again chose the reduced model as the final model based on the AIC. The odds ratio between the models was 3.66. The final model for the difference of dwell time on threatening versus relaxed posture contained snake morphotype (F(2,2,591) = 12.57, p < 0.001), nationality (F(1,141) = 0.94, p = 0.333), and their interaction (F(2,2,591) = 4.33, p = 0.013). Other factors were successively taken out of the model (gender: F(1,138) = 3.15; p = 0.078; age: F(1,138) = 0.50, p = 0.504; nationality – gender interaction: F(1,138) < 0.01, p = 0.975). The results showed that both Somalis and Czechs gazed at cobras in a threatening posture longer than at cobras in a relaxed posture. In viper and other snake morphotype stimuli, only Czechs dwelled on the snakes in threatening posture longer; Somalis divided their attention equally. For more details, see Table 1 and Figure 3. The average dwell time per trial across all stimuli and participants was 4,368 ms for the left and right IAs combined. For average dwell times per trial of each stimulus species, see Supplementary Table S2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Results of the model for the difference of dwell time on threatening versus relaxed posture.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean difference of the dwell time on snakes in the threatening versus relaxed posture – the effect of snake morphotype and participants’ nationality. Positive values signify longer dwell time on the threatening posture, negative values on the relaxed posture, and zero signifies exactly equal attention on both. All means are tested against zero (right) and means of the same morphotype are compared between Somalis and Czechs (left). Error Bars are 95% confidence intervals, significances are indicated by asterisks (ns – p ≥ 0.05; * – p < 0.05; ** – p < 0.01; *** – p < 0.001).

4. Discussion

To summarise the results, we found that snakes in a threatening posture attracted more attention than those in a relaxed, non-threatening posture which was manifested both in the number of fixations and the total dwell time. This was especially prominent in cobras, however, vipers in a threatening posture were also fixated more often than vipers in a non-threatening posture. Additionally, differences in dwell time revealed that Czechs dwelled on snakes in threatening posture longer no matter the snake morphotype. Contrarily, in Somalis, this held true only for the cobra morphotype; in the other two investigated morphotype groups (vipers and others) there was no difference in the attention paid to the snakes in threatening and non-threatening postures. Nonetheless, the only significant difference between Somalis and Czechs was in their reaction to vipers and specifically only in the dwell time, not the fixation count. Hence, we consider the overall pattern of reaction towards snake threatening postures relatively consistent across nationalities. We found no effect of gender.

In the last couple of years, researchers paid special attention to what features are crucial for the recognition of a snake. An early candidate in question was a curvilinear body shape. Whilst this feature can play an important role (Wolfe et al., 1992; LoBue, 2014), alone, it is most probably not sufficient as worms (of a similar curvilinear body shape) evoked a smaller reaction in an ERP study (Van Strien et al., 2016). Next, the effect of body posture was investigated. Lobue and DeLoache (2011) suggested that a snake’s coiled body shape is responsible for the faster detection of snakes in relation to distractors. However, Masataka et al. (2010) argued that the striking (vs. relaxed) body posture rather enhanced the speed of detection but was not its basis per se. This notion was supported by Etting and Isbell (2014) in their rhesus macaque behavioural study. Recently, the topic of recognition-relevant features of snakes was reviewed by Kawai (2019). In concordance with the most current results, he concluded that snake scales are the key characteristic (He et al., 2014; Van Strien et al., 2016; Isbell and Etting, 2017; Van Strien and Isbell, 2017; Kawai, 2019). Interestingly, the importance of snake scales could be illustrated already in hominids, as some palaeolithic engravings likely represent a snakeskin (Coss and Charles, 2021). Although we agree with the studies on the importance of snake scales, this feature cannot explain the results of this study since it is the key for snake identification only in the context of other animals. Therefore, we propose to take a step back and look at the bigger picture once more.

Above, we suggested that the primate visual system is not only adapted for faster detection of snakes (Isbell, 2006, 2009) but also fine-tuned for the snake’s threat display. Notably, some snake-typical features which were previously found to enhance the speed of snake detection seem to be exaggerated in snakes’ threat display. In the case of vipers, such feature is the coiled shape (Lobue and DeLoache, 2011; Etting and Isbell, 2014), which is very tight under the threat creating many loops, whilst in cobras, the risen body front exaggerates the snake’s curvilinear body shape (Wolfe et al., 1992; LoBue, 2014). These exaggerated features typical for threat display might be behind the larger spontaneous attention paid to snakes in threatening postures. Moreover, the erect posture (seen in threatening cobras) is very conspicuous, and many animal species take advantage of it when wanting to intimidate an opponent. Indeed, people perceive animals in an erect posture as more fear-eliciting (Prokop et al., 2021). This, however, opens a question of whether the attentional privilege of snakes in threatening posture is driven by visual or affective features. It has been previously shown that emotions can modulate attention towards a stimulus. Soares et al. (2009) reported that participants found the animal they were afraid of faster than non-feared but fear-relevant animals. Similar results were also found by Miltner et al. (2004) or Lipp and Waters (2007). The arousal might also affect visual attentional performance (reviewed in Zsido, 2023). For instance, Zsido et al. (2022) showed that stimulus arousal might be an important cue facilitating target recognition in a memory test. As for now, however, we do not have enough data to decide the extant visual and affective features of snakes in threatening posture modulate attention and it might even be that these two types of features are inseparable in nature.

Although the overall pattern of reaction towards snakes was similar for both Somalis and Czechs, we would also like to shortly address the found difference. Czechs dwelled longer at the snake in threatening posture no matter the morphotype, but the same was true only for cobras in Somalis. Could it be that Somalis differentiate (in a perceptual sense) between threatening and non-threatening postures only in cobras simply because the difference is the most conspicuous? This explanation would not fit well into the evolutionary framework and indeed, it is not the case. In a complementary study, individual photos of snakes very similar to photos used in this experiment were presented to Somalis and they were asked to order them according to elicited fear from the most to the least fear-eliciting (Frynta et al., in prep.). In this forced-choice experiment, six out of eight viper species were rated as significantly more fear-eliciting when in the threatening posture than in the non-threatening posture (Frynta et al., in prep.). We instead suggest that Somalis attribute the same level of threat to vipers no matter their body posture leading to the same observed dwell time. LoBue (2014) previously showed that knowledge or expectations can interact with low-level features of the stimuli in visual search tasks. In our case, the (communal) knowledge of the nature of cobra and viper attacks seems to be the key. Cobras are active in their defence; they either flee or display to the opponent and strike only afterwards. Vipers, on the other hand, are passive; they often rely on their cryptic coloration and motionless stance and strike when the opponent (usually unknowingly) comes too close. Even though the majority of Somali participants were university students now living in a city, most of them came from rural areas and pastoral families. We find it very likely that they personally knew someone that was bitten by a snake. When we asked local villagers, at least one person was willing to share their experiences in every village. No communal knowledge can be expected in Czechs since local snake fauna is not dangerous to humans and, moreover, participants were mostly from urban areas. This finding might be of importance for future studies since it illustrates that not all snakes are the same (see also Landová et al., 2018, 2020; Janovcová et al., 2019; Rádlová et al., 2019; Frynta et al., this issue).

To conclude, our results show that human attention is directed more towards cobras and also vipers in threatening postures. We hypothesise that it is a result of primate-snake coevolution during which not only snakes represented a danger to primates but also primates represented a danger to snakes. To be clear, we do not argue that snakes evolved specific threatening postures in response to predator pressure from primates. That is very unlikely because of multiple reasons including primates are not snake’s primary predators, and the threatening postures are not addressed uniquely to primates but to a variety of other potential predators, e.g., mongooses (Herpestidae) or birds (the secretarybird Sagittarius serpentarius, crested seriema Cariama cristata, or others). We simply argue that at some point in evolutionary history, the primate-snake relationship must have become less one-sided since this is the situation we witness today (Headland and Greene, 2011; Falótico et al., 2018; see also Harris et al., 2021). As a part of their defensive behaviour, snakes would be displaying threat and these threat signals would in turn become associated with intense danger leading to prioritised attention toward threatening displays. The features of the threat display could not be reliably used for the detection of snakes because the threatening posture is a defensive behaviour and hence is not displayed when hunting for prey. Nonetheless, prioritised attention is still adaptive as the threat display signals the immediate danger of a ready-to-strike snake. Although the prioritised attention towards snakes was previously tested in several different cultures, this is the first study investigating a population from Sub-Saharan Africa – a key region with regards to the evolutionary Snake detection theory.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by The Institutional Review Board of Charles University, Faculty of Science and The Institutional Review Board of Amoud University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

DF and EL: conceptualization. IŠ: data curation. IŠ and DF: formal analysis, writing – original draft, and writing – review & editing. DF: funding acquisition and supervision. IŠ, MJ, VR, HE, KR, DK, DS, DB, PF, and DF: investigation. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was exclusively supported by the grant of the Czech Science Foundation (no. GACR 20-21608S).

Acknowledgments

For the Somaliland part of the study, we would like to thank the Amoud University management, namely, the former president, Suleiman A. Guleid, and the current president, Mohamed Muse Jibril. Further, we would like to thank minister Shukri H. Ismail and the director of wildlife Abdinasir Hussein Saed from the Ministry of Environment and climate change. For the Czech part of the study, we would like to thank Šárka Peléšková and Klára Bystroňová for their assistance with the Czech part of the experiment, Vojtěch Oram for his help in converting the data and Tomáš Mazuch for help with stimuli photography.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1234593/full#supplementary-material

References

Alves, R., Silva, V. N., Trovão, D. M., Oliveira, J. V., Mourão, J. S., Dias, T. L., et al. (2014). Students’ attitudes toward and knowledge about snakes in the semiarid region of Northeastern Brazil. J. Ethnobiol. Ethnomed. 10, 1–8. doi: 10.1186/1746-4269-10-30

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barlow, A., Wüster, W., Kelly, C. M. R., Branch, W. R., Phelps, T., and Tolley, K. A. (2019). Ancient habitat shifts and organismal diversification are decoupled in the African viper genus Bitis (Serpentes: Viperidae). J. Biogeogr. 46, 1234–1248. doi: 10.1111/jbi.13578

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bennie, M., Loaring, C., and Trim, S. (2011). Laboratory husbandry of arboreal tarantulas (Theraphosidae) and evaluation of environmental enrichment. Anim. Tech. Welf. 10, 163–169.

Google Scholar

Bertels, J., Bourguignon, M., De Heering, A., Chetail, F., De Tiège, X., Cleeremans, A., et al. (2020). Snakes elicit specific neural responses in the human infant brain. Sci. Rep. 10:7443. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-63619-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borgi, M., and Cirulli, F. (2015). Attitudes toward animals amongst kindergarten children: species preferences. Anthrozoös 28, 45–59. doi: 10.2752/089279315X14129350721939

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coelho, C. M., Suttiwan, P., Faiz, A. M., Ferreira-Santos, F., and Zsido, A. N. (2019). Are humans prepared to detect, fear, and avoid snakes? The mismatch between laboratory and ecological evidence. Front. Psychol. 10:2094. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coss, R. G., and Charles, E. P. (2021). The saliency of snake scales and leopard rosettes to infants: its relevance to graphical patterns portrayed in prehistoric art. Front. Psychol. 12:763436. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.763436

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, Y., and Hondzinski, J. M. (2006). Gaze tracking accuracy in humans: two eyes are better than one. Neurosci. Lett. 396, 257–262. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.11.071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeLoache, J. S., and LoBue, V. (2009). The narrow fellow in the grass: human infants associate snakes and fear. Develop. Sci. 12, 201–207. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00753.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eberle, M., and Kappeler, P. M. (2008). Mutualism, reciprocity, or kin selection? Cooperative rescue of a conspecific from a boa in a nocturnal solitary forager the gray mouse lemur. Am. J. Primatol. 70, 410–414. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20496

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Etting, S. F., and Isbell, L. A. (2014). Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) use posture to assess level of threat from snakes. Ethology 120, 1177–1184. doi: 10.1111/eth.12293

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Etting, S. F., Isbell, L. A., and Grote, M. N. (2014). Factors increasing snake detection and perceived threat in captive rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Am. J. Primatol. 76, 135–145. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falótico, T., Verderane, M. P., Mendonça-Furtado, O., Spagnoletti, N., Ottoni, E. B., Visalberghi, E., et al. (2018). Food or threat? Wild capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) as both predators and prey of snakes. Primates 59, 99–106. doi: 10.1007/s10329-017-0631-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fančovičová, J., Prokop, P., Szikhart, M., and Pazda, A. (2020). Snake coloration does not influence children’s detection time. Hum. Dimens. Wildl. 25, 489–497. doi: 10.1080/10871209.2020.1758252

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G* power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, E., Griggs, L., and Mouchlianitis, E. (2007). The detection of fear-relevant stimuli: are guns noticed as quickly as snakes? Emotion 7, 691–696. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.691

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frynta, D., Elmi, H. S. A., Rexová, K., Janovcová, M., Rudolfová, V., Štolhoferová, I., et al. (2023). Animals evoking fear in the cradle of humankind: snakes, scorpions, and large carnivores. Sci. Nat. 110:33. doi: 10.1007/s00114-023-01859-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerdes, A. B. M., Pauli, P., and Alpers, G. W. (2009). Toward and away from spiders: eye-movements in spider-fearful participants. J. Neur. Tr. 116, 725–733. doi: 10.1007/s00702-008-0167-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbons, A. (2017). Oldest members of our species discovered in Morocco. Science 356, 993–994. doi: 10.1126/science.356.6342.993

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gokula, V. (2011). An ethogram of spot-billed pelican (Pelecanus philippensis). Chinese Birds 2, 183–192. doi: 10.5122/cbirds.2011.0030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grassini, S., Holm, S. K., Railo, H., and Koivisto, M. (2016). Who is afraid of the invisible snake? Subjective visual awareness modulates posterior brain activity for evolutionarily threatening stimuli. Biol. Psychol. 121, 53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, H. W. (1988). “Antipredator mechanisms in reptiles” in Biology of the Reptilia. eds. C. Gans and R. B. Huey (New York: John Wiley and Sons), 152.

Google Scholar

Harris, R. J., Nekaris, K. A. I., and Fry, B. G. (2021). Monkeying around with venom: an increased resistance to α-neurotoxins supports an evolutionary arms race between afro-Asian primates and sympatric cobras. BMC Biol. 19:253. doi: 10.1186/s12915-021-01195-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayakawa, S., Kawai, N., and Masataka, N. (2011). The influence of color on snake detection in visual search in human children. Sci. Rep. 1, 1–4. doi: 10.1038/srep00080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, H., Kubo, K., and Kawai, N. (2014). Spiders do not evoke greater early posterior negativity in the event-related potential as snakes. Neuroreport 25, 1049–1053. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Headland, T. N., and Greene, H. W. (2011). Hunter–gatherers and other primates as prey, predators, and competitors of snakes. PNAS 108, E1470–E1474. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1115116108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isbell, L. A. (2006). Snakes as agents of evolutionary change in primate brains. J. Hum. Evol. 51, 1–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.12.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isbell, L. A. (2009). The fruit, the tree, and the serpent: Why we see so well. Great Britain: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Isbell, L. A., and Etting, S. F. (2017). Scales drive detection, attention, and memory of snakes in wild vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus pygerythrus). Primates 58, 121–129. doi: 10.1007/s10329-016-0562-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janovcová, M., Rádlová, S., Polák, J., Sedláčková, K., Peléšková, Š., Žampachová, B., et al. (2019). Human attitude toward reptiles: a relationship between fear, disgust, and aesthetic preferences. Animals 9:238. doi: 10.3390/ani9050238

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, C. H., and Caine, N. G. (2021). Preferential snake detection in a simulated ecological experiment. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 175, 895–904. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.24224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawai, N. (2019). “Searching for the critical features of snakes” in The fear of snakes: Evolutionary and psychobiological perspectives on our innate fear. ed. N. Kawai (Singapore: Springer), 121–153.

Google Scholar

Kawai, N., and He, H. (2016). Breaking snake camouflage: humans detect snakes more accurately than other animals under less discernible visual conditions. PlosOne 11:e0164342. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0164342

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawai, N., and Koda, H. (2016). Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) quickly detect snakes but not spiders: evolutionary origins of fear-relevant animals. J. Comp. Psychol. 130, 299–303. doi: 10.1037/com0000032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawai, N., and Qiu, H. (2020). Humans detect snakes more accurately and quickly than other animals under natural visual scenes: a flicker paradigm study. Cognition Emotion 34, 614–620. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2019.1657799

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, C. M. R., Barker, N. P., Villet, M. H., and Broadley, D. G. (2009). Phylogeny, biogeography and classification of the snake superfamily Elapoidea: a rapid radiation in the late Eocene. Cladistics 25, 38–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1096-0031.2008.00237.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landová, E., Bakhshaliyeva, N., Janovcová, M., Peléšková, Š., Suleymanova, M., Polák, J., et al. (2018). Association between fear and beauty evaluation of snakes: cross-cultural findings. Front. Psychol. 9:333. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landová, E., Peléšková, Š., Sedláčková, K., Janovcová, M., Polák, J., Rádlová, S., et al. (2020). Venomous snakes elicit stronger fear than nonvenomous ones: psychophysiological response to snake images. PlosOne 15:e0236999. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0236999

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langridge, K. V., Broom, M., and Osorio, D. (2007). Selective signalling by cuttlefish to predators. Curr. Biol. 17, R1044–R1045. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenth, R. (2022). Emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. Available at: https://github.com/rvlenth/emmeans

Google Scholar

Lipp, O. V., and Waters, A. M. (2007). When danger lurks in the background: attentional capture by animal fear-relevant distracters is specific and selectively enhanced by animal fear. Emotion 7, 192–200. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LoBue, V. (2014). Deconstructing the snake: the relative roles of perception, cognition, and emotion on threat detection. Emotion 14, 701–711. doi: 10.1037/a0035898

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lobue, V., and DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Detecting the snake in the grass: attention to fear-relevant stimuli by adults and young children. Psychol. Sci. 19, 284–289. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02081.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lobue, V., and DeLoache, J. S. (2011). What's so special about slithering serpents? Children and adults rapidly detect snakes based on their simple features. Vis. Cogn. 19, 129–143. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2010.522216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masataka, N., Hayakawa, S., and Kawai, N. (2010). Human young children as well as adults demonstrate ‘superior’ rapid Snake detection when typical striking posture is displayed by the Snake. PlosOne 5:e15122. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miltner, W. H. R., Krieschel, S., Hecht, H., Trippe, R., and Weiss, T. (2004). Eye movement and behavioral responses to threatenting and nonthreatening stimuli during visual search in phobic and nonphobic subjects. Emotion 4, 323–339. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.4.4.323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, S. G., and King, J. E. (1973). Snake avoidance in feral and laboratory reared squirrel monkeys. Behaviour 47, 281–288. doi: 10.1163/156853973X00120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishida, T., Kano, T., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., and Nakamura, M. (1999). Ethogram and ethnography of Mahale chimpanzees. Anthropol. Sci. 107, 141–188. doi: 10.1537/ase.107.141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Öhman, A., Flykt, A., and Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130, 466–478. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Öhman, A., and Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol. Rev. 108, 483–522. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.483

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Öhman, A., and Mineka, S. (2003). The malicious serpent: snakes as a prototypical stimulus for an evolved module of fear. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 12, 5–9. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okon-Singer, H., Alyagon, U., Kofman, O., Tzelgov, J., and Henik, A. (2011). Fear-related pictures deteriorate the performance of university students with high fear of snakes or spiders. Stress 14, 185–193. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2010.527401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onyishi, I. E., Nwonyi, S. K., Pazda, A., and Prokop, P. (2021). Attitudes and behaviour toward snakes on the part of Igbo people in southeastern Nigeria. Sci. Total Environ. 763:143045. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.143045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orquin, J. L., and Holmqvist, K. (2018). Threats to the validity of eye-movement research in psychology. Behav. Res. Methods 50, 1645–1656. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0998-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pandey, D. P., Subedi Pandey, G., Devkota, K., and Goode, M. (2016). Public perceptions of snakes and snakebite management: implications for conservation and human health in southern Nepal. J. Ethnobiol. Ethnomed. 12, 1–25. doi: 10.1186/s13002-016-0092-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penkunas, M. J., and Coss, R. G. (2013). A comparison of rural and urban Indian children's visual detection of threatening and nonthreatening animals. Develop. Sci. 16, 463–475. doi: 10.1111/desc.12043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, S., Manson, J. H., Dower, G., and Wikberg, E. (2003). White-faced capuchins cooperate to rescue a groupmate from a Boa constrictor. Folia Primatol. 74, 109–111. doi: 10.1159/000070008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinheiro, J., and Bates, D., R Core Team (2022). Nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R package. Available at: https://svn.r-project.org/R-packages/trunk/nlme/

Google Scholar

Prokop, P., Fančovičová, J., and Kučerová, A. (2018). Aposematic colouration does not explain fear of snakes in humans. J. Ethol. 36, 35–41. doi: 10.1007/s10164-017-0533-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prokop, P., Zvaríková, M., Zvarík, M., Pazda, A., and Fedor, P. (2021). The effect of animal bipedal posture on perceived cuteness, fear, and willingness to protect them. Front. Ecol. Evol. 9:681241. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2021.681241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prüfer, K., Posth, C., Yu, H., Stoessel, A., Spyrou, M. A., Deviese, T., et al. (2021). A genome sequence from a modern human skull over 45,000 years old from Zlatý kůň in Czechia. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 5, 820–825. doi: 10.1038/s41559-021-01443-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Development Core Team (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing

Google Scholar

Rádlová, S., Janovcová, M., Sedláčková, K., Polák, J., Nácar, D., Peléšková, Š., et al. (2019). Snakes represent emotionally salient stimuli that may evoke both fear and disgust. Front. Psychol. 10:1085. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01085

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudolfová, V., Štolhoferová, I., Elmi, H. S. A., Rádlová, S., Rexová, K., Berti, D. A., et al. (2022). Do spiders ride on the fear of scorpions? A cross-cultural eye tracking study. Animals 12:3466. doi: 10.3390/ani12243466

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E. P. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behav. Ther. 2, 307–320. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(71)80064-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., and Marler, P. (1980). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210, 801–803. doi: 10.1126/science.7433999

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shibasaki, M., and Kawai, N. (2009). Rapid detection of snakes by Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata): an evolutionarily predisposed visual system. J. Comp. Psychol. 123:131. doi: 10.1037/a0015095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shibasaki, M., and Kawai, N. (2011). Visual searching for fear-relevant stimuli: snakes draw our attention more strongly than spiders do. Cogn. Stud. Bull. Jap. Cogn. Sci. Soc. 18, 158–172.

Google Scholar

Shine, R. (1990). Function and evolution of the frill of the frillneck lizard, Chlamydosaurus kingii (Sauria: Agamidae). Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 40, 11–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1990.tb00531.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Šmíd, J., and Tolley, K. A. (2019). Calibrating the tree of vipers under the fossilized birth-death model. Sci. Rep. 9:5510. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-41290-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soares, S. C., Esteves, F., and Flykt, A. (2009). Fear, but not fear-relevance, modulates reaction times in visual search with animal distractors. J. Anxiety Disord. 23, 136–144. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soares, S. C., and Esteves, F. (2013). A glimpse of fear: Fast detection of threatening targets in visual search with brief stimulus durations. PsyCh J. 2, 11–16. doi: 10.1002/pchj.18

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soares, S. C., Lindström, B., Esteves, F., and Öhman, A. (2014). The hidden snake in the grass: superior detection of snakes in challenging attentional conditions. PLoS One 9:e114724. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114724

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stringer, C. (2016). The origin and evolution of Homo sapiens. Philos. T. Roy. Soc. B 371:20150237. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Subra, B., Muller, D., Fourgassie, L., Chauvin, A., and Alexopoulos, T. (2018). Of guns and snakes: testing a modern threat superiority effect. Cognition Emotion 32, 81–91. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1284044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teixeira, D. S., dos Santos, E., Leal, S. G., de Jesus, A. K., Vargas, W. P., Dutra, I., et al. (2016). Fatal attack on black-tufted-ear marmosets (Callithrix penicillata) by a Boa constrictor: a simultaneous assault on two juvenile monkeys. Primates 57, 123–127. doi: 10.1007/s10329-015-0495-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tierney, K. J., and Connolly, M. K. (2013). A review of the evidence for a biological basis for snake fears in humans. Psychol. Rec. 63, 919–928. doi: 10.11133/j.tpr.2013.63.4.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Le, Q., Isbell, L. A., Matsumoto, J., Le, V. Q., Hori, E., Tran, A. H., et al. (2014). Monkey pulvinar neurons fire differentially to snake postures. PlosOne 9:e114258. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Le, Q., Isbell, L. A., Matsumoto, J., Nguyen, M., Hori, E., Maior, R. S., et al. (2013). Pulvinar neurons reveal neurobiological evidence of past selection for rapid detection of snakes. PNAS 110, 19000–19005. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312648110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Strien, J. W., Christiaans, G., Franken, I. H. A., and Huijding, J. (2016). Curvilinear shapes and the snake detection hypothesis: an ERP study. Psychophysiology 53, 252–257. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12564

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Strien, J. W., and Isbell, L. A. (2017). Snake scales, partial exposure, and the Snake detection theory: a human event-related potentials study. Sci. Rep. 7:46331. doi: 10.1038/srep46331

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 585–594. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, L., Brandl, P., and Frynta, D. (2015). Fear reactions to snakes in naïve mouse lemurs and pig-tailed macaques. Primates 56, 279–284. doi: 10.1007/s10329-015-0473-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfe, J. M., Yee, A., and Friedman-Hill, S. R. (1992). Curvature is a basic feature for visual search tasks. Perception 21, 465–480. doi: 10.1068/p210465

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wombolt, J. R., and Caine, N. G. (2016). Patterns on serpentine shapes elicit visual attention in marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Am. J. Primatol. 78, 928–936. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22563

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaher, H., Murphy, R. W., Arredondo, J. C., Graboski, R., Machado-Filho, P. R., Mahlow, K., et al. (2019). Large-scale molecular phylogeny, morphology, divergence-time estimation, and the fossil record of advanced caenophidian snakes (Squamata: Serpentes). PlosOne 14:e0216148. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0216148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, B., Zhou, Z.-G., Zhou, Y., and Chen, Y.-C. (2020). Increased attention to snake images in cynomolgus monkeys: an eye-tracking study. Zool. Res. 41, 32–38. doi: 10.24272/j.issn.2095-8137.2020.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zsido, A. N. (2023). The effect of emotional arousal on visual attentional performance: a systematic review. Psychol. Res. 2023, 1–24. doi: 10.1007/s00426-023-01852-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zsido, A. N., Csatho, A., Matuz, A., Stecina, D., Arato, A., Inhof, O., et al. (2019a). Does threat have an advantage after all? Proposing a novel experimental design to investigate the advantages of threat-relevant cues in visual processing. Front. Psychol. 10:2217. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02217

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zsido, A. N., Deak, A., and Bernath, L. (2019b). Is a snake scarier than a gun? The ontogenetic–phylogenetic dispute from a new perspective: the role of arousal. Emotion 19, 726–732. doi: 10.1037/emo0000478

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zsido, A. N., Stecina, D. T., and Hout, M. C. (2022). Task demands determine whether shape or arousal of a stimulus modulates competition for visual working memory resources. Acta Psychol. 224:103523. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103523

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Africa, eye-tracking, snake, Snake detection theory, spontaneous attention

Citation: Štolhoferová I, Frynta D, Janovcová M, Rudolfová V, Elmi HSA, Rexová K, Berti DA, Král D, Sommer D, Landová E and Frýdlová P (2023) The bigger the threat, the longer the gaze? A cross-cultural study of Somalis and Czechs. Front. Psychol. 14:1234593. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1234593

Received: 04 June 2023; Accepted: 13 September 2023;
Published: 27 September 2023.

Edited by:

Carlos M. Coelho, Chulalongkorn University, Thailand

Reviewed by:

Andras Norbert Zsido, University of Pécs, Hungary
Pavol Prokop, Comenius University, Slovakia

Copyright © 2023 Štolhoferová, Frynta, Janovcová, Rudolfová, Elmi, Rexová, Berti, Král, Sommer, Landová and Frýdlová. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Petra Frýdlová, petra.frydlova@natur.cuni.cz

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.