Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 15 November 2023
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Effect of Parenting on Child and Adolescence Development View all 15 articles

Parenting styles, empathy and aggressive behavior in preschool children: an examination of mediating mechanisms

\r\nZhumei LinZhumei Lin1Ziqian ZhouZiqian Zhou1Lijun ZhuLijun Zhu2Weige Wu,*Weige Wu3,4*
  • 1Department of Psychology, Xiamen Medical College, Xiamen, China
  • 2Second Affiliated Hospital of Xiamen Medical College, Xiamen, China
  • 3Xiamen Xianyue Hospital, Xianyue Hospital Affiliated with Xiamen Medical College, Xiamen, China
  • 4Fujian Psychiatric Center, Fujian Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders, Xiamen, China

Introduction: This study examined the interplay between parenting styles, empathy, and aggressive behavior in Chinese preschool children aged 3–5 years.

Methods: Data were collected from 87 participants using the Child Behavior Checklist, Children’s Empathy Quotient, and Parenting Style Questionnaire, and were subsequently analyzed.

Results: The findings revealed significant age and gender differences in empathy, but not in parenting styles or aggressive behavior. Additionally, a substantial correlation was identified between authoritarian parenting style and aggressive behavior, as well as between children’s empathy levels and aggressive behavior. This indicates that empathy may act as a mediator between parenting style and aggressive behavior.

Discussion: Our findings suggest that an authoritarian parenting style influences aggressive behavior both directly and indirectly through its effect on children’s empathy. These results point toward the possibility that an authoritarian parenting style may stifle the development of empathy in preschool children, subsequently heightening their aggressive behavior.

1. Introduction

Aggressive behavior (AB), characterized by an individual’s intentional infliction of physical or mental harm on others or unlawful possession of their property, defies social norms (Perlman and Hirdes, 2010). The majority of studies investigating antisocial behavior prevalence and its progression during early to middle childhood have primarily concentrated on aggressive antisocial behavior. Previous studies have shown that aggression develops at an early age, and that there is a degree of continuity in general aggressive behavior from childhood and adolescence to adulthood (Wahl and Metzner, 2012). An alarmingly high prevalence of aggressive tendencies has been observed in children and has emerged as a pressing public health issue in recent years (Ahemaitijiang et al., 2021). Caims (1979) pointed out that the frequency of aggressive behavior is highest in preschool children and decreases linearly with age. The detection rate of preschool children’s behavioral problems in China reached 14.8% (Yajing et al., 2019), which is higher than the 8–10% reported in Western countries (Wichstrøm et al., 2017). Research on children’s aggressive behavior found that boys were generally more aggressive than girls (Zhang et al., 2003; Yokota, 2017). Aggressive behavior has been identified as an important precursor to numerous social maladjustments, such as juvenile delinquency, peer rejection, and various criminal activities (Reijntjes et al., 2011; Burt et al., 2016).

Parenting style represents the family emotional context in which parent-child interactions take place (Park and Walton-Moss, 2012). The parenting style model developed by Baumrind is the most widely cited. According to a number of relevant dimensions (i.e., acceptance, control, demandingness and disciplinary practices), Baumrind (1968) proposed the three prototypes of parenting style as authoritative, authoritarian and permissive. Authoritarian parents emphasize discipline and obedience. They are less likely to discuss rules with their children and believe in strict adherence to their rules. The parenting environment they provide is distant, cold and limited to one-way communication. In contrast, authoritative parents are more democratic and concerned with explaining the rules and helping the child understand the reasons behind them, rather than strictly following the rules. This is conceptually different from authoritarian parenting in that these parents use two-way communication and reasoning with their children. Based on Maccoby and Martin (1983) proposed a two-dimensional model (parental responsiveness and parental demandingness) and created a typology of four parenting styles: authoritative (parental warmth and parental strictness together), authoritarian (parental strictness but not parental warmth), indulgent (parental warmth but not parental strictness) and neglectful (neither parental warmth nor parental strictness). Based on these models, a considerable number of studies have investigated the influence of parenting on children’s emotional and behavioral development (Perez-Gramaje et al., 2020).

Parenting style has been found to significantly impact the aggressive behavior exhibited by children and adolescents (Lemola et al., 2012; Davies et al., 2019; Qi, 2019). Parental behaviors involving punishment, rejection, or exclusion and other negative parenting styles can induce children and adolescents to mimic delinquent behaviors, intensify hostile emotions and cognitive attributions (Gershoff, 2002; Goulter et al., 2020), stifle and obstruct the growth of emotional and behavioral regulation abilities (Thibodeau et al., 2015; Ganiban et al., 2021), thereby facilitating the emergence of aggressive behaviors (Zhang et al., 2016; Ahemaitijiang et al., 2021). For instance, children who endured severe maltreatment during their early years are prone to display heightened antisocial aggressive behavior (Masud et al., 2019). In contrast, positive parenting styles, such as parental nurturing and appropriate restraint, have demonstrated a deterring effect on antisocial behavior (Criss et al., 2010; Kostulski et al., 2021). Moreover, parenting style not only influences the evolution of children’s social behavior but also their empathy capabilities (Schaffer et al., 2009). Prior researches suggest that parenting serves as a typical extrinsic factor shaping children’s empathy development, with supportive parenting styles exhibiting a positive correlation with children’s empathetic traits (Knafo and Plomin, 2006; Movsesjan et al., 2014; Ma et al., 2020), which are crucial for the progression of aggressive behavior.

Empathy is defined as an individual’s capacity to acknowledge and comprehend the thoughts and emotions of others, and to evaluate and identify problems from others’ perspectives (Decety and Jackson, 2010). It is postulated as a potent mitigator for the development of severe behavioral issues in children (Combe, 2021). Children demonstrating high levels of empathy are more sensitive to others’ emotional states, and this shared emotional recognition can stimulate prosocial behavior while suppressing detrimental actions toward others (Eisenberg et al., 2004; Combe, 2021). Many studies have pointed out that preschoolers’ cognitive and emotional empathy abilities improved significantly with age (Decety and Svetlova, 2012). The main factor influencing the development of emotional empathy in adolescents is the increase in age, and the development of empathy will gradually show gender differences with age. Research on gender differences in empathy has yielded fruitful results. Most studies show that the level of empathy is higher in girls than in boys (Auyeung et al., 2009) while other few studies found no gender differences (Volbrecht et al., 2007). This may be because there are different components of empathy, and the different components may show different gender differences.

While numerous studies have explored the connections between parenting style, empathy, and aggressive behavior, the results have been inconsistent. For example, Miller et al. (2014) found that the impact of various parenting styles on children’s conduct problems depends on their level of empathy. Conversely, Avc and Sak (2021) found no mediating effect of empathy between parenting styles and aggression. Moreover, the majority of these studies primarily focused on adolescents, largely neglecting younger children. Currently, no research has been undertaken specifically on preschool children to examine the intricate relationships between these three factors. Considering that aggressive behaviors in children can surface as early as two years old and rapidly escalate throughout the preschool years (Zhang et al., 2003), the influence of parental behaviors at this stage could present a window of opportunity to modify the trajectory of conduct problems. Therefore, this study aims to investigate the relationships between parenting style, aggressive behavior, and empathy among preschool children. This focus might provide a more effective framework for understanding the mechanisms underlying the development of children’s aggressive behavior.

In this study, we consider a sample of Chinese preschool children aged between 3 and 5 years to explore the effects of parenting style and children’s empathy levels on their aggressive behavior. We also aim to uncover the interaction mechanisms between these influencing factors. Based on a review of the existing literature (Miller et al., 2014), we hypothesize that empathy level acts as a mediator between parenting style and children’s aggressive behavior.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Sample and procedure

A total of 94 preschool children aged 3–5 years were selected from a kindergarten in Chengde, China, employing a cluster sampling approach. Parents were given questionnaires, and after excluding incomplete or repetitive responses, we analyzed data from 87 participants, resulting in a response rate of 92.6%. The participant cohort comprised 30 children aged 3 (Mage = 3.7, SD = 0.19, 53.3% boys), 28 aged 4 (Mage = 4.7, SD = 0.18, 46.4% boys), and 29 aged 5 (Mage = 5.6, SD = 0.19, 37.9% boys). The primary caregivers, while at the kindergarten, provided demographic information and completed the questionnaires anonymously. They could withdraw their participation at any stage, and the completed questionnaires were collected immediately. The survey was conducted following obtaining consent from both the kindergarten and the children’s parents or primary caregivers. The Academic Ethics Committee of Xiamen Medical College granted approval for this study.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Child behavior checklist

The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) compiled by Achenbach (2011), was used to evaluate children’s aggressive behavior. This scale, designed for parents, teachers, and children over 10 years old, is a recognized tool for child behavior assessment. The parent form of the CBCL-Aggression Subscale assesses preschool children’s aggressive behavior, featuring 28 items such as “My child physically assaults others.” The scale utilizes a 3-point Likert scale (1 = rarely or never, 3 = most of the time or always), with higher scores indicating higher aggression levels. This study utilized the parent form of the CBCL-Aggression Subscale, demonstrating a high internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.91).

2.2.2. Children’s empathy quotient

The Children’s Empathy Quotient (EQ-C) developed by Auyeung et al. (2009) and translated into Chinese by Peng (2017), was employed to measure children’s empathy. The EQ-C comprises 27 items, e.g., “My child likes to look after other people.” The guardians of the child complete the measure using a 4-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 4 = strongly agree). Higher scores signify a higher level of children’s empathy. In this study, the Cronbach’s α of the scale was 0.79. Despite poor discrimination for items 9, 15, 22, and 23 [t statistics non-significant level (p > 0.05)], these items were not eliminated to maintain comparison consistency with existing literature. The Cronbach’s α of the scale improved (above 0.8) after deleting these four items.

2.2.3. Parenting style questionnaire

Parenting styles were assessed using the Parenting Style Questionnaire compiled by Chen and Luster (1999), encompassing authoritative, authoritarian, and laissez-faire dimensions. However, only the authoritative and authoritarian styles were investigated in this study, reflecting the modern parenting trend where parents prioritize their children’s education and seldom adopt a laissez-faire style (Gong, 2006). The questionnaire includes 40 items such as “I have patience with children.” A 5-point Likert scale (1 = completely non-compliant, 5 = fully compliant) was used. The Cronbach’s α for the authoritative dimension was 0.90, for the authoritarian dimension was 0.85, and the overall Cronbach’s α was 0.80.

2.3. Statistical analysis

Data was analyzed using SPSS 20.0. The analysis incorporated multivariate analysis of variance, partial correlation analysis, and multiple regression analysis. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. Means and standard deviations were used to describe the study variables, and partial correlation coefficients were applied to analyze the correlations between variables.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive statistics and variance analysis

Parental educational levels were categorized as follows: 1 = never went to school; 2 = sixth grade or less; 3 = more than sixth grade, but did not graduate high school; 4 = high school graduate; 5 = college or university graduate; 6 = professional training beyond a 4-year college. The most frequent level of education for both fathers and mothers was 4, indicating high school level education. The mean level of average parental education was 4.2 (SD = 0.84), signifying some level of post-high school education. Accounting for parental education as a covariate, the means, standard deviations, and results from the variance analysis are displayed in Table 1. There was a significant effect of age on children’s empathy levels (p < 0.01, ?2 = 0.21). Post hoc tests using Tukey-HSD showed that the empathy levels of 3-year-old children were lower than those of 4- and 5-year-old children (p3–4 < 0.001, p3–5 < 0.01), with no significant difference detected between the 4- and 5-year-old groups. Additionally, the main effect of gender on empathy was significant (p < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.65), suggesting that girls demonstrated higher empathy levels than boys.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics, variance analysis between variables.

3.2. Partial correlation analysis

In the correlation and mediation analyses, gender, age, and parental education were used as control variables. The partial correlation results (see Table 2) indicated a positive correlation between children’s empathy and authoritative parenting style, whereas negative correlations were found with the authoritarian parenting style and aggressive behavior. The correlation between authoritarian parenting style and aggressive behavior was significant, which was not the case for the authoritative parenting style. These results suggest that children’s empathy might mediate the relationship between the authoritarian parenting style (but not the authoritative style) and children’s aggressive behavior.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlation between variables.

3.3. Mediation analyses

Controlling for gender, age, and parental education, the mediation effect was analyzed using regression (see Table 3). The regression coefficient of aggression on authoritarian parenting style was significant (c = 0.43, t = 4.07, p < 0.001). Additionally, the regression coefficients of empathy on authoritarian style (a = −0.40, t = −4.10, p < 0.001) and aggression on empathy (b = −0.36, t = −3.10, p < 0.01) were significant, suggesting a significant indirect effect. Further analysis showed that the regression coefficient of aggressive behavior on authoritarian parenting style remained significant (c’ = 0.43, t = 4.06, p < 0.001), indicating a significant direct effect, with ab and c’ differing in direction. These results suggest that children’s empathy partially mediates the relationship between authoritarian parenting style and aggressive behavior in a negative manner, accounting for 33% of the total effect. Conversely, the regression coefficient of aggression on authoritative parenting style was not significant (c = −0.21, t = −1.87, p > 0.05).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Regression analysis of the mediating effect of empathy.

4. Discussion

4.1. Child empathy, aggression, and parenting styles: developmental trends

Our preliminary analysis uncovered no significant age or sex differences in parenting styles, mirroring the findings of Gong (2006). Such uniformity could be attributed to contemporary Chinese family structures, typically comprising one or two children, leading parents to invest equally in their child’s upbringing, irrespective of gender. Moreover, since the children in our study were preschoolers, their kindergarten experiences, including learning tasks and other facets, might lack sufficient variability to elicit distinctive parenting styles based on age. Intriguingly, we observed no significant age or gender differences in children’s aggressive behavior either, contrasting with earlier studies (Zhang et al., 2003; Yokota, 2017). We speculate that this may be due to the homogeneity of parenting styles, which fails to delineate differences in aggressive behavior by age or gender.

Contrastingly, significant age and gender differences were observed in children’s empathic tendencies. Specifically, 4- and 5-year-olds exhibited higher empathy levels than 3-year-olds, implying that children’s ability to understand and share others’ emotions improves with age, likely due to maturing cognitive and emotional capacities. Additionally, girls showed higher empathy levels than boys, aligning with prior studies (O’Brien et al., 2013; Sandy et al., 2017; Agnieszka et al., 2020). Such findings imply that gender disparities in empathy emerge as early as ages 3 to 5, potentially stemming from societal expectations of differing gender roles. Cultural norms often encourage girls to be more empathetic and care for others, while boys are urged to be analytical and practical (Lam et al., 2012). According to gender role socialization theory, parents’ rearing pattern of their children is influenced by gender stereotypes, resulting in different parenting needs for boys and girls (Hastings and Derose, 2005). Parents of girls are more likely to emphasize prosocial behaviors and are more likely to talk to girls about other people’s feelings, especially emotional feelings, which will strengthen girls’ ability to be attentive to the needs of others (Coltrane and Adams, 2008). Parents of boys place more emphasis on independence and exploration. As part of the social environment, children are influenced by their parents’ different upbringing, so that both sexes gradually accept different gender role norms in the process of socialization. When women successfully internalize gender roles, they tend to be more sensitive than men to the needs of others in the process of communication, including sensitivity to the emotions of others, especially the negative emotions of others (Jiajin et al., 2010).

The observed gender difference in empathy, despite uniform parenting styles, hints at influences beyond societal role expectations, possibly including inherent physiological factors (Field et al., 2007). Neurobiological research has linked empathy disparities between genders to hormonal factors (Lutchmaya et al., 2002; Honk et al., 2011). For instance, the hormone oxytocin reportedly enhances emotional empathy, while testosterone negatively impacts cognitive empathy, thus contributing to noticeable empathy differences between genders (Chapman et al., 2006; Honk et al., 2011). This biological evidence supports the idea that innate physiological traits partly explain gender differences in empathy. To some extent, the gender differences of empathy in present study may also be attributed to the physiological factors.

4.2. Empathy as a mediator

Parenting style is generally considered to have a significant impact on children’s aggressive behavior (Ahemaitijiang et al., 2021). In this study, we found a significant negative correlation between authoritarian parenting style and children’s aggressive behavior, while no correlation was found between authoritative parenting and children’s aggressive behavior. Authoritarian parents who demand absolute obedience from their children, even resorting to threats and punishment, and rarely consider the children’s needs and wishes, may inadvertently breed aggressive behavior. Furthermore, parenting styles are significantly correlated with children’s empathy level, with children’s empathy being positively correlated with authoritative parenting styles and negatively with authoritarian styles. This aligns with existing studies, signifying that different parenting styles yield different impacts on empathy (Jolliffe and Farrington, 2004; Pinquart, 2017).

Authoritative parents get along with children in an accepting and warm way and the encouragement and warmth from parents can promote the development of children’s empathy (Ma et al., 2020). Because in this parenting way, children’s own feelings are accepted and they are more inclined to emotionally understand and accept others. Inversely, when children are treated negatively and rudely, the development of their empathy ability will be negatively affected. Moreover, children’s empathy level also showed a significantly negative correlation with their aggressive behavior, matching previous findings (Shaleh and Binti, 2021; Gómez-Leal et al., 2022). Highly empathetic children are more attuned to others’ emotions and needs, thus promoting prosocial behaviors while avoiding or reducing aggression toward others. On the other hand, children with low empathy are less capable of perceiving emotional information, resulting in more aggressive antisocial behaviors. These results bolster the notion that empathy plays a vital role in fostering moral behavior development and social adaptation in individuals (Vachon et al., 2014).

Previous studies have focused on the influence of parenting style and empathy on children’s aggressive behavior, but this study’s regression analysis results validated that empathy plays a mediating role between parenting style and aggressive behavior in preschool children. Specifically, children’s empathy level plays a partially negative mediating role between authoritarian parenting style and children’s aggressive behavior, consistent with Miller, Johnston, and Pasalich (Miller et al., 2014). The more authoritarian parenting is, the more children’s aggressive behavior will be, and this effect is mediated by the reduction of children’s empathy level due to authoritarian parenting. Overly authoritarian approaches disrupt the intimate balance between children and parents, impacting children’s empathy ability, and thereby increasing children’s aggressive behavior (Yoo et al., 2013).

In current research, there is a controversial subject about which parenting style is associated with better psychosocial adjustment of the child. Contrary to previous suggestions that parental strictness and coercion may be beneficial in raising aggressive adolescents, there are findings that highlight the positive impact of parental warmth without strictness, even with aggressive adolescents (Perez-Gramaje et al., 2020). This may be due to the fact that the subjects were at different ages, which shows that we should adopt different appropriate ways of bringing up children and adolescents. At the preschool stage, parents should minimize authoritarian behavior and emotionally accept children, respect their preferences and opinions, encourage self-expression and respond efficiently to their needs. Nagging parenting styles can provide children with a destructive model for developing empathy, which in turn can increase their aggressive behavior. There’s also research showing that indulgent parenting (warmth without strictness) is associated with equal or better empathy than the authoritative style (warmth and strictness), while non-warm parenting (authoritarian and neglectful) is consistently associated with poor child adjustment (Fuentes et al., 2022). This finding is interesting given the general emphasis in China that parents should not spoil their children. As modern Chinese parents rarely exhibit neglectful parenting styles (Gong, 2006), and indulgent parenting styles were not investigated in this study, future research can explore the relationship between indulgent parenting styles and child behavior in different cultural contexts.

4.3. Limitations and future directions

Despite the meaningful findings drawn from this study, there exist some limitations that need to be acknowledged, and these pave the way for future research directions. Firstly, the use of a cross-sectional design hinders the possibility of capturing developmental changes in children’s empathy and aggressive behavior over time or establishing causality between variables. To overcome this limitation, future research could consider adopting a longitudinal design that would provide a dynamic view of these psychological phenomena. And increased collection of demographic information, such as who (mother/father/both) completed the parental survey and the marital status of the parents, could provide additional layers of understanding when interpreting the results.

Secondly, our findings, which are based on data drawn from a single kindergarten’s preschool children, may not necessarily generalize to broader populations. The sample’s homogeneity may limit the universal application of our findings. Future studies should seek to diversify the samples and settings, potentially including multiple kindergartens from different regions or even different countries.

Thirdly, the parent-reported measures used to collect data on parenting styles, children’s empathy, and aggression may be subject to bias and may not accurately represent children’s realities. Future research might employ more direct and diversified data collection methods, such as child interviews, teacher reports, or observational methods, to gain a more comprehensive and accurate understanding of children’s empathy and aggression, as well as parenting styles.

Lastly, the constructs of empathy can be divided into cognitive and affective empathy, which may show different associations with aggression. Accordingly, future research should consider exploring the mediating roles of different types of empathy in the relationship between parenting styles and aggressive behavior. This exploration would deepen the understanding of how specific components of empathy influence children’s aggressive behavior under different parenting styles.

5. Conclusion

In conclusion, our research investigated the relationship between parenting style, children’s empathy, and aggressive behavior among Chinese preschoolers aged 3–5 years. The findings demonstrated that an authoritarian parenting style predicts children’s aggression both directly and indirectly, through the mediating role of children’s empathy. We found that as the level of authoritarian parenting increases, children’s empathy development decreases, leading to a rise in aggressive behavior. These findings underscore the critical importance of parenting styles in shaping children’s social-emotional development. They also highlight the pivotal role of empathy as a buffer against aggressive behavior, thereby emphasizing the importance of fostering empathy in early childhood. Our study implies that for healthier child development, it is essential for parents to reduce the rough parenting strategies that restrict empathy in children. This not only promotes social-emotional well-being but can also serve as a powerful tool to minimize aggressive behaviors.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in this article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Academic Ethics Committee of Xiamen Medical College. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

ZL: writing – original draft, investigation, and data curation. ZZ and LZ: data collection and methodology. WW: writing – review and editing. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by the Xiamen Medical and Health Guiding Project (3502Z20214ZD1281), the Natural Science Foundation of Fujian Province (2023J05290), and Young and Middle-aged Teacher Education Research Project of Fujian (JAS22175).

Acknowledgments

We thank the Kindergarden contributing to the data collection: No. 3 Experimental Kindergarten of Chengde City.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1243623/full#supplementary-material

References

Achenbach, T. M. (2011). “Child Behavior Checklist for Ages,” in Encyclopedia of clinical neuropsychology, eds J. S. Kreutzer, J. DeLuca, and B. Caplan (New York, NY: Springer), doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_1529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agnieszka, L., Katarzyna, T., and Sandra, B. (2020). Empathy, resilience, and gratitude - does gender make a difference? Anal. Psicol. 36, 521–532. doi: 10.6018/analesps.36.3.391541

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahemaitijiang, N., Ren, H., Wang, H., and Han, Z. R. (2021). Longitudinal association between emotion-related parenting behavior and child aggression: The moderating role of parent-child physiological synchrony. Aggress. Behav. 47, 267–275. doi: 10.1002/ab.21945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auyeung, B., Wheelwright, S., Allison, C., Atkinson, M., Samarawickrema, N., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). The children’s empathy quotient and systemizing quotient: Sex differences in typical development and in autism spectrum conditions. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 1509–1521. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0772-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avc, E., and Sak, R. (2021). The relationship between parenting styles and fourth graders levels of empathy and aggressiveness. Curr. Psychol. 40, 510–522. doi: 10.1007/s12144-018-9959-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumrind, D. (1968). Authoritarian vs authoritative parental control. Adolescence 3, 255–272.

Google Scholar

Burt, S. A., Donnellan, M. B., Slawinski, B. L., and Klump, K. L. (2016). The phenomenology of non-aggressive antisocial behavior during childhood. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 44, 651–661. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0076-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caims, R. B. (1979). Social development: The origins and plasticity of interchanges. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.

Google Scholar

Chapman, E., Baron-Cohen, S., Auyeung, B., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., and Hackett, G. (2006). Fetal testosterone and empathy: Evidence from the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test. Soc. Neurosci. 1, 135–148. doi: 10.1080/17470910600992239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F. M., and Luster, T. (1999). “Chinese Parenting Reconsideration: Parenting Practices in Taiwan,” in Proceedings of the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting, Albuquerque, NM.

Google Scholar

Coltrane, S., and Adams, M. (2008). Gender and Families. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.

Google Scholar

Combe, K. L. (2021). Empathy and prosocial behaviour in early childhood: A review. J. Health Visit. 9, 380–390. doi: 10.12968/johv.2021.9.9.380

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Criss, M. M., Shaw, D. S., and Ingoldsby, E. M. (2010). Mother–son positive synchrony in middle childhood: Relation to antisocial behavior. Soc. Dev. 12, 379–400. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, P. T., Pearson, J. K., Cicchetti, D., Martin, M. J., and Cummings, E. M. (2019). Emotional insecurity as a mediator of the moderating role of dopamine genes in the association between interparental conflict and youth externalizing problems. Dev. Psychopathol. 31, 1111–1126. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419000634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., and Jackson, P. L. (2010). A social-neuroscience perspective on empathy. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 54–58. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00406.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., and Svetlova, M. (2012). Putting together phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspectives on empathy. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2, 1–24. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., Liew, J., and Pidada, S. U. (2004). The longitudinal relations of regulation and emotionality to quality of Indonesian children’s socioemotional functioning. Dev. Psychol. 40, 790–804. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T., Diego, M., Hernandez-Reif, M., and Fernandez, M. (2007). Depressed mothers’ newborns show less discrimination of other newborns’ cry sounds. Infant Behav. Dev. 30, 431–435. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuentes, M. C., Garcia, O. F., Alcaide, M., Garcia-Ros, R., and Garcia, F. (2022). Analyzing when parental warmth but without parental strictness leads to more adolescent empathy and self-concept: Evidence from Spanish homes. Front. Psychol. 13:1060821. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1060821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ganiban, J. M., Liu, C., Zappaterra, L., An, S., and Leve, L. D. (2021). Gene × environment interactions in the development of preschool effortful control, and its implications for childhood externalizing behavior. Behav. Genet. 51, 448–462. doi: 10.1007/s10519-021-10073-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviors and experiences: a meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychol. Bull. 128, 539–579. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.128.4.539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gómez-Leal, R., Megías-Robles, A., Gutiérrez-Cobo, M. J., Cabello, R., and Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2022). Personal risk and protective factors involved in aggressive behavior. J. Interpers. Viol. 37, 1489–1515. doi: 10.1177/0886260520926322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gong, G. (2006). The relationship between parenting styles and anxiety of preschool children. Master’s thesis. Shandong: Shandong Normal University.

Google Scholar

Goulter, N., Mcmahon, R. J., Pasalich, D. S., and Dodge, K. A. (2020). Indirect effects of early parenting on adult antisocial outcomes via adolescent conduct disorder symptoms and callous-unemotional traits. J.Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 49, 930–942. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2019.1613999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hastings, P. D., and Derose, R. L. (2005). Links among gender, inhibition, and parental socialization in the development of prosocial behavior. Merrill-Palmer Q. 51, 467–493. doi: 10.2307/23096098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honk, J., Schutter, D. J., Bos, P. A., Kruijt, A-W., Lentjes, E. G., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). Testosterone administration impairs cognitive empathy in women depending on second-to-fourth digit ratio. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 3448–3452. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011891108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiajin, Y., Yu, W., EnXia, J., and Hong, Y. (2010). Gender differences in emotional processing and its neural mechanisms. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 18, 1899–1908.

Google Scholar

Jolliffe, D., and Farrington, D. P. (2004). Empathy and offending: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Aggress. Viol. Behav. 9, 441–476. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2003.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knafo, A., and Plomin, R. (2006). Prosocial behavior from early to middle childhood: genetic and environmental influences on stability and change. Dev. Psychol. 42, 771–786. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kostulski, M., Breuer, D., and Dpfner, M. (2021). Does parent management training reduce behavioural and emotional problems in children with intellectual disability? A randomised controlled trial. Res. Dev. Disabil. 114, 103958. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2021.103958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, C. B., Solmeyer, A. R., and Mchale, S. M. (2012). Sibling relationships and empathy across the transition to adolescence. J. Youth Adolesc. 41, 1657–1670. doi: 10.1007/s10964-012-9781-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemola, S., Schwarz, B., and Siffert, A. (2012). Interparental conflict and early adolescents’ aggression: is irregular sleep a vulnerability factor? J. Adolesc. 35, 97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S., and Raggatt, P. (2002). Foetal testosterone and eye contact in 12-month-old human infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 25, 327–335. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00094-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, Y., Chen, X., Nunez, A., Yan, M., and Zhao, F. (2020). Influences of parenting styles on adolescents’ empathy through the intervening effects of self-integrity and sense of coherence. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 116, 105246. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maccoby, E. E., and Martin, J. A. (1983). “Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child interaction,” in Handbook of Child Psychology Formerly Carmichaels Manual of Child Psychology, eds P. H. Mussen and E. M. Hetherington (New York, NY: Wiley).

Google Scholar

Masud, H., Shakil, A. M., Cho, K. W., and Fakhr, Z. (2019). Parenting styles and aggression among young adolescents: A systematic review of literature. Commun. Mental Health J. 55, 1015–1030. doi: 10.1007/s10597-019-00400-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, N., Johnston, C., and Pasalich, D. (2014). Parenting and conduct problems: moderation by child empathy. J. Psychopathol. Behav. 36, 74–83. doi: 10.1007/s10862-013-9366-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Movsesjan, A., Puzaitë, D., and Lazdauskas, T. (2014). 5–6 Year Old Children’s Empathy and Its Relation with Attention and Parenting Style. Ugdymo Psichol. 25, 5–17. doi: 10.15823/up.2014.01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, E., Konrath, S., Grühn, D., and Hagen, A. (2013). Empathic concern and perspective taking: linear and quadratic effects of age across the adult life span. J. Gerontol. 68B, 168–175. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, H., and Walton-Moss, B. (2012). Parenting style, parenting stress, and children’s health-related behaviors. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 33, 495–501.

Google Scholar

Peng, L. (2017). Investigation and Analysis of Cognitive Styles of Autistic Children and their Parents-Based on the Empathizing-systemizing Theory. Master’s thesis. Hangzhou: Zhejiang Normal University.

Google Scholar

Perez-Gramaje, A. F., Garcia, O. F., Reyes, M., Serra, E., and Garcia, F. (2020). Parenting styles and aggressive adolescents: Relationships with self-esteem and personal maladjustment. Eur. J. Psychol. Appl. Legal Context 12, 1–10. doi: 10.5093/ejpalc2020a1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perlman, C. M., and Hirdes, J. P. (2010). The aggressive behavior scale: A new scale to measure aggression based on the minimum data set. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 56, 2298–2303. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.02048.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinquart, M. (2017). Associations of parenting dimensions and styles with externalizing problems of children and adolescents: An updated meta-analysis. Dev. Psychol. 53, 873–932. doi: 10.1037/dev0000295.supp

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qi, W. (2019). Harsh parenting and child aggression: Child moral disengagement as the mediator and negative parental attribution as the moderator. Child Abuse Neglect. 91, 12–22. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reijntjes, A., Kamphuis, J. H., Prinzie, P., Boelen, P. A., Schoot, M., and Telch, M. J. (2011). Prospective linkages between peer victimization and externalizing problems in children: a meta-analysis. Aggress. Behav. 37, 215–222. doi: 10.1002/ab.20374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandy, O., Carolien, R., Evelien, B., Crone, E. A., and Berna, G. L. (2017). Assessing empathy across childhood and adolescence: Validation of the empathy questionnaire for children and adolescents (EmQue-CA). Front. Psychol. 8:870. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaffer, M., Clark, S., and Jeglic, E. L. (2009). The role of empathy and parenting style in the development of antisocial behaviors. Crime Delinq. 55, 586–599. doi: 10.1177/0011128708321359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaleh, M., and Binti, F. (2021). Empathy and criminal behaviour of aggressors at detention centre of Kendari, Indonesia. Psychol. Educ. 58, 918–923. doi: 10.17762/pae.v58i2.2027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thibodeau, E. L., Cicchetti, D., and Rogosch, F. A. (2015). Child maltreatment, impulsivity, and antisocial behavior in African American children: Moderation effects from a cumulative dopaminergic gene index. Dev. Psychopathol. 27, 1621–1636. doi: 10.1017/S095457941500098X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vachon, D. D., Lynam, D. R., and Johnson, J. A. (2014). The (non)relation between empathy and aggression: Surprising results from a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 140, 751–773. doi: 10.1037/a0035236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volbrecht, M. M., Lemery-Chalfant, K., Aksan, N., Zahn-Waxler, C., and Goldsmith, H. H. (2007). Examining the familial link between positive affect and empathy development in the second year. J. Genet. Psychol. 168, 105–130. doi: 10.3200/GNTP.168.2.105-130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wahl, K., and Metzner, C. (2012). Parental influences on the prevalence and development of child aggressiveness. J. Child. Fam. Stud. 21, 344–355. doi: 10.1007/s10826-011-9484-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wichstrøm, L., Belsky, J., and Steinsbekk, S. (2017). Homotypic and heterotypic continuity of symptoms of psychiatric disorders from age 4 to 10 years: a dynamic panel model. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 58, 1239–1247. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yajing, L., Zhansong, M., Chao, L., Zheng, X., and Tongying, C. (2019). Analysis of the relationship between family environment and behavioral problems in preschool children in Shunyi district. Chin. J. Child Health Care 27, 98–100. doi: 10.11852/zgetbjzz2018-1419

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yokota, K. (2017). The evolutionary roots of gender differences in aggressive behavior. Japan. Psychol. Rev. 60, 15–22. doi: 10.24602/sjpr.60.1_15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoo, H., Feng, X., and Day, R. D. (2013). Adolescents’ Empathy and Prosocial Behavior in the Family Context: A Longitudinal Study. J. Youth Adolesc. 42, 1858–1872. doi: 10.1007/s10964-012-9900-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, W., Cao, C., Wang, M., Ji, L., and Cao, Y. (2016). Monoamine Oxidase A (MAOA) and Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT) gene polymorphisms interact with maternal parenting in association with adolescent reactive aggression but not proactive aggression: evidence of differential susceptibility. J. Youth Adolesc. 45, 812–829. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0442-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, W., Ji, L., Gong, X., Zhang, Q., and Wang, Y. (2003). A longitudinal study on the development of 3- to 4-year-old children’s aggressive behavior. J.Psychol. Sci. 26, 49–52. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1671-6981.2003.01.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: parenting style, empathy, aggressive behavior, preschool children, authoritarian

Citation: Lin Z, Zhou Z, Zhu L and Wu W (2023) Parenting styles, empathy and aggressive behavior in preschool children: an examination of mediating mechanisms. Front. Psychol. 14:1243623. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1243623

Received: 21 June 2023; Accepted: 30 October 2023;
Published: 15 November 2023.

Edited by:

Bin-Bin Chen, Fudan University, China

Reviewed by:

Xinpei Xu, Shanghai Normal University, China
James A. Graham, The College of New Jersey, United States

Copyright © 2023 Lin, Zhou, Zhu and Wu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Weige Wu, mnshop@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.