Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 24 May 2024
Sec. Emotion Science

Valence-specific EEG microstate modulations during self-generated affective states

  • 1CINETic Center, Department of Research and Development, National University of Theatre and Film “I.L. Caragiale”, Bucharest, Romania
  • 2Faculty of Automatic Control and Computers, POLITEHNICA University of Bucharest, Bucharest, Romania
  • 3Department of Psychology, Faculty of Educational Sciences, University “Stefan cel Mare” of Suceava, Suceava, Romania

Introduction: This study aims to explore the temporal dynamics of brain networks involved in self-generated affective states, specifically focusing on modulating these states in both positive and negative valences. The overarching goal is to contribute to a deeper understanding of the neurodynamic patterns associated with affective regulation, potentially informing the development of biomarkers for therapeutic interventions in mood and anxiety disorders.

Methods: Utilizing EEG microstate analysis during self-generated affective states, we investigated the temporal dynamics of five distinct microstates across different conditions, including baseline resting state and self-generated states of positive valence (e.g., awe, contentment) and negative valence (e.g., anger, fear).

Results: The study revealed noteworthy modulations in microstate dynamics during affective states. Additionally, valence-specific mechanisms of spontaneous affective regulation were identified. Negative valence affective states were characterized by the heightened presence of attention-associated microstates and reduced occurrence of salience-related microstates during negative valence states. In contrast, positive valence affective states manifested a prevalence of microstates related to visual/autobiographical memory and a reduced presence of auditory/language-associated microstates compared to both baseline and negative valence states.

Discussion: This study contributes to the field by employing EEG microstate analysis to discern the temporal dynamics of brain networks involved in self-generated affective states. Insights from this research carry significant implications for understanding neurodynamic patterns in affective regulation. The identification of valence-specific modulations and mechanisms has potential applications in developing biomarkers for mood and anxiety disorders, offering novel avenues for therapeutic interventions.

Introduction

Emotional or affective states play an essential role in our well-being by modulating our thoughts, behaviors, and social interactions. Moreover, we spend a significant part of our daily life navigating spontaneously self-generate affective states by imagining past or future scenarios to adapt to challenging life contexts or enjoy rewarding experiences impacting decision-making and general well-being (Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2013; Ruby et al., 2013). Many studies focused on emotions elicited by external stimulations without personal context (e.g., audio, visual, audio-visual stimulation). However, substantial inter-individual variability in the neural, physiological, and behavioral results challenges these methods based on imposed categorical boundaries between emotions (Barrett, 2006; Lindquist et al., 2012). To overcome these limitations and accommodate the inter-individual variability from the subjective experience versus the normative categorization of stimuli, we can focus on self-generated affective states based on naturalistic depictions of real-life emotion-induction context scenarios (Wilson-Mendenhall et al., 2013).

Only a few studies investigated self-generated affective states. Using a method closer to the spontaneous mind-wandering affective elicitation, Onton and Makeig (2009) developed a study where participants self-induced several affective states following verbally guided narrative suggestions (Onton and Makeig, 2009). Using spatiotemporal decomposition of the EEG, authors reached small to high accuracy in emotional classification based on independent-component analysis (ICA) decomposition of EEG signal (Kothe et al., 2013; Hsu et al., 2022). However, the complex multidimensional ICA discriminated between self-generate affective states when compared to relaxation and only at the individual level (Kothe et al., 2013; Hsu et al., 2022). During the affective states, ICs in prefrontal, sensorimotor, premotor, and higher-level visual brain areas differentiated between affective and relaxed states (Kothe et al., 2013; Hsu et al., 2022). However, no difference was noted between negative and positive affective states despite clear differentiations in the fMRI literature pointing toward valence-specific modulations where the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), frontal pole, rostro-dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and supplementary motor area predicted negative valence states and regions of the reward circuit such as the midbrain, ventral striatum, and caudate nucleus activated more during the positive valence states (Colibazzi et al., 2010).

An alternative method to investigate valence-specific brain spatiotemporal dynamics during affective states, and, at the same time, taking advantage of the high temporal resolution and more naturalistic approach of the EEG, is by employing the EEG microstates analysis to the available dataset from Onton and Makeig (2009). Brain activity is inherently spontaneous and self-organizing, and this spontaneous neural activity forms the basis of our mental states, providing a window into understanding physiological variations and mental health conditions. One method to explore these phenomena is to analyze spontaneous EEG using the microstate framework. Microstate analysis assumes that spontaneous brain activity is organized into sub-second periods of large-scale oscillations, reflecting the brain’s mode of information integration (Michel and Koenig, 2018). In other words, the EEG microstate reveals the fast-changing temporal dynamics of resting state networks with high temporal resolution (Lehmann et al., 1987; Michel and Koenig, 2018). Notably, empirical studies increasingly demonstrate systematic links between variations in EEG microstates and fluctuations in mental states, supporting the utility of microstate analysis in probing brain dynamics and mental health (Khanna et al., 2015; Michel and Koenig, 2018; Chivu et al., 2023; Schiller et al., 2023; Tarailis et al., 2023). EEG microstate dynamics discriminate between different cognitive states like mental calculation, visualization, verbalization, and autobiographical memory, and socio-affective states and traits (Milz et al., 2016; Seitzman et al., 2017; Bréchet et al., 2019; Schiller et al., 2023; Tarailis et al., 2023). Moreover, EEG microstate temporal dynamics are differentiating between pathological brain states (Tomescu et al., 2014, 2015; Rieger et al., 2016; Michel and Koenig, 2018; Damborská et al., 2019; Chivu et al., 2023).

With a high degree of reproducibility, four (A-D) and seven (A-G) EEG microstates have been identified across many conditions and participants. Accumulating evidence suggests that EEG microstates represent the electrical fingerprints of resting-state networks; however, their one-to-one correspondence is still debated (Britz et al., 2010; Musso et al., 2010; Yuan et al., 2012; Custo et al., 2017; Michel and Koenig, 2018). Generally, A-B microstates are related to bottom-up visual and auditory/language-related. Microstate A initially recognized for its right frontal-to-left posterior pattern, is associated with auditory and visual processing, although its exact role remains unclear due to its interaction with arousal states (Milz et al., 2016; Seitzman et al., 2017; Michel and Koenig, 2018; Antonova et al., 2022; Tarailis et al., 2023). Microstate B consistently links to visual processing, including tasks involving self-related processes and scene imagery, with implications extending beyond visual stimuli and interacting with other microstates, notably microstate C (Milz et al., 2016; Seitzman et al., 2017; Michel and Koenig, 2018; Bréchet et al., 2019; Antonova et al., 2022; Tarailis et al., 2023). Microstates C, D, and E have been associated with core regions of top-down functional networks such as the default mode (DMN), the dorsal attention (DAN), and salience networks (SN) (Michel and Koenig, 2018). Microstate C is implicated in processing personally significant information and facilitating self-reflection, and Microstate D is intricately associated with higher-order cognitive functions like working memory and attention (Faber et al., 2017; Bréchet et al., 2019; Zappasodi et al., 2019; Vellante et al., 2020; Antonova et al., 2022; Tarailis et al., 2023). Microstate E is tied to interoceptive and emotional processing, indicating involvement in salience and emotional significance (Michel and Koenig, 2018; Schiller et al., 2019; Deolindo et al., 2021; Hu et al., 2021; Tarailis et al., 2023). Cognitive and socio-affective manipulation manipulations significantly mediate DMN, DAN, and SN-associated microstates (Schiller et al., 2023; Tarailis et al., 2023). Microstate F, with a left-lateralized maximum, potentially contributes to personally significant information processing and theory of mind (Custo et al., 2017; Bréchet et al., 2019; Tarailis et al., 2021, 2023). Lastly, microstate G, characterized by right-lateralized activity, may indicate physical well-being through its association with the somatosensory network (Custo et al., 2017; Damborská et al., 2019; Tarailis et al., 2023). Further research is needed to validate these interpretations and explore their broader implications, particularly as microstates F and G have only recently emerged in the literature.

Few studies investigated how temporal dynamics of EEG microstates advance our understanding of affective processing and regulation. With the goal of decoding emotional states, temporal structures of microstates were used to classify between arousal and valence with approximately 65% accuracy (Chen et al., 2021). Other studies show that C microstate coverage and the occurrence of microstate B were essential for recognizing discrete positive and negative emotions (Shen et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2023). Another study showed that microstate D displayed a negative association with valence (Shen et al., 2020). In a recent study utilizing a video-watching paradigm, the temporal dynamics of C and D microstates dissociate high versus low valence and arousal (Hu et al., 2023). While the C microstate showed a positive relation with arousal, microstate D occurred more often during negative valence videos (Hu et al., 2023).

Different methodological approaches might explain the contradictory results. For example, the high inter-individual variability of emotional reactivity to external stimuli might account for the lack of consistency. To overcome this challenge, we can focus on the neural mechanisms of self-generated affective states that naturally occur during our daily spontaneous mind-wandering. Moreover, self-generated affective state modulations based on naturalistic life scenarios might reveal unique microstate dynamics patterns reflecting spontaneous endogenous affective state regulation. With this aim, we investigated spatiotemporal microstate changes between baseline resting-state, positive (awe, compassion, contentment), and negative (anger, disgust, fear) affective self-generated states. Disentangling these brain dynamics might be essential in understanding basic affective and mood state regulation patterns that predict well-being.

Methods

Dataset description

In this study, we analyzed an EEG dataset collected and described in previous research by Onton and Makeig (2022). The study employed guided imagery to facilitate participants’ self-induction of several affective states. High-density EEG data was collected during the elicitation and maintenance of these affective states (Onton and Makeig, 2009).

At the beginning and end of the session, participants were instructed to rest for two minutes, summing up to four minutes of non-affective baseline recording. Then, participants received instructions on the button press to self-report their affective states, thus marking the beginning and the end of each affective state before transitioning to the next state. There were seven negative valences, undesirable target emotions (anger, disgust, fear, frustration, grief, jealousy, and sadness), and eight positive valences, pleasant target emotions (awe, compassion, contentment, excitement, happiness, joy, love, and relief), presented in an alternate pseudo-randomized order (Onton and Makeig, 2009; Onton and Makeig, 2022).

The EEG data were acquired from 250 scalp, four infraocular, and two electrocardiographic electrodes at a sampling rate of 256 Hz using the Biosemi AtiveTwo EEG system. Individual locations of the electrode positions in 3D (x, y, z) coordinates were provided for each participant. For more details, see Onton and Makeig (2009). Participants sat in a comfortable chair in a dimly-light, quiet room, received audio instruction, and listened to the narrative descriptions using earbuds. Participants were instructed to extend the duration of the affective state naturally. This resulted in experimental sessions that lasted roughly 80 min. The 480 recorded affective states (15 emotions x 32 subjects) ranged from 43 s to 12 min (on average, 218 ± 94 s) (Onton and Makeig, 2009, 2022).

Thirty-four participants (14 male, 19 female; age range: 18–38 years; age mean and standard deviation: 25.5 ± 5 years) volunteered for this experiment at the University of California, San Diego (UCSD). All participants gave informed consent, and the experimental procedures complied with the institutional requirements of UCSD (Onton and Makeig, 2009).

In the present article paradigm, the evoked affective state was considered felt only after the corresponding button was pressed. As no button press makers were identified for subject 33, he has been excluded from further analysis.

Data processing

The dataset included preprocessed data with excluded noisy channels of electrodes with poor skin contact and a 1-Hz high pass filter. We first continued pre-processing the data by applying a 40 Hz low-pass order 8 Butterworth filter. The free academic software Cartool, Matlab academic software, and EEGlab Matlab plugin were used for the EEG data processing. Next, the affective state epochs were exported for each subject using the marked beginning and end of each affective experience. These were further ordered and concatenated into three categories: baseline, positive affective, and negative affective states.

The preprocessed data were subjected to the Infomax independent component analysis (ICA) (Jung et al., 2000), and based on visual inspection of the topography and amplitude of the components, we eliminated cardiac and eye movement artifacts. ICA was carried out on the entire dataset, including data from the marginal electrodes, allowing for more precise identification of eye, pulse, and muscular tension components.

After excluding noisy channels in the original database preprocessing, the retained electrode numbers varied between 134 and 235. To unify the space across subjects while reducing the computational load of further investigations, we reduced the number of electrodes to the standard 128 channels (Biosemi). First, the ECG and ocular electrodes were identified and eliminated from the analysis. Then, we interpolated the EEG tracks from the individual electrode 3D space maps by selecting the front, top, back, left, and right landmarks (H22, B12, C14, A31, D20) to the 128 Biosemi 3D common standard coordinate system for statistical analysis across subjects (FpZ, Cz, Oz, T7, and T8). The Cartool interpolation tool, using a 3D spline interpolation that accounts for the actual geometry of the head, was used for interpolation (Brunet et al., 2011). Finally, before microstate analysis, the data were downsampled to 128 Hz and re-referenced to the average reference for further analysis.

Microstate analysis

Microstate analysis mainly consists of two stages: first, the clustering of EEG data to find the most representative template maps, which correspond to the different microstates, and second, fitting them back to the EEG data to quantify their temporal parameters. The free academic software Cartool and Matlab were used for the microstate analysis.

The EEG topographies surrounding the local maxima of the Global Field Power (GFP) exhibit the highest signal-to-noise ratio (Murray et al., 2008). Topographies corresponding to GFP peaks were submitted to a modified k-means cluster analysis to identify the most representative classes of stable topographies. GFP represents the global pattern of brain activity and is defined as the standard deviation across electrodes (Murray et al., 2008).

The K-means clustering was carried out in two stages, first at the individual level and then at the group level, by clustering each individual dominant topography separately for each condition: baseline, negative affect, and positive affect. To establish the optimal cluster number, Cartool computes seven maximally independent criteria and then combines them using the median as an optimal estimation. The criteria encompass Cross-Validation based on the predictive residual variance, Gamma (concordant vs. discordant cluster pairs), Davies and Bouldin (ratio of within to between cluster differentiation), Silhouettes (consistency & cluster goodness of fit), Krzanowski-Lai Index, Point-biserial, and Dunn, an evaluation of how all clusters are well separated (Custo et al., 2017; Bréchet et al., 2019).

We determined that five microstates can optimally describe the group topographical variability (baseline 87.1%, negative affect 87.2%, positive affect 86.6% of explained variance). To compare the temporal dynamics between conditions, we labeled them A, B, C, D, and E in line with the topographies reported in the literature (Michel and Koenig, 2018). To statistically confirm the similarity of microstates between conditions, we computed the spatial correlation between the topographies of the tree conditions baseline, negative, and positive affect. Figure 1 shows the correlation coefficient of the between-condition spatial correlation analysis for each microstate topography. The results show the highest correlation coefficient for the microstates labeled with the same letter between conditions, ensuring we compared the same states between conditions.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Left: Microstate topographies identified for each condition (top to bottom: baseline, negative affect, and positive affect conditions). Right: Microstate correlation coefficient matrixes used for labeling and microstate ordering between conditions.

Then, each subject’s recorded brain electrical activity is modeled as a time sequence of microstates. After identifying microstate topographies for each individual and then across individuals for each condition, the second group clustering at the condition level resulted in a set of five microstates that were used in the fitting step for each condition separately (Figure 1). During the fitting process of the microstates, the entire EEG of the participants was used, excluding only the marked artifact epochs. A temporal smoother with the following settings was applied: Besag factor of 10, window half-size 3 (24 ms). The brief time intervals of microstate presence with less than 23.4 ms that might occur during low signal-to-noise ratios were redistributed to the previous or next microstate as a function of the highest spatial correlation. Each time point of the individual data was assigned to the microstate cluster with which it correlated best to measure the temporal parameters of microstates. Short periods of noise in the data were eliminated using a 0.7 correlation coefficient threshold. The analysis excluded these periods (Brunet et al., 2011). For each affective state condition (positive or negative) and baseline, we estimated the occurrence and duration of each microstate in each individual. The average uninterrupted time (in ms) that a specific microstate map was present, or the time the subject remained in that state, is called mean duration (ms). A microstate’s occurrence (Hz) reveals how frequently a specific microstate occurs every second.

The experimental sessions lasted roughly 80 min. The 480 recorded affective states (15 emotions × 32 subjects) ranged from 43 s to 12 min (on average, 218 ± 94 s) (Onton and Makeig, 2009, 2022). The data collection in the study had a baseline period of 4 min recorded at the beginning of the session, leaving the self-affect generated states with a duration between two and three times larger than the baseline (4 vs. 12 min) that might bias the duration and occurrence of the microstate’s parameters. Although the microstate parameters are computed at the individual-level average, we still believe the total length of the condition can confound these averages. Moreover, individual-generated affect states varied in size between participants. Thus, to control for inter-individual variability in the maintenance of affective states and differences that might arise from the total duration of affective states included in the dataset compared with the baseline, we selected affects with similar duration between participants. For this reason, for negative and positive affect conditions, only four minutes of the self-generated recordings were included in the statistical analysis. More specifically, we included highly arousing negative emotions such as anger, disgust, and fear and positive emotions like awe, compassion, and contentment. As one participant (27) did not auto-evaluate himself as attaining the compassion emotion (by not pressing the corresponding button), we included the next positive emotion, excitement.

The temporal dynamic analysis is based on the temporal properties of each microstate relative to the affective state, including their occurrence (Hz) and mean duration (ms) (Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of mean duration (ms) and occurrence (Hz) of microstates during baseline, positive, and negative affective states.

Statistical analysis

As mean duration (ms) and occurrence (Hz) of microstates parameters are not normally distributed, we used Wilcoxon signed-rank two-tailed tests to examine how the self-generated affective state [active affective, i.e., negative (NEG), positive (POS), or baseline (BAS)] impact temporal properties of microstates. We applied false discovery rate (FDR) Benjamini Hochberg correction for multiple comparisons with p < 0.05 significance threshold (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).

We employed the biserial rank coefficient (rb) as a measure of effect size for the Wilcoxon signed-rank two-tailed tests (Kerby, 2014). The rb values of 0.10, 0.24 and 0.37 are considered limit statistical (small), fair (medium) and evident (high) statistical effects (Fritz et al., 2011).

Results

Results for microstates mean duration (ms)

We found significant differences in the mean duration of microstate B (BAS: M = 65.95, SD = 4.85, NEG: M = 67.34, SD = 5.74, POS: M = 68.93, SD = 6.88), which increased significantly during both POS (W = 69, Z = 3.77, p = 0.0007, rb = 0.75) and NEG (W = 151, Z = 2.31, p = 0.02, rb = 0.46) affective states vs. baseline (Figure 2; Table 2). In addition, the microstate’s B mean duration also proved significantly higher during the POS than the NEG (W = 149, Z = 2.34, p = 0.03, rb = 0.46). During NEG (W = 138, Z = 2.54, p = 0.01, rb = 0.50) and POS (W = 105, Z = 3.13, p = 0.004, rb = 0.62) microstates, the C mean duration (BAS: M = 101.12, SD = 24.41, NEG: M = 97.11, SD = 21.30, POS: M = 96.50, SD = 23.51) decreased significantly compared to BAS but did not show significant differences between the NEG and POS (p = 0.5) conditions. D (BAS: M = 63.82, SD = 5.32, NEG: M = 66.49, SD = 5.89, POS: M = 64.24, SD = 5.72) microstates were significantly longer in NEG when compared to BAS (W = 89, Z = 3.42, p = 0.001, rb = 0.68) and POS (W = 59, Z = 3.95, p = 0.0003, rb = 0.78), while E microstates (BAS: M = 68.26, SD = 7.05, NEG: M = 64.71, SD = 7.19, POS: M = 66.82, SD = 7.78) significantly lasted for a shorter amount of time in NEG compared to BAS (W = 41, Z = 4.27, p = 0.00009, rb = 0.85) and POS (W = 100, Z = 3.22, p = 0.0031, rb = 0.64) (Figure 2; Table 2). In addition, we found a trend for microstate A (BAS: M = 66.25, SD = 6.39, NEG: M = 65.96, SD = 5.95, POS: M = 65.03, SD = 5.64) decreased duration in POS compared to BAS (W = 170, Z = 1.97, p = 0.08, rb = 0.39).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Results for microstates mean duration (ms). Group comparison of the distribution and the median of the mean duration (ms) of each identified microstate fitted for BAS, NEG, and POS. Intercategorically connected black points represent intra-individual data; the median is represented in white. The significant differences are marked with *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results for microstates mean duration (ms).

Results for microstates occurrence (Hz)

Microstate A (BAS: M = 1.68, SD = 0.67, NEG: M = 1.73, SD = 0.71, POS: M = 1.55, SD = 0.66) occurs much less frequently in POS than both BAS (W = 113, Z = 2.99, p = 0.004, rb = 0.59) and NEG (W = 79, Z = 3.60, p = 0.0005, rb = 0.71) while the microstate B (BAS: M = 1.72, SD = 0.69, NEG: M = 1.77, SD = 0.68, POS: M = 1.97, SD = 0.71) occurrence rate is significantly higher in POS than in BAS (W = 22, Z = 4.61, p = 0.00001, rb = 0.92) or NEG (W = 36, Z = 4.36, p = 0.00006, rb = 0.87). There are no significant differences in microstate C occurrence rates for either category (BAS vs. NEG p = 0.21; BAS vs. POS p = 0.54, NEG vs. POS p = 0.23). In the case of microstate D (BAS: M = 1.42, SD = 0.63, NEG: M = 1.76, SD = 0.63, POS: M = 1.63, SD = 0.66), the occurrence rate is significantly higher in POS compared to BAS (W = 61, Z = 3.92, p = 0.0002, rb = 0.78), NEG compared to BAS (W = 20, Z = 4.65, p = 0.00001, rb = 0.92), and NEG compared to POS (W = 115, Z = 2.95, p = 0.003, rb = 0.59). Microstate E (BAS: M = 1.88, SD = 0.79, NEG: M = 1.68, SD = 0.82, POS: M = 1.88, SD = 0.88) is significantly lower in NEG than BAS (W = 28, Z = 4.51, p = 0.00001, rb = 0.90) and POS (W = 64, Z = 3.86, p = 0.0002, rb = 0.77) (Figure 3). The statistical results of the temporal dynamic analysis are presented in detail in Table 3.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Results for microstate occurrence (Hz). Group comparison of the distribution and the median of the occurrence (Hz) of each identified microstate fitted for BAS, NEG, and POS. Intercategorically connected black points represent intra-individual data; the median is represented in white. The significant differences are marked with *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results for microstate occurrence (Hz).

Discussion

This study investigated the temporal dynamics of brain networks during positive and negative valence self-generated states to expand the knowledge of spontaneous socio-affective states. Utilizing EEG microstate analysis, five distinct microstates were examined during baseline resting state and negative and positive affect self-generated states. Results showed significant modulations in microstate dynamics during affective states, with valence-specific mechanisms that contribute to understanding the neurodynamic patterns in affective regulation and may inform the development of biomarkers for mood and anxiety disorders, offering new avenues for therapeutic interventions.

Summary of findings

Self-generated affective states significantly change the resting pattern of spontaneous microstates with small to medium effect sizes that reflect general affect and valence-specific mechanisms of spontaneous affective regulation. The valence-specific modulation points toward a mechanism by which self-generated positive valence affective states are characterized by more prevalent B and les present A microstates compared to baseline and negative valence affective states. Overall, affective states lead to the up-regulation of microstates B and D dynamics and the down-regulation of microstates C relative to baseline. Negative valence self-generated affective states specifically modulate the increased presence of D microstates and decreased occurrence of E microstates compared to baseline and positive valence affective states. Results are further discussed in the context of the microstate’s functional meaning and association with previous findings of the socio-affective literature.

Valence-specific modulations of A and B microstates

Microstate A and B are most often associated with bottom-up networks, auditory-language related and visual activity in the temporal and cortex, left–right cuneus, inferior, and middle occipital gyrus (Britz et al., 2010; Custo et al., 2017; Michel and Koenig, 2018; Tarailis et al., 2023). These microstates tend to increase in presence with more engagement during visualization, verbalization, and autobiographical memory tasks. However, their initial one-to-one functional separation into A verbal-auditory and B visual-related microstates is more complicated than initially reported; see Tarailis et al. (2023) for more details (Tarailis et al., 2023).

Regarding microstate A, more convergent literature points toward an association with arousal and alertness (Tarailis et al., 2023). For example, Antonova et al. (2022) found a positive correlation between the mean duration of microstate A and subjective levels of alertness (Antonova et al., 2022). At the same time, other researchers found positive associations between microstate A and prosocial behavior (Schiller et al., 2020). We previously found an increased microstate A presence after both Social Imitation and the control activity of a self-guided arm movement task (Tomescu et al., 2022). Additionally, after the social imitation task, results varied as a function of extraversion (Tomescu et al., 2022). Following this line of associations, compared to baseline or negative valence affective states, the decreased microstate A presence during positive self-generated affective states might also suggest a more relaxed, less alert arousal during a positive affective resting state. Moreover, the microstate A decreased presence results might be specific for the self-generated type of positive affect states as not observed after emotion-inducing videos (Hu et al., 2023).

Microstate B increased during both self-generated positive valence affective experiences and, as previously reported, after emotion-inducing videos (Hu et al., 2023). Additionally, we observed an increased presence of microstate B during negative affective states. As the functional relevance of microstate B has been related to autobiographical memory and scene visualization (Tarailis et al., 2023), the results here might suggest a reflection of the more engagement of these strategies during the self-generated positive affective states. However, significant negative correlations have been found in mood disorder patients with depression scores (Atluri et al., 2018; Yan et al., 2021). Recently, we conducted a meta-analysis on clinical studies suggesting the increased B microstate in patients might reflect a compensatory mechanism as larger effect sizes were observed in unmedicated mood disorder patients (Chivu et al., 2023). We suggested that mood and anxiety disorder patients might engage too often in visually related past experiences, such as ruminative thought patterns, which fail to compensate for the mood and anxiety symptoms and negatively impact mental health (Chivu et al., 2023). Indeed, microstate B presence was positively associated with self-related thoughts about self-behavior and feelings (Zanesco et al., 2020). However, the results here might suggest that patients could also engage in past experiences of positive valence to compensate for depressive mood.

Affective states modulations of C microstates

The results align with brain sources of microstates C, pointing toward association with task-negative posterior DMN core regions like the posterior cingulate cortex, precuneus, and left angular gyrus (Custo et al., 2017). Based on the functional relevance of microstate C and previous findings of its temporal reduction during states of cognitive or behavioral manipulations (Tarailis et al., 2023), the decrease of C microstates during self-generated affective states might reflect a general re-organization pattern of mind-wandering and a possible more aroused, goal-oriented pattern of thought.

Microstate C increases during states of relaxation and correlates with more profound states of mind-wandering (less thought discontinuity, less verbal thought about self) after a Social Imitation task (Tomescu et al., 2022). Indeed, we previously found a negative association between C microstates and self-oriented, verbal, and discontinuous patterns of thoughts associated with decreased self-reported levels of stress (Tomescu et al., 2022). In addition, Pan et al. (2020) also found a negative association between microstate C and rumination level. The authors argue that microstate C might be a reliable index of ruminations (Pan et al., 2020). Ruminations are negative valence impulsive patterns of thoughts and an essential mind-wandering transdiagnostic factor in all mood disorders (McLaughlin and Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Pan et al., 2020). In our data, we observed a decreased C microstates mean duration for positive and negative valence affective states, suggesting an association with more aroused resting states compared to baseline.

Additionally, our results align with Hu et al., showing decreased microstate C after emotional audio-visual tasks without a main effect of valence (Hu et al., 2023). In parallel to a reduced presence of C microstates, they also found an increased presence of B and D microstates (Hu et al., 2023). We confirm their findings, showing that during sef-generated positive and negative valence affective states, there is an increase in the presence of B and D microstates. Moreover, we extend their findings, showing a decreased presence of A and E microstates and valence-specific microstates modulations.

Valence-specific affect modulation of D microstates

Microstate D is among the four canonical microstates. Studies investigating cognitive state modulations on temporal dynamics of microstates support the view that microstate D is associated with the dorsal attention network involving allocation and maintenance of attentional resources (Britz et al., 2010; Custo et al., 2017; Michel and Koenig, 2018; Tarailis et al., 2023). For example, studies report that microstate D is more present when participants are asked to perform demanding cognitive tasks, such as mental serial subtraction tasks based on focused states of attention (Seitzman et al., 2017; Bréchet et al., 2019). More importantly, microstate D is less present in socially induced spontaneous relaxed states (Tomescu et al., 2022) and shows reduced presence with altered states of attention, consciousness, and lack of cognitive control, such as during auditory-verbal hallucinations in SZ patients (Kindler et al., 2011), deep hypnosis (Katayama et al., 2007), sleep, and dreaming (Brodbeck et al., 2012; Bréchet et al., 2020).

Temporal dynamics of microstate D are consistently altered in mood and anxiety patients, showing significant negative associations with depressive symptomatology (Murphy et al., 2020; Chivu et al., 2023). Given our results here, the increased D microstates during negative valence self-generated affective states support the view that mood disorders might arise from a failure to down-regulate negative emotions. In addition, microstate D quantifiers positively correlate with alertness and reaction time scores in a non-clinical population (Zanesco et al., 2020). Thus, microstate D increase might be related to attention and cognitive control neural resources during emotional attention-demanding tasks.

Our results align with previous reports of D modulations after emotional-inducing stimuli (Hu et al., 2023). Moreover, based on the association of D microstates with the dorsolateral attention network (DAN), our results are in line with previous ICA-derived prefrontal activation during affective state modulations on the same dataset when compared to relaxed states (Kothe et al., 2013; Hsu et al., 2022). Additionally, we extend these observations by showing that during negative valence affective states, we see a significantly increased presence when compared to the positive affective states, which might reflect the attentional negative valence bias reported in the literature, where negative valence stimuli attract more attentional resources (Baumeister et al., 2001).

These results are also in line with fMRI studies on self-generated emotional states showing that unpleasant emotions induced greater activation in a set of regions that included the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, frontal pole, mid-rostral-dorsal ACC, and supplementary motor area (Colibazzi et al., 2010). These activations might subserve functions like attention allocation, executive functioning, goal-oriented behavior, and emotional regulation during responses to threat-related stimuli (Colibazzi et al., 2010). One study investigated stress-related modulation of EEG microstates and found an increased presence of D microstate and, more significantly, an increased transition between D and salience network-related E microstates (Hu et al., 2023). Moreover, these patterns of increased transitions negatively correlated with salivary cortisol (Hu et al., 2023), further suggesting a possible important role of salience-related and D microstates during negative valence affective states and emotional regulation.

Valence-specific affect modulation of E microstates

Although both microstate C and E microstates were previously associated with salience processing when more than five microstates are represented in the data, microstate E is more associated with the task-positive salience resting-state network with core regions in the superior frontal gyrus, bilateral middle prefrontal cortices ACC and insular cortices (Britz et al., 2010; Custo et al., 2017; Michel and Koenig, 2018; Tarailis et al., 2023). Functionally, microstate E was previously related to the processing of interoceptive and emotional information with increased presence during negative valence effective states, such as after stress exposure, and with increased cognitive load tasks (Hu et al., 2023; Tarailis et al., 2023). Thus, we expected our results to show an increased presence during self-generated affective states. However, these surprising results might be related to the unpredictability nature of the stress exposure and efforts to down-regulate the autonomic system and the nature of the high level of vigilance after stress exposure. Altered E microstates were noted in post-traumatic stress disorder patients, further supporting the association of E microstates with negative valence affect and anxiety (Terpou et al., 2022; Chivu et al., 2023). Terpou et al. (2022) proposed that the brain regions functionally related to the salience network and decreased E microstates might reflect a failure to map relevant bottom-up stimuli, resulting in a hypervigilance state in patients suffering from anxiety-related disorders like PTSD (Terpou et al., 2022). Following the same line of thought, our decreased E microstate during negative valence affective states might be specifically associated with the integrated nature of self-generate negative valence affective states that do not require active salience processing and autonomic activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPA) for adaptation to stressful contexts. However, more studies are needed to sustain this interpretation and association with successful emotional regulation.

Limitations and conclusions

By examining EEG microstates in self-induced affective states, we show valence-specific microstate modulation that extends previous and fast-growing socio-emotional microstate literature. Limitations should be considered, for example, the relatively small number of participants, the small baseline sample, and the lack of information on the qualitative experience and strategies of the self-induced affective state. More studies are needed to see how these modulations are influenced by inter-individual emotional regulation traits, clinical symptomatology, and socio-emotional context to sustain general well-being and mental health. However, our findings already provide valuable insights into the neural aspects of emotional regulation and their potential implications for therapeutic interventions in emotional disorders.

Data availability statement

This study analysed publicly available EEG datasets collected and described in previous research by Onton and Makeig (2022).The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. All participants volunteered for this experiment, gave written informed consent, and the experimental procedures complied with the institutional ethical requirements of the University of California, San Diego (Onton and Makeig, 2009).

Author contributions

KN: Data curation, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing – original draft. MT: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was financed by the Romanian Executive Unit for Higher Education Financing (UEFISCDI) TE126/2022 grant via PN-III-P1-1.1-TE-2021 to MT, registration number UNATC 2178/03.06.2022 and UEFISCDI 1764/06.06.2022. This funding was allocated to the project “Neurophysiological markers of resilience in common mental health disorders” (NEURESIL, neuresil.ro) via national competition. The corresponding author MT was supported via a return home fellowship awarded by the International Brain Research Organization (IBRO). Additionally, this article was supported via a fellowship to KN awarded by the Ministry of Investments and European Projects through the Human Capital Sectoral Operational Program 2014–2020, Contract no. 62461/03.06.2022, SMIS code 153735.

Acknowledgments

We thank the participants of the study and the authors of Onton and Makeig (2009) for initiating, collecting, and making the dataset available (Onton and Makeig, 2009). We extend our gratitude to volunteer students who supported this research during their lab internship, Octavian F. Mirică and Cosmina A. Duțică, and researchers Alexandra Sofonea and Alina A. Chivu.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Andrews-Hanna, J., Kaiser, R., Turner, A., Reineberg, A., Godinez, D., Dimidjian, S., et al. (2013). A penny for your thoughts: dimensions of self-generated thought content and relationships with individual differences in emotional wellbeing. Front. Psychol. 4:900. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00900

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonova, E., Holding, M., Suen, H. C., Sumich, A., Maex, R., and Nehaniv, C. (2022). EEG microstates: functional significance and short-term test-retest reliability. Neuroimage 2:100089. doi: 10.1016/j.ynirp.2022.100089

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Atluri, S., Wong, W., Moreno, S., Blumberger, D. M., Daskalakis, Z. J., and Farzan, F. (2018). Selective modulation of brain network dynamics by seizure therapy in treatment-resistant depression. NeuroImage. Clinical 20, 1176–1190. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.10.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 1, 28–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00003.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., and Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 5, 323–370. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological) 57, 289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bréchet, L., Brunet, D., Birot, G., Gruetter, R., Michel, C. M., and Jorge, J. (2019). Capturing the spatiotemporal dynamics of self-generated, task-initiated thoughts with EEG and fMRI. NeuroImage 194, 82–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bréchet, L., Brunet, D., Perogamvros, L., Tononi, G., and Michel, C. M. (2020). EEG microstates of dreams. Sci. Rep. 10:17069. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-74075-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Britz, J., Van De Ville, D., and Michel, C. M. (2010). BOLD correlates of EEG topography reveal rapid resting-state network dynamics. NeuroImage 52, 1162–1170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.052

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brodbeck, V., Kuhn, A., von Wegner, F., Morzelewski, A., Tagliazucchi, E., Borisov, S., et al. (2012). EEG microstates of wakefulness and NREM sleep. NeuroImage 62, 2129–2139. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.060

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunet, D., Murray, M. M., and Michel, C. M. (2011). Spatiotemporal analysis of multichannel EEG: CARTOOL. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011:813870. doi: 10.1155/2011/813870

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J., Li, H., Ma, L., Bo, H., Soong, F., and Shi, Y. (2021). Dual-threshold-based microstate analysis on characterizing temporal dynamics of affective process and emotion recognition from EEG signals. Front. Neurosci. 15:689791. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.689791

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chivu, A. C., Pascal, S., Damborská, A., and Tomescu, M. (2023). EEG microstates in mood and anxiety disorders: a Meta-analysis. Brain Topogr. 37, 357–368. doi: 10.1007/s10548-023-00999-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Colibazzi, T., Posner, J., Wang, Z., Gorman, D., Gerber, A., Yu, S., et al. (2010). Neural systems subserving valence and arousal during the experience of induced emotions. Emotion 10, 377–389. doi: 10.1037/a0018484

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Custo, A., Van De Ville, D., Wells, W. M., Tomescu, M. I., Brunet, D., and Michel, C. M. (2017). Electroencephalographic resting-state networks: source localization of microstates. Brain Connect. 7, 671–682. doi: 10.1089/brain.2016.0476

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Damborská, A., Tomescu, M. I., Honzírková, E., Barteček, R., Hořínková, J., Fedorová, S., et al. (2019). EEG resting-state large-scale brain network dynamics are related to depressive symptoms. Front. Psych. 10:548. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00548

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deolindo, C. S., Ribeiro, M. W., de Aratanha, M. A. A., Scarpari, J. R. S., Forster, C. H. Q., da Silva, R. G. A., et al. (2021). Microstates in complex and dynamical environments: unraveling situational awareness in critical helicopter landing maneuvers. Hum. Brain Mapp. 42, 3168–3181. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25426

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Faber, P. L., Travis, F., Milz, P., and Parim, N. (2017). EEG microstates during different phases of transcendental meditation practice. Cogn. Process. 18, 307–314. doi: 10.1007/s10339-017-0812-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritz, C., Morris, P., and Richler, J. (2011). Effect size estimates: current use, calculations, and interpretation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 2–18. doi: 10.1037/a0024338

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, S.-H., Lin, Y., Onton, J., Jung, T.-P., and Makeig, S. (2022). Unsupervised learning of brain state dynamics during emotion imagination using high-density EEG. NeuroImage 249:118873. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118873

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, N., Long, Q., Li, Q., Hu, X., Li, Y., Zhang, S., et al. (2021). The modulation of salience and central executive networks by acute stress in healthy males: an EEG microstates study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 169, 63–70. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.09.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, W., Zhang, Z., Zhao, H., Zhang, L., Li, L., Huang, G., et al. (2023). EEG microstate correlates of emotion dynamics and stimulation content during video watching. Cereb. Cortex 33, 523–542. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac082

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, T.-P., Makeig, S., Westerfield, M., Townsend, J., Courchesne, E., and Sejnowski, T. J. (2000). Removal of eye activity artifacts from visual event-related potentials in normal and clinical subjects. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 1745–1758. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00386-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Katayama, H., Gianotti, L. R. R., Isotani, T., Faber, P. L., Sasada, K., Kinoshita, T., et al. (2007). Classes of multichannel EEG microstates in light and deep hypnotic conditions. Brain Topogr. 20, 7–14. doi: 10.1007/s10548-007-0024-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerby, D. S. (2014). The simple difference formula: an approach to teaching nonparametric correlation. Comprehen. Psychol. 3:3. doi: 10.2466/11.IT.3.1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khanna, A., Pascual-Leone, A., Michel, C. M., and Farzan, F. (2015). Microstates in resting-state EEG: current status and future directions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 49, 105–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.12.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Killingsworth, M. A., and Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science 330:932. doi: 10.1126/science.1192439

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kindler, J., Hubl, D., Strik, W. K., Dierks, T., and Koenig, T. (2011). Resting-state EEG in schizophrenia: auditory verbal hallucinations are related to shortening of specific microstates. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 1179–1182. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.10.042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kothe, C. A., Makeig, S., and Onton, J. A. (2013). Emotion recognition from EEG during self-paced emotional imagery. Humaine Assoc. Conf. Affect. Comput. Intell. Interact. 2013, 855–858. doi: 10.1109/ACII.2013.160

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmann, D., Ozaki, H., and Pal, I. (1987). EEG alpha map series: brain micro-states by space-oriented adaptive segmentation. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 67, 271–288. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(87)90025-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindquist, K., Wager, T., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., and Barrett, L. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Behav. Brain Sci. 35, 121–143. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X11000446

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Hu, X., Shen, X., Lv, Z., Song, S., and Zhang, D. (2023). The EEG microstate representation of discrete emotions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 186, 33–41. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2023.02.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McLaughlin, K. A., and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Rumination as a transdiagnostic factor in depression and anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 49, 186–193. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.12.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Michel, C. M., and Koenig, T. (2018). EEG microstates as a tool for studying the temporal dynamics of whole-brain neuronal networks: a review. NeuroImage 180, 577–593. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.062

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Milz, P., Faber, P. L., Lehmann, D., Koenig, T., Kochi, K., and Pascual-Marqui, R. D. (2016). The functional significance of EEG microstates—associations with modalities of thinking. NeuroImage 125, 643–656. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, M., Whitton, A. E., Deccy, S., Ironside, M. L., Rutherford, A., Beltzer, M., et al. (2020). Abnormalities in electroencephalographic microstates are state and trait markers of major depressive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 45, 2030–2037. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-0749-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, M. M., Brunet, D., and Michel, C. M. (2008). Topographic ERP analyses: a step-by-step tutorial review. Brain Topogr. 20, 249–264. doi: 10.1007/s10548-008-0054-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Musso, F., Brinkmeyer, J., Mobascher, A., Warbrick, T., and Winterer, G. (2010). Spontaneous brain activity and EEG microstates. A novel EEG/fMRI analysis approach to explore resting-state networks. NeuroImage 52, 1149–1161. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.093

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Onton, J., and Makeig, S. (2009). High-frequency broadband modulation of electroencephalographic spectra. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 3:61. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.061.2009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Onton, J., and Makeig, S. (2022). Imagined emotion study [dataset]. OpenNeuro.

Google Scholar

Pan, D., Hoid, D., Gu, R., and Li, X. (2020). Emotional working memory training reduces rumination and alters the EEG microstate in anxious individuals. NeuroImage 28:102488. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102488

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rieger, K., Diaz Hernandez, L., Baenninger, A., and Koenig, T. (2016). 15 years of microstate research in schizophrenia – where are we? A Meta-analysis. Front. Psych. 7:22. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruby, F. J. M., Smallwood, J., Engen, H., and Singer, T. (2013). How self-generated thought shapes mood—the relation between mind-wandering and mood depends on the socio-temporal content of thoughts. PLoS One 8:e77554. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077554

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiller, B., Kleinert, T., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Klauer, K. C., and Heinrichs, M. (2020). Temporal dynamics of resting EEG networks are associated with prosociality. Sci. Rep. 10:13066. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-69999-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiller, B., Koenig, T., and Heinrichs, M. (2019). Oxytocin modulates the temporal dynamics of resting EEG networks. Sci. Rep. 9:13418. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-49636-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiller, B., Sperl, M. F. J., Kleinert, T., Nash, K., and Gianotti, L. R. R. (2023). EEG microstates in social and affective neuroscience. Brain Topogr. doi: 10.1007/s10548-023-00987-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seitzman, B. A., Abell, M., Bartley, S. C., Erickson, M. A., Bolbecker, A. R., and Hetrick, W. P. (2017). Cognitive manipulation of brain electric microstates. NeuroImage 146, 533–543. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, X., Hu, X., Liu, S., Song, S., and Zhang, D. (2020). Exploring EEG microstates for affective computing: decoding valence and arousal experiences during video watching. 42nd Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine & Biology Society (EMBC), 841–846.

Google Scholar

Tarailis, P., Koenig, T., Michel, C. M., and Griškova-Bulanova, I. (2023). The functional aspects of resting EEG microstates: a systematic review. Brain Topogr. 37, 181–217. doi: 10.1007/s10548-023-00958-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarailis, P., Šimkutė, D., Koenig, T., and Griškova-Bulanova, I. (2021). Relationship between spatiotemporal dynamics of the brain at rest and self-reported spontaneous thoughts: an EEG microstate approach. J. Personalized Med 11:216. doi: 10.3390/jpm11111216

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Terpou, B. A., Shaw, S. B., Théberge, J., Férat, V., Michel, C. M., McKinnon, M. C., et al. (2022). Spectral decomposition of EEG microstates in post-traumatic stress disorder. NeuroImage 35:103135. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2022.103135

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomescu, M. I., Papasteri, C. C., Sofonea, A., Boldasu, R., Kebets, V., Pistol, C. A. D., et al. (2022). Spontaneous thought and microstate activity modulation by social imitation. NeuroImage 249:118878. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118878

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomescu, M., Rihs, T., Becker, R., Britz, J., Custo, A., Grouiller, F., et al. (2014). Deviant dynamics of EEG resting state pattern in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome adolescents: a vulnerability marker of schizophrenia? Schizophr. Res. 157, 175–181. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.05.036

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomescu, M. I., Rihs, T. A., Roinishvili, M., Karahanoglu, F. I., Schneider, M., Menghetti, S., et al. (2015). Schizophrenia patients and 22q11.2 deletion syndrome adolescents at risk express the same deviant patterns of resting state EEG microstates: a candidate endophenotype of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Res. 2, 159–165. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2015.04.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vellante, F., Ferri, F., Baroni, G., Croce, P., Migliorati, D., Pettoruso, M., et al. (2020). Euthymic bipolar disorder patients and EEG microstates: a neural signature of their abnormal self experience? J. Affect. Disord. 272, 326–334. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.03.175

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F., and Barsalou, L. W. (2013). Neural evidence that human emotions share Core affective properties. Psychol. Sci. 24, 947–956. doi: 10.1177/0956797612464242

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, D., Liu, J., Liao, M., Liu, B., Wu, S., Li, X., et al. (2021). Prediction of clinical outcomes with EEG microstate in patients with major depressive disorder. Front. Psych. 12:695272. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.695272

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, H., Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Drevets, W. C., and Bodurka, J. (2012). Spatiotemporal dynamics of the brain at rest—exploring EEG microstates as electrophysiological signatures of BOLD resting state networks. NeuroImage 60, 2062–2072. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zanesco, A. P., King, B. G., Skwara, A. C., and Saron, C. D. (2020). Within and between-person correlates of the temporal dynamics of resting EEG microstates. NeuroImage 211:116631. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116631

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zappasodi, F., Perrucci, M. G., Saggino, A., Croce, P., Mercuri, P., Romanelli, R., et al. (2019). EEG microstates distinguish between cognitive components of fluid reasoning. NeuroImage 189, 560–573. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.067

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: EEG microstates, valence, self-generated affect, positive affect, negative affect

Citation: Nazare K and Tomescu MI (2024) Valence-specific EEG microstate modulations during self-generated affective states. Front. Psychol. 15:1300416. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1300416

Received: 23 September 2023; Accepted: 26 April 2024;
Published: 24 May 2024.

Edited by:

Frederic Von Wegner, University of New South Wales, Australia

Reviewed by:

Marco Bilucaglia, Università IULM, Italy
Luke Tait, Cardiff University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2024 Nazare and Tomescu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Miralena I. Tomescu, miralena.tomescu@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.