Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 23 February 2024
Sec. Forensic and Legal Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic New Directions in Forensic Psychology: Applying Neuropsychology, Biomarkers and Technology in Assessment & Intervention View all 13 articles

Neuropsychological assessment of aggressive offenders: a Delphi consensus study

  • 1De Waag (Outpatient Forensic Mental Health Clinic), Forensic Care Specialists, Utrecht, Netherlands
  • 2Brain and Cognition, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands

Objective: This study explores the intricate relationship between cognitive functioning and aggression, with a specific focus on individuals prone to reactive or proactive aggression. The purpose of the study was to identify important neuropsychological constructs and suitable tests for comprehending and addressing aggression.

Methods: An international panel of 32 forensic neuropsychology experts participated in this three-round Delphi study consisting of iterative online questionnaires. The experts rated the importance of constructs based on the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework. Subsequently, they suggested tests that can be used to assess these constructs and rated their suitability.

Results: The panel identified the RDoC domains Negative Valence Systems, Social Processes, Cognitive Systems and Positive Valence Systems as most important in understanding aggression. Notably, the results underscore the significance of Positive Valence Systems in proactive aggression and Negative Valence Systems in reactive aggression. The panel suggested a diverse array of 223 different tests, although they noted that not every RDoC construct can be effectively measured through a neuropsychological test. The added value of a multimodal assessment strategy is discussed.

Conclusions: This research advances our understanding of the RDoC constructs related to aggression and provides valuable insights for assessment strategies. Rather than suggesting a fixed set of tests, our study takes a flexible approach by presenting a top-3 list for each construct. This approach allows for tailored assessment to meet specific clinical or research needs. An important limitation is the predominantly Dutch composition of the expert panel, despite extensive efforts to diversify.

1 Introduction

Aggressive offenses have far-reaching consequences for individuals and society, including financial strain on health and justice sectors, public safety issues, reduced quality of life for victims, their relatives, and the offenders (Patel and Taylor, 2012; Langton et al., 2014; Rivara et al., 2019). Neuropsychological profiling is an underused clinical tool to assess the complex web of risk factors for aggressive behavior. This is surprising as the intricate relationship between cognitive functioning and reactive and proactive aggression has been widely studied. Although empirical studies and systematic reviews have uncovered neurocognitive mechanisms underlying reactive and proactive aggression (Alcázar-Córcoles et al., 2010; Kuin et al., 2015; Van De Kant et al., 2020), expert knowledge on individual neuropsychological assessment has not been integrated into the current body of research. An overarching framework that bridges the gap between fundamental research and clinical experience is therefore much needed. In the current study, a panel of experts is asked (i) which Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) domains (Insel et al., 2010; further explained below) are important in explaining reactive vs. proactive aggression and, (ii) which neuropsychological tasks are suitable for assessing those domains.

A common definition of aggression is “behavior that is intended to harm another person who is motivated to avoid that harm” (Allen and Anderson, 2017). Notably, aggressive offenders make up a substantial proportion (up to 70%) of prisons, forensic hospitals and outpatient mental health facilities (McMurran et al., 2000; Völlm et al., 2018). There is a great need for research into the risk factors of aggressive behavior to help reduce recidivism (Smeijers, 2017; Wigham et al., 2022). One of the potential risk factors for aggression that warrants exploration is cognitive functioning, particularly through neuropsychological assessments. Cognitive limitations are more prevalent among offenders than in the general population (Ogilvie et al., 2011), particularly among aggressive offenders (Cruz et al., 2020). For example, research found a prevalence of clinically significant executive deficits (a subset of cognitive functions) in an offender population ranging from 5.2% to 27.2% (correctional offenders) and 9.5–35.7% (forensic psychiatric patients), compared to 2.5% in the general population (Shumlich et al., 2019). Furthermore, multiple factors can be at the root of cognitive limitations, including traumatic brain injury, substance abuse, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, all of which are more prevalent among offenders (Harris, 2006; Ginsberg et al., 2010; Farrer and Hedges, 2011; Frost et al., 2013; Fayyad et al., 2017; Hellenbach et al., 2017; Muñoz García-Largo et al., 2020; Matheson et al., 2022). As such, it is necessary to further highlight the role of cognitive factors in the context of offending behavior, and this study aims to do so by improving knowledge about neuropsychological assessment in forensic populations.

1.1 Reactive and proactive aggression

The term “aggression” refers to a spectrum of acts that range from shouting or pushing to aggravated assault or homicide. By the definition stated above, rape, sexual assault, and robbery would also be classified as aggressive offenses. As the literature shows, most offenders are generalists, meaning they commit more than one type of crime in their lives (Simon, 1997; Soothill et al., 2000; Sullivan et al., 2006). Therefore, we chose to include aggressive sexual- or property crimes while non-aggressive crimes such as fraud were outside the scope of this study. Understanding the different determinants of aggression has been a subject of interest in various fields such as psychology, criminology, and neuroscience since the mid-20th century. Several taxonomies have been proposed in the literature (Parrott and Giancola, 2007; Krahé, 2013), but there is no consensus yet about which categorization is most appropriate. The most well-known distinction is the reactive-proactive dichotomy, sometimes referred to as hostile-instrumental (Buss, 1961). Reactive aggression occurs in reaction to a provocation or frustration and is impulsive in nature. Proactive aggression on the other hand is generally goal-directed and premeditated. Both types of aggression can occur within an individual, and thus, the strict classification into one of these two categories has been disputed in the literature (Bushman and Anderson, 2001). Currently, a dimensional view of aggression is favored, acknowledging that individuals often exhibit varying degrees of both reactive and proactive aggression rather than rigidly categorizing them into distinct types. Interestingly, research on factors associated with or related to reactive and proactive aggression provides empirical support for the usefulness of the distinction. For example, reactive aggression has been linked to heightened emotional reactivity, impulsivity, verbal impairments and impairments in executive functioning, and hostile attribution bias. Proactive aggression on the other hand is linked to a lack of moral emotions, callous and unemotional traits, and low physiological arousal (Cima and Raine, 2009). To summarize, individuals can exhibit both types of aggression, with a tendency toward one type, reflecting a predominant behavioral disposition. As both types of aggression appear to be related to different constructs, the current study considers both types of aggression separately.

1.2 Research Domain Criteria (RDoC)

The National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) developed the RDoC (Insel et al., 2010), to—as opposed to traditional categorial diagnostic systems such as the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM; American Psychiatric Association, 2022)— investigate core dimensions of functioning that underlie various mental health conditions. In addition, as aggression can occur within various mental health conditions such as personality disorders, intermittent explosive disorder and conduct disorder, the RDoC framework provides a transdiagnostic perspective to uncover shared mechanisms that contribute to aggression across these diverse disorders. The RDoC describes six domains: (1) Negative Valence Systems, responsible for responses to aversive situations or context, such as fear, anxiety, and loss; (2) Positive Valence Systems, responsible for responses to positive motivational situations or contexts, such as reward seeking, consummatory behavior, and reward/habit learning; (3) Cognitive Systems, responsible for various cognitive processes; (4) Social Processes, which mediate responses to interpersonal settings of various types, including perception and interpretation of others' actions; (5) Sensorimotor Systems, responsible for the control and execution of motor behaviors, and their refinement during learning and development; and (6) Arousal/Regulatory Systems responsible for generating activation of neural systems as appropriate for various contexts, and providing appropriate homeostatic regulation of such systems as energy balance and sleep (see Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Overview of the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) domains, including 25 constructs (bold) and 33 subconstructs (regular text).

1.3 Aggression and cognitive domains

In this section, the existing knowledge regarding the interplay between aggression and the domains outlined in the RDoC framework is briefly elucidated. If available, we refer to systematic reviews and/or meta-analyses. Neuropsychological studies have revealed differences between (aggressive) offenders and non-offending controls in different RDoC domains, such as Cognitive Systems (including executive functions, attention, and language) (Cohen et al., 2003; Ogilvie et al., 2011; Anderson et al., 2016; Burgess, 2020; Chow et al., 2022), Social Processes (Marsh and Blair, 2008; Karoglu et al., 2022), and Positive/Negative Valence systems (Estrada et al., 2019; Manning, 2020; mainly reward and threat processing). In our recent multi-level meta-analysis, we have studied all domains of cognitive functioning in relation to offending behavior (Hutten et al., preprint). Overall, offenders performed worse on neuropsychological tests than non-offending controls, and this was the case for all of the cognitive domains studied. A notable observation from this meta-analysis was the substantial variation in tests (146 different tests), and the lack of studies from non-Western countries. Through the Delphi method, we aim to gather insights from forensic neuropsychology experts across the world to obtain consensus on the most suitable tests to measure neuropsychological functioning in aggressive offenders. With this, we aim to expand on this empirical knowledge by connecting research findings and their translational application in forensic practice.

The primary goal of offender rehabilitation is reducing recidivism. A recent global systematic review found 2-year recidivism rates of 18–55% after incarceration and 10–47% after community sentences (Yukhnenko et al., 2023). Psychological treatment has a small but positive effect on recidivism in violent offenders, with a 10.2% difference in recidivism between treated vs. non-treated offenders (Papalia et al., 2019). Despite these findings, there remains a need for further enhancements in intervention strategies to reduce recidivism more effectively. More knowledge on the relation between the RDoC domains and aggression could enhance offender rehabilitation in several ways. Studies have found worse executive functioning in recidivists compared to first time offenders (Ross and Hoaken, 2011; Sánchez de Ribera et al., 2022). Conventional risk assessment tools appear to have reached their ceiling effect, achieving a moderate area under the curve of 0.70, (Monahan and Skeem, 2014; Ogonah et al., 2023). Risk-assessment tools often measure cognitive factors like impulsivity and self-control through less objective methods such as observer ratings and self-reports. Neuropsychological tasks are considered more objective, excluding the impact of compromised self-insight (Steward and Kretzmer, 2022). Accordingly, expanding risk assessment to include neuropsychological and neurobiological factors alongside the existing psychosocial risk factors may enhance the accuracy recidivism predictions (Aharoni et al., 2013, 2014; Haarsma et al., 2020; Zijlmans et al., 2021; Nauta-Jansen, 2022). In addition to predicting recidivism, cognitive functioning—in particular inhibitory and cognitive flexibility difficulties—also appears to predict treatment dropout and treatment success (Fishbein et al., 2009; Cornet et al., 2014). Identifying the specific cognitive domains that are impaired in offenders and related to aggression is crucial to providing targeted interventions and reducing the risk of criminal behavior. For example, an aggression regulation training could be suitable for individuals with aggression arising from inhibitory problems, while people with difficulties in emotion recognition might benefit more from an emotion recognition training (Li et al., 2023). Hence, misidentification of the determinants of the aggression may lead to suboptimal treatment.

Although a clear link has been demonstrated between cognitive limitations and aggression, the use of neuropsychology in forensic settings has not reached its full potential. For example, incorporation of neurobiological information in Dutch pretrial forensic reports was low and did not rise significantly from 2005 to 2015 (Kempes et al., 2019). Additionally, even when neurobiological factors were acknowledged in relation to the offense, they were often overlooked in discussions about future risk assessment and -management. There are three explanations for this observation which are not mutually exclusive. First of all, clinicians are likely to struggle identifying the most suitable instruments as there is a plethora of neuropsychological tests available. A systematic review on neuropsychological assessment practices in forensic settings found a notable diversity in assessment tools, with 140 different types of tests. The authors conclude that a wide range of neuropsychological functions are being measured by a large number of instruments (Venturi Da Silva and Cavalheiro Hamdan, 2022). Related to this, many tests have multiple outcomes—often measuring different cognitive functions—or multiple ways to calculate the outcomes. This heterogeneity may compromise the reliability of test results and raises questions about how information is understood by clinicians and legal practitioners (Serafim et al., 2015). Second, for most neuropsychological tests normative data are collected from general population samples and have not been validated for the offender population. Possibly, the use of default norm scores leads to insufficient differentiation among individuals in the offender setting (Cornet et al., 2016). As such, it remains unclear which tests are most sensitive and suitable for the aggressive offender population. Third, offender populations present unique challenges in conducting neuropsychological assessments, such as high rates of noncompliance, low motivation (for treatment and/or assessment), and limited education and literacy levels (Hetland et al., 2007; Tuominen et al., 2014). Cultural and linguistic differences may also need to be considered when conducting neuropsychological assessments with offender populations. Considering these challenges, further research and tailored approaches are required to address the selection of suitable tests and norms for the aggressive offender population, to ensure accurate and reliable assessments.

1.4 Study objectives

This study aims to identify the most suitable neuropsychological tests for cognitive assessment within the aggressive offender population, distinguishing between predominantly reactive vs. proactive aggressive offenders. With this, our research contributes to the advancement of forensic neuropsychology. By pinpointing the specific cognitive domains associated with both reactive and proactive aggression, we aim to pave the way for more targeted assessments and interventions in aggressive offender populations. To achieve this goal, we need to bridge the gap between research and clinical practice and strife toward consensus among an international panel of experts from the field of forensic neuropsychology. In the current study, we will apply the Delphi methodology for this purpose. Our objectives encompass two categories of questions posed to the expert panel: firstly, we seek theoretical insights into the constructs commonly associated with aggression, emphasizing their significance in the evaluation of aggressive offenders; secondly, we aim to pinpoint the most suitable tests for this evaluation, thereby facilitating future test selection in forensic contexts.

2 Materials and methods

This study was preregistered at AsPredicted (#103758) and has been approved by the Ethics Review Board (ERB) of the University of Amsterdam (ERB number: 2022-BC-15289).

2.1 The Delphi methodology

We conducted a Delphi consensus study to obtain consensus among an international panel of experts in forensic neuropsychology. While meta-analyses and reviews allow us to have and overview of the current scientific knowledge, the Delphi method allows us to obtain insight into the existing clinical expertise. The Delphi method is a technique used to achieve consensus among a group of experts by soliciting their opinions through a series of questionnaires and providing them with controlled feedback (Dalkey and Helmer, 1963). The Delphi method is based on the concept of collective wisdom, which assumes that the combined opinion of multiple people is closer to the truth than a single individual's perspective (Habibi et al., 2014). Recently, researchers have been striving to achieve consensus on various neuropsychological topics, such as the definition of the term ‘impairment' or inconsistent use of test score labels (e.g., Guilmette et al., 2020). Our study aligns with these developments. The Delphi methodology, with its collaborative and iterative nature, serves as an effective tool within this context, facilitating the establishment of a shared foundation for understanding and addressing diverse neuropsychological considerations in the field. This is carried out by aggregating the results of online, anonymous questionnaires in a systematic way. The current study consisted of three rounds, which are described in 2.4 Procedure and data analysis.

2.2 Expert panel selection

Both researchers and clinicians employed in the field of forensic neuropsychology were invited to participate in the panel. Potential researchers were identified based on the articles that emerged from our literature review which is in review (Hutten et al., preprint). The researchers who had a minimum of two publications on the topic of forensic neuropsychology, of which one in the last five years (to confirm that they were still actively engaged in the field) were approached to participate in the Delphi study. For clinicians, the inclusion criterium is at least 4 years' experience as a (clinical) neuropsychologist in the forensic setting. Recruitment took place through the author's networks, (international) societies or networks for neuropsychology/forensic psychology, social media, and through the “snowballing technique” (Iqbal and Pipon-Young, 2009). Panels with 10 to 50 members are recommended for Delphi studies (Turoff, 1975). In total, 127 potential experts were invited personally by email. Sixty-three potential experts started the questionnaire and provided digital informed consent. Thirty potential experts responded they could not participate (no time: 13, questioned their own expertise: 15, no reason: 2). Finally, 32 experts completed the first-round questionnaire.

2.3 Research Domain Criteria (RDoC)

This Delphi study was based on the RDoC framework (Insel et al., 2010). The RDoC model is a research framework that approaches mental health and psychopathology by examining major domains of basic human neurobehavioral functioning, rather than relying on traditional diagnostic categories. The model consists of six major functional domains (see Figure 1), and each domain is studied by exploring different aspects using constructs that are examined across a range of functioning from normal to abnormal.

2.4 Procedure and data analysis

In three consecutive rounds of online questionnaires (compiled through Qualtrics, 2023), experts rated the importance of a predetermined list of the RDoC constructs on a 5-point scale from 1 “not important” to 5 “essential”, with a non-neutral midpoint of 3 (moderately important). Using a non-neutral midpoint forces panelists to deliberate and to decide about the importance of the constructs. If they felt incompetent to answer a question, a “don't know” option was available (Linstone and Turoff, 1975). Subsequently, the panel members provided suggestions for tests that can be used to measure the constructs they rated at least moderately important (rating 3 or higher). In addition, they rated each other's test suggestions as suitable or not suitable for aggressive offenders. Throughout the questionnaires, panel members can provide explanations or reasoning. Before distributing the questionnaire for the first round, two clinical neuropsychologists filled in the questionnaire to provide feedback and ensure clarity of the questions.

After each round, the constructs that did not achieve consensus (about their importance) moved into the subsequent round for re-rating. Our operationalization of consensus is interquartile range (IQR) ≤ 1. For a four to five-point Likert scale, an IQR of 1 or less is considered a high level of consensus (Raskin, 1994; Rayens and Hahn, 2000).

For the importance ratings of the RDoC constructs, means and standard deviations are reported. We conducted Mann-Whitney U tests to analyze the difference in importance scores between reactive and proactive aggression, primarily due to the ordinal nature of the data. For the suitability of tests, we reported the percentage of the panel that rated the test as suitable.

2.4.1 Round 1

The objectives of the first round were (1) to identify the most important RDoC constructs that should be included in the assessment of aggressive offenders, and (2) to collect suggestions for tests that are recommended to assess these constructs. Before the experts started with the main questions, they were asked to fill in some information about their age, gender, profession, current workplace, and academic degree.

Then, the panel members were asked to rate the importance of the RDoC constructs. For the constructs they rated as at least moderately important, they were also asked to rate the importance of the underlying subconstructs. The experts were able to suggest additional constructs not delineated in the RDoC. The research team (JH, JvH, SH, TZ, and HG) evaluated these suggestions to confirm they were not already covered in the RDoC, they were clearly described, and they were within the scope of the RDoC [as suggested by Jorm (2015)]. These additional constructs were then added to subsequent rounds.

Next, for the constructs that they rated as at least moderately important, the experts gave suggestions for tests that they recommend for the assessment of this construct. They could give several suggestions per construct.

2.4.2 Round 2

The 32 panel members who completed round 1 were invited to participate in round 2 of the study, which 26 of them did. (Sub)constructs that did not reach consensus in round 1 were rated again. These constructs were presented along with feedback outlining the average panel rating, each expert's own previous response, and a synopsis of comments that were offered by experts in support of their opinion. In addition, the additional constructs added by the panelists in round 1 were rated for importance. Then, the experts scored the suitability of the recommended tests suggested in round 1 (suitable/not suitable/don't know). If the round 1 tests suggestions were not specific enough—e.g., a test category such as “gambling tests” or a measurement goal such as “verbal comprehension tests”—the panel was asked to specify in this round.

2.4.3 Round 3

The 26 panel members who completed round 2 were invited to participate in round 3 of the study. Round 3 was completed by 24 panel members. This round was mostly similar to round 2. In addition, the top-3 tests that were rated most frequently as suitable and were known by at least half of the panel were presented to the panel members. They were asked to rank these tests from most to least suitable.

3 Results

Thirty-two experts completed round 1 of the study (mean age = 43.44, SD = 11.20, 15 males, 17 females). Characteristics of the expert panel are displayed in Table 1. Despite repeated attempts (see paragraph 2.2) to gather an international expert panel, most experts were currently working/living in the Netherlands (n = 17). Seven of the experts were researchers, nine were clinicians, fifteen professionals integrated their therapeutic work with scientific research, and one was currently employed as manager. Of the original panel, twenty-four completed all three rounds of questionnaires and were included in the consortium.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the initial expert panel (N = 32).

In round 1, for each construct a panel member rated as at least moderately important (rating 3 or higher), the panel member was asked to suggest one or more tests to measure this construct. In total, 223 different tests were suggested by the panel.

In round 2, the panel rated these tests as “suitable”, “not suitable” or “don't know”. In round 3, we presented the panel with the three most-suitable tests (that were known by at least half of the panel) per construct, and we asked them whether they agreed with this top three. However, many did not fill in these questions. One explanation is that they did not know one or more of the tests, making it impossible to rank them. Another possibility is a decrease in motivation as the questionnaires were quite extensive and time consuming. Because of this, we based the top-3 tests in Table 3 on the suitability ratings from round 2. For certain constructs, fewer than three tests were familiar to at least half of the panel, resulting in less than three test suggestions (or even zero) being included in the overview.

To aid clinicians in their test selection, we included some practical information about the administration time, age range, manual, and psychometric properties of the tests. We derived this information from test manuals, systematic reviews, and books. If these were not available, we reported on single studies with a sample that was most similar to the aggressive offender population. Our goal was not to create an exhaustive and comprehensive overview, as it falls beyond the scope of this study. Therefore, we refer readers to the British Psychological Society test reviews using the EFPA review model (British Psychological Society, n.d.), the Buros Center for Testing (Buros Center for Testing, n.d.), or for Dutch readers the COTAN (NIP, n.d.) for more information about the psychometric properties of tests.

Below, we discuss the results per domain, sorted by importance-rating (see Table 2 and Figure 2). First, the importance ratings are discussed, including the reasoning provided by the panel members. Then, the test suggestions are discussed.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Final ratings of the RDoC constructs.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Boxplots of importance scores of the RDoC domains for reactive (R) and proactive (P) aggression. SP, Social Processes; SM, Sensorimotor Systems; PV, Positive Valence Systems; NV, Negative Valence Systems; CS, Cognitive Systems; AR, Arousal/Regulatory Systems.

3.1 Negative valence systems

Negative Valence Systems were rated as the most important domain (M = 3.81, SD = 0.14), with a significant difference between reactive (M = 4.08, SD = 0.32) and proactive (M = 3.54, SD = 0.19) aggression (U = 2, p = 0.009). The ability to learn from one's own errors was suggested as an addition to this domain in round 1. Reaction to threat was rated as more important for reactive than for proactive aggression (acute: M = 4.50 vs. 3.24, potential: M = 4.31 vs. 3.44, sustained: M = 4.12 vs 3.68). The panel reasoned that as reactive aggression is driven by an immediate emotional reaction to a perceived threat or provocation, these constructs are more relevant in reactive aggression. Anxiety might make individuals more sensitive to perceived provocations, increasing the likelihood of aggression. Prolonged exposure to threat (sustained threat) might result in chronic stress and might cause individuals to use aggression to end the threat. Loss and being unable to achieve goals or experience rewards (frustrative non-reward) can lead to feelings of anger, sadness and disappointment. Aggression might be a way to cope with these feelings.

In total, 22 neuropsychological tests were suggested by the panel to assess Negative Valence Systems. The panel commented that it might be better to assess this domain by including biological measures (heart rate, eye tracking/pupil size, skin conductance) or self-report. For frustration, observation from potentially frustrating tests was also suggested, however, it was noted that test observations should not be confused with objective test results. Frustration from not performing the test correctly is not the intended measurement of the test and therefore, not an objective test result. For some Negative Valence constructs, it was impossible to validly assemble a top-3 as many tests that were suggested were unknown by more than half of the panel. Therefore, Acute Threat and Loss have only two tests in the overview and Sustained threat only one.

3.2 Social processes

The domain Social Processes was rated as the second most important overall (M = 3.80, SD = 0.33), with a nonsignificant difference between reactive (M = 3.90, SD = 0.35) and proactive (M = 3.70, SD = 0.37) and aggression (U = 190.5, p = 0.097). Four additional constructs were added to this domain: sympathy, moral reasoning, ability to correctly understand the authenticity of others' emotions, and emotional contagion. The panel commented that the ability to interpret social cues (including other people's emotions and social ambiguity), is crucial in understanding and preventing aggression. Misinterpretations can increase the risk of both reactive and proactive aggression. However, proactive aggression may be less influenced by this as people with high callous-unemotional traits tend to be less concerned with other people's emotions. Another important aspect within this domain is empathy. High callous unemotional traits in individuals displaying proactive aggression often involves cognitive empathy without affective empathy, enabling manipulative behavior.

It was noted by the panel that it might be more feasible to assess Social Processes with interviews, questionnaires and observations instead of neuropsychological tests. In total, 34 tests were suggested for this domain, resulting in a top-3 tests for each construct.

3.3 Cognitive systems

Next, Cognitive Systems were rated as M = 3.65 (SD = 0.44), with a non-significant difference between reactive (M = 3.72, SD = 0.48) and proactive aggression (M = 3.58, SD = 0.44; U = 134.5, p = 0.389). The panel reflected on why the Cognitive Systems are (not) important to consider in aggressive patients. Working memory is needed to process and react to triggers (reactive aggression), but on the other hand, working memory is required to plan proactive aggressive behaviors. Attention was deemed important in reactive aggression, as it can be biased toward potential threats, while ignoring neutral or friendly information. Cognitive control is linked to inhibition, which can help prevent future (especially reactive) aggression. In addition, cognitive control is important to be able to find non-aggressive solutions to problems, and to apply lessons from therapy into daily life (also related to declarative memory). Language was considered important in assessing aggression because poor verbal skills can hinder the ability to find non-aggressive solutions in conflicts, potentially leading to misunderstandings and frustration. The role of perception was somewhat unclear among the panel members. Counterfactual reasoning and information processing speed were added as additions to this domain.

For Cognitive Systems, a large number (154) of different tests was suggested. The top-3 tests per construct are displayed in Table 3.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Characteristics of the top-3 most suitable tests per construct, sorted by highest importance rating (N = 25).

3.4 Positive valence systems

Positive Valence Systems were rated with M = 3.54 (SD = 0.11). Interestingly, this was the only domain that was deemed significantly more important for proactive (M = 3.76, SD = 0.11) than for reactive aggression (M = 3.32, SD = 0.14; U = 144, p < 0.001). The panel members reasoned that individuals with high reward responsiveness may be more motivated to engage in proactive aggression, driven by the pursuit of rewards and experiencing greater pleasure and motivation when such rewards are at stake. Reward learning is important for proactive aggression, as individuals who have learned that aggressive behavior leads to desired outcomes are more likely to repeat such behavior to achieve their goals. Lastly, the value of potential rewards shape proactive aggression, with those highly valuing rewards associated with aggression, like financial gain or social status, being more inclined to engage in this form of aggression. Reactive aggression is more indirectly related to reward as the alleviation of distress or protection can be considered the reward in this context.

The experts suggested 14 different tests to assess Positive Valence Systems. The top-3 tests are displayed in Table 3. For Reward Valuation, there were only two tests known by half of the panel. It was noted by the panel that many of these tests do not directly measure reactions to rewards, but this can be inferred through observation.

3.5 Arousal/regulatory systems

Arousal/Regulatory Systems were rated with M = 3.44 (SD = 0.50) importance overall, with non-significant difference between reactive (M = 3.66, SD = 0.63) and proactive (M = 3.21, SD = 0.38) aggression (U = 2, p = 0.400). The construct Arousal was considered very important in reactive aggression (M = 4.38), where impulsive and emotionally charged responses are common. Conversely, in proactive aggression (M = 3.63), the issue often revolves around the absence of arousal or under-arousal, suggesting a potential opposite relationship. It was highlighted that arousal is a state rather than a trait and is subject to rapid fluctuations influenced by environmental factors that can be challenging to measure. Disturbances in sleep and circadian rhythms could have consequences on daily mood patterns, possibly affecting emotional regulation and impulsivity.

The panel noted the absence of tests to measure arousal. Instead, they proposed physiological measures (such as EEG, heart rate variability, skin conductance, pupil dilation) behavioral/observational methods (such as wearables, questionnaires, or diaries), and neuroimaging. The seven tests that were suggested by the panel are often developed to measure different constructs such as motor skills, attention, and inhibition, and were all—except for the go/no-go task—unknown by half of the panel. Therefore, no top-3 could be validly constructed.

3.6 Sensorimotor systems

The domain with the lowest importance rating was Sensorimotor Systems (M = 3.18, SD = 0.48), with non-significant difference between reactive (M = 3.25, SD = 0.48) and proactive aggression (M = 3.10, SD = 0.51; U = 45, p = 739). The construct “sensorimotor integration” was suggested as an addition to this domain. The panel reasoned that sensorimotor systems might be relevant in understanding reactive aggression, which can be impulsive and driven by limbic responses, particularly in individuals with trauma or dissociation. These motor reactions can lead to a loss of agency and ownership over actions, potentially becoming self-fulfilling. Automatic aggressive behaviors learned from early experiences may be tied to sensorimotor patterns, particularly in reactive aggression. However, there's debate over whether these constructs can be clinically measured and if they directly correlate with quantifiable aggression.

Nevertheless, the panel suggested 39 different tests to measure Sensorimotor Systems. These tests encompass a wide range from executive functioning/planning tests (e.g., tower tests) to tests that more directly measure motor skills and coordination. A panel member proposed the idea of using advanced technology like movement sensors and virtual reality to understand how people physically react to challenging situations.

3.7 Additional suggestions

Lastly, the panel suggested four constructs that do not fit within the RDoC domains but might be worth considering when assessing aggressive offenders. For intelligence, the panel agreed (IQr = 1) that this is moderately to very important to include (intelligence: reactive 3.54, proactive: 3.75). It was noted that general intelligence might not provide additional information beyond the specific cognitive functions already encompassed within the model or if these specific functions might completely explain the association between intelligence and aggression. Secondly, cognitive distortions—which are biased or irrational patterns of thoughts and perception that can influence a person's beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors—were deemed moderately to very important (IQr = 1, reactive: 3.78, proactive: 3.87). A panel member noted that cognitive distortions are influenced by inner psychological patterns or past traumas and can cause a person to misinterpret what's going on, making them more likely to engage in violent behavior. Third, emotion regulation was rated as essential for reactive aggression (4.39, IQr = 1), but there was no consensus for proactive aggression (3.74, IQr = 2). Lastly, symptom/performance validity was added as a suggestion, but the panel did not reach consensus on this construct (reactive: 2.90, proactive: 3.14, IQr = 2). The panel members commented that the addition of symptom/performance validity tests is valuable for detecting feigned or exaggerated symptoms and can help to ensure that decision about risk assessment/management and legal decisions are based accurate information. However, these type of tests are less directly related to understanding the origins of aggressive/offending behavior per se.

4 Discussion

In this Delphi study, we investigated two questions by surveying an international expert panel. Firstly, we sought theoretical insights into the constructs commonly associated with aggression, emphasizing their importance in the evaluation of predominantly reactive vs. predominantly proactive aggressive offenders. Secondly, we aimed to pinpoint the most suitable tests for this assessment, thereby facilitating future test selection in forensic contexts.

4.1 RDoC constructs

Overall, all RDoC domains were considered at least moderately important (>3) by the expert panel for the neuropsychological assessment of aggressive offenders. Taken together, Social Processes and Negative Valence Systems were rated as the most important in understanding aggression, while Sensorimotor Systems were considered least important. These findings are in line with studies that found a relation between aggression and executive functions and attention (Bergvall et al., 2001; Ogilvie et al., 2011; Burgess, 2020; Cruz et al., 2020), language (Cohen et al., 2003; Anderson et al., 2016; Chow et al., 2022), social cognition (Karoglu et al., 2022), and reward and threat processing (Estrada et al., 2019; Manning, 2020). Below, we will further explore the importance of the RDoC constructs considering the distinction between reactive and proactive aggression.

4.2 Reactive vs. proactive aggression

The extent to which experts differed in their opinion about the theoretical importance of the RDoC constructs for understanding reactive aggression compared to proactive aggression was rather small for most domains. The most pronounced difference was that Positive Valence Systems were deemed more important to understand proactive aggression, whereas Negative Valence Systems were considered most relevant for understanding reactive aggression. Both come as no surprise based on previous research. Differences in reward processing are found in children and adults with conduct disorder, callous unemotional treats, antisocial personality disorder and psychopathy (Estrada et al., 2019). As these diagnoses are generally related to proactive aggression (Merk et al., 2005; Cima and Raine, 2009; White et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2017), this outcome fits well into what we know. In addition, studies have shown that in people with impulsive-antisocial traits linked to psychopathy, their brains released more dopamine in the nucleus accumbens when exposed to rewards, suggesting an hyperreactivity to rewards (Buckholtz et al., 2010). This highlights the relevance of trying to unravel the antecedents of aggression for assessment and treatment. Reactive aggression on the other hand is a primary reaction to perceived treat.

For the other domains, the difference in perceived importance between reactive and proactive aggression were rather small. This may be explained by the fact that some RDoC constructs, such as arousal, are quite broad. It has been reported in empirical studies that reactive aggression involves high affective-physiological arousal while proactive aggression is characterized by minimal autonomic arousal (Chase et al., 2001; Blair, 2003). In other words, arousal might be important in both types of aggression, albeit in different ways. Another example: compromised working memory might be associated with increased reactive aggression, as it is needed to process and react to triggers, while in proactive aggression, working memory is required for planning acts of violence, making it equally important but in a different manner. In other words, while RDoC constructs are important to evaluate to gain insights into the determinants of both forms of aggression, they may play different roles in the two types of aggression.

4.3 Expert recommendations for neuropsychological test usage

In total, 223 different tests were suggested by the panel. This indicates that an large number of neuropsychological tests have been developed in the past decades and attests to the field's rapid development. It also presents a challenge for clinicians in choosing the most suitable tests. In addition, aggression is a multifaceted construct that cannot be measured through a single test. The distinction between reactive and proactive aggression adds another layer of complexity. In response to these challenges, we constructed a guide for clinicians and researchers, a curated selection of the three most favored tests as assessed by our panel of experts.

It must be noted that our aim was to provide an overview that offers a selection of the most suitable tests to measure the RDoC constructs, rather than constructing a fixed battery of tests. By presenting an overview of the most important neuropsychological constructs along with the most suitable tests to measure them, clinicians and researchers can select specific constructs that are most relevant to their case. However, for certain subgroups, particularly when assessing patients with intellectual disabilities or patients who are illiterate, the tests suggested in our study might not be suitable. In those cases, clinicians are encouraged to seek for alternative tests. In the case of intellectual disabilities, it is proposed to use adapted versions of the original tests (such as the children's version) (Willner et al., 2010). In the case of illiteracy, the suggestion is to modify tests to resemble real-life situations instead of school-based procedures (Kosmidis, 2018). It is noteworthy that both intellectual disabilities and illiteracy more prevalent in forensic populations than in the general population (Harris, 2006; Tuominen et al., 2014; Hellenbach et al., 2017; Muñoz García-Largo et al., 2020), underscoring the importance of considering these factors in the selection of appropriate assessment tools. In addition, it is important to note that some of the tests that emerged from our study are subject to criticism, often in absence of better alternatives (e.g., the Thematic Apperception Test, see Lilienfeld et al., 2000). It is beyond the scope of this study to address tests individually.

Assessing these constructs may help to explain the determinants of the aggressive behavior which can provide valuable input for tailored treatment planning. Another important outcome is that the panel indicated that not every RDoC construct is appropriate to be measured by neuropsychological testing. For example, it was noted that the construct affiliation/attachment can be more effectively assessed through a structured interview, and arousal through observation or physiological measures. The RDoC matrix provides numerous examples of self-report and physiological measures for assessing its constructs (National Institute of Mental Health, 2023). Hence, a combination of neuropsychological tests, interviews, self-report, observation, and physiological measures might be needed to optimally measure the RDoC constructs.

4.4 Limitations

The findings of this Delphi study need to be considered in the context of a few limitations. Firstly, despite repeated and extensive attempts to include a representative global panel, half of the panel consisted of people from the Netherlands. The continents of South America and Africa were not represented at all and other continents were underrepresented (especially taking the number of inhabitants into account). Since neuropsychological practices are affected by, for example, the country's health care system, legal framework, and cultural norms, this is likely to have influenced the results of the study (Kasten et al., 2021). This may have also limited generalizability as certain recommendations might be more tailored to the Netherlands.

Furthermore, while every effort was made to ensure conciseness of the questionnaires, it is essential to recognize that participant motivation can influence the quality and consistency of expert input in iterative research endeavors like the Delphi method. Eight panel members (25% of the original panel, of which three from the Netherlands, 2 USA, 1 Italy, 1 Sweden, 1 Australia) did not complete all three rounds. This may have had implications for representativeness of the panel as they might have had different perspectives than the remaining 24 experts. Fortunately, most information was gathered in round 1 where experts rated all RDoC constructs and provided their test suggestions.

The panel generated a large number (223) of neuropsychological tests that can be used to measure the RDoC constructs. The panel was unfamiliar with many of the tests (56% of the tests were unknown to more than half of the panel), which prevented them from forming an opinion about their suitability. As a consequence, we could not validly construct a top-3 for each RDoC construct. For these constructs, we refer readers to the RDoC matrix for other assessment suggestions (National Institute of Mental Health, 2023).

Other limitations stem from the Delphi methodology. The approach toward consensus may exclude different but possibly important perspectives of individual panel members. The results of a Delphi study represent the ratings with the most overlap between the panel members, but this is not necessarily the “objective truth”. Our study wasn't designed to uncover objective truths; instead, we aimed to identify best practices. Moreover, the Delphi procedure precludes direct contact between panel members to avoid group pressure toward conformity and possible effects of authority. However, a discussion can often lead to valuable insights. To address this, the panel members could read each other's comments and reasonings anonymously in round 2 and 3. This could help them in understanding the source of potential discrepancies in ratings and possibly change their opinion. We highlighted that they were not obliged to change their ratings if their opinion had not been changed.

4.5 Implications and future directions

While this study represents a significant step forward in the endeavor to achieve adequate neuropsychological assessment of aggressive offenders, it is essential to acknowledge that our understanding of the relationship between the RDoC domains and aggression remains complex. Studying the interrelations between the constructs might provide more insights into aggressive behavior. For example, a lack of attention might lead to misinterpretation of social cues and a compromised working memory can lead to difficulties in emotion regulation.

Another prominent challenge that emerges from our study is the validation of the neuropsychological tests proposed by the expert panel. Moreover, to ensure that neuropsychological assessments are meaningful and sensitive to the unique characteristics of aggressive offenders, the field should focus on collecting more appropriate normative data.

Furthermore, the possible incorporation of neuropsychological test findings into risk assessment and management should be studied more thoroughly. This approach aligns with the Risk-Need-Responsivity (RNR) model (Bonta and Andrews, 2023), a leading framework in the forensic field. The findings have two connections to the RNR model. First, previous studies have indicated the added value of including biopsychosocial factors for the prediction of recidivism (Aharoni et al., 2013, 2014; Haarsma et al., 2020; Zijlmans et al., 2021). This aligns with the 'Need' principle of the RNR model, which emphasizes the importance of targeting criminogenic needs that are associated with an individual's likelihood of reoffending. Second, beyond understanding the cognitive limitations associated with aggression, future research should explore how this knowledge can be translated into effective intervention strategies. Specifically, cognitive limitations may play a crucial role in an individual's responsiveness to treatment, adhering to the 'Responsivity' principle of the RNR model. One might expect that if offenders have attentional difficulties or memory problems, that will influence treatment effectiveness. Longitudinal studies can help to understand how changes in neuropsychological function are related to changes in aggression and recidivism, further strengthening the connection between the RNR principles and the incorporation of neuropsychological assessments in risk assessment and -management strategies.

4.6 Conclusion

This Delphi consensus study shed light on the role of the RDoC framework in understanding and assessing aggression in offenders. The experts' ratings underline the multidimensional nature of aggression, calling for a holistic approach when assessing and addressing aggression. Furthermore, distinguishing between reactive and proactive aggression provides useful insights into the mechanisms involved in aggressive behavior.

The extensive list of proposed neuropsychological tests, as well as the construction of a top-3 list for each construct, provide clinicians and researchers with a useful resource when it comes to selecting suitable tests. This overview allows for a flexible approach by tailoring assessments to specific clinical or research requirements. Furthermore, the acknowledgment that certain constructs may be better examined through interviews, observations, or physiological measures emphasizes the added value of a multimodal assessment strategy.

Future research should focus on test validation, normative data collecting, and the integration of neuropsychological findings into risk assessment and intervention as our understanding of the complex relationship between RDoC domains and aggression advances. Our Delphi consensus study not only enhances our comprehension of aggression in offenders through the application of the RDoC framework but also provides a comprehensive guide for clinicians and researchers in the selection of neuropsychological tests. The findings of this Delphi study offer a steppingstone for advancing the field of neuropsychological assessment in understanding and addressing aggressive behavior.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Review Board (ERB) of the University of Amsterdam. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

JCH: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – original draft. JEH: Supervision, Writing – review and editing, Conceptualization. SH: Writing – review and editing, Conceptualization. TZ: Supervision, Writing – review and editing, Conceptualization. HG: Writing – review and editing, Supervision, Conceptualization.

Forensic Neuropsychology Consortium Members

Hasanen Al-Taiar, Nadia Bolognini, Lisanne Breuer, Josanne van Dongen, William Garmoe, Beverly Griffor, Jochem Jansen, Frank Jonker, Mike van Kessel, Niki Kuin, Femke Kuipers, Gary de Man, Bernice Marcopulos, Jesse Meijers, Pieter Molleman, Milo Oudenhoven, Rajakumari Reddy, J. Rajeshwaran Jamuna, Cristina Scarpazza, Anna van der Schoot, Carmen Sergiou, Andrea Stracciari, Marijke Willems, Stefano Zago.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Eva Warreman for sharing her insights and experiences in conducting a Delphi study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1328839/full#supplementary-material

References

Aharoni, E., Mallett, J., Vincent, G. M., Harenski, C. L., Calhoun, V. D., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., et al. (2014). Predictive accuracy in the neuroprediction of rearrest. Soc. Neurosci. 9, 332–336. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.907201

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aharoni, E., Vincent, G. M., Harenski, C. L., Calhoun, V. D., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., Gazzaniga, M. S., et al. (2013). Neuroprediction of future rearrest. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 110, 6223–6228. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1219302110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alcázar-Córcoles, M. Á., Verdejo-García, A., Bouso-Saiz, J. C., and Bezos-Saldaña, L. (2010). Neuropsychology of impulsive aggression. Revista de Neurologica 10, 2009316. doi: 10.33588/rn.5005.2009316

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, J. J., and Anderson, C. A. (2017). “Aggression and violence: definitions and distinctions,” in The Wiley Handbook of Violence and Aggression, ed. P. Sturmey (New York: John Wiley and Sons, Ltd), 1–14.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2022). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5-TR). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association Publishing.

Google Scholar

Anderson, S. A. S., Hawes, D. J., and Snow, P. C. (2016). Language impairments among youth offenders: a systematic review. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 65, 195–203. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2016.04.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., and Plumb, I. (2001). The reading the mind in the eyes test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Child Psychiat. Psychiat. 42, 715. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Batista, A. X., Adda, C. C., Miotto, E. C., Lúcia, M. C. S. D., and Scaff, M. (2007). Torre de Londres e Torre de Hanói: Contribuições distintas para avaliação do funcionamento executivo. J. Bras. Psiquiatr. 56, 134–139. doi: 10.1590/S0047-20852007000200010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A. (2016). Iowa Gambling Task, Version 2. PAR. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

Google Scholar

Benton, A. L., Varney, N. R., and De Hamsher, K. (1978). Visuospatial judgment: a clinical test. Arch. Neurol. 35, 364. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1978.00500300038006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergvall, A. H., Wessely, H., Forsman, A., and Hansen, S. (2001). A deficit in attentional set-shifting of violent offenders. Psychol. Med. 31, 1095–1105. doi: 10.1017/S0033291701004317

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, R. J. R. (2003). Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. Br. J. Psychiat. 182, 5–7. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.1.5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonta, J., and Andrews, D. A. (2023). The psychology of criminal conduct (Seventh Edition). Routledge: Taylor and Francis Group.

Google Scholar

British Psychological Society (n.d.). Test Reviews. Available online at: https://explore.bps.org.uk/test-reviews (accessed September 21 2023).

Google Scholar

Buckholtz, J. W., Treadway, M. T., Cowan, R. L., Woodward, N. D., Benning, S. D., Li, R., et al. (2010). Mesolimbic dopamine reward system hypersensitivity in individuals with psychopathic traits. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 419–421. doi: 10.1038/nn.2510

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, J. (2020). A brief review of the relationship of executive function assessment and violence. Aggress. Violent Behav. 4, 101414. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2020.101414

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buros Center for Testing (n.d.). Buros Online Shop: Neuropsychological. Available online at: https://marketplace.unl.edu/buros/test-reviews/categories/neuropsychological?dir=desc&limit=80&order=name&p=1 (accessed September 21 2023).

Google Scholar

Bushman, B. J., and Anderson, C. A. (2001). Is it time to pull the plug on the hostile versus instrumental aggression dichotomy? Psychol. Rev. 108, 273–279. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.108.1.273

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, A. H. (1961). The Psychology of Aggression. New York: John Wiley and Sons Inc., 307.

Google Scholar

Cambridge Cognition (2012). CANTABeclipse Test Administration Guide. Cambdige, UK: Cambridge Cognition Ltd.

Google Scholar

Chase, K. A., O'Leary, K. D., and Heyman, R. E. (2001). Categorizing partner-violent men within the reactive–proactive typology model. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 69, 567–572. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.69.3.567

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chow, J. C., Wallace, E. S., Senter, R., Kumm, S., and Mason, C. Q. (2022). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the language skills of youth offenders. J. Speech, Lang. Hear. Res. 65, 1166–1182. doi: 10.1044/2021_JSLHR-20-00308

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cima, M., and Raine, A. (2009). Distinct characteristics of psychopathy relate to different subtypes of aggression. Pers. Individ. Dif. 47, 835–840. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.031

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, R. A., Brumm, V., Zawacki, T. M., Paul, R., Sweet, L., and Rosenbaum, A. (2003). Impulsivity and verbal deficits associated with domestic violence. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 9, 760–770. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703950090

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cornet, L. J. M., Bootsman, F., Alberda, D. L., and de Kogel, C. H. (2016). “Neurowetenschappelijke toepassingen in de jeugdstrafrechtketen: Inventarisatie instrumenten, preventie en interventie,” in Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek- en Documentatiecentrum (Den Haag).

Google Scholar

Cornet, L. J. M., de Kogel, C. H., Nijman, H. L. I., Raine, A., and van der Laan, P. H. (2014). Neurobiological factors as predictors of cognitive–behavioral therapy outcome in individuals with antisocial behavior: a review of the literature. Int. J. Offender Ther. Comp. Criminol. 58, 1279–1296. doi: 10.1177/0306624X13494694

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cruz, A. R., de Castro-Rodrigues, A., and Barbosa, F. (2020). Executive dysfunction, violence and aggression. Aggress. Violent Behav. 51, 101380. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2020.101380

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalkey, N., and Helmer, O. (1963). An experimental application of the Delphi method to the use of experts. Manage. Sci. 9, 458–467. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.9.3.458

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., and Kramer, J. H. (2007a). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS): Color Word Interference Test. Handleiding: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., and Kramer, J. H. (2007b). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS): Tower Test. Handleiding: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., and Kramer, J. H. (2007c). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS): Trail Making Test. Handleiding: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., and Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) [Database record]. APA PsycTests. doi: 10.1037/t15082-000

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duckworth, A. L., and Kern, M. L. (2011). A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. J. Res. Pers. 45, 259–268. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Estrada, S., Tillem, S., Stuppy-Sullivan, A., and Baskin-Sommers, A. (2019). “Specifying the connection between reward processing and antisocial psychopathology across development: review, integration, and future directions,” in The Oxford Handbook of Positive Emotion and Psychopathology, ed. J. Gruber (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 311–332.

Google Scholar

Farrer, T. J., and Hedges, D. W. (2011). Prevalence of traumatic brain injury in incarcerated groups compared to the general population: a meta-analysis. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiat. 35, 390–394. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.01.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fayyad, J., Sampson, N. A., Hwang, I., Adamowski, T., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Al-Hamzawi, A., et al. (2017). The descriptive epidemiology of DSM-IV Adult ADHD in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. ADHD Attent. Deficit Hyperact. Disord. 9, 47–65. doi: 10.1007/s12402-016-0208-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fishbein, D., Sheppard, M., Hyde, C., Hubal, R., Newlin, D., Serin, R., et al. (2009). Deficits in behavioral inhibition predict treatment engagement in prison inmates. Law Hum. Behav. 33, 419–435. doi: 10.1007/s10979-008-9163-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frost, R. B., Farrer, T. J., Primosch, M., and Hedges, D. W. (2013). Prevalence of traumatic brain injury in the general adult population: a meta-analysis. Neuroepidemiology 40, 154–159. doi: 10.1159/000343275

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ginsberg, Y., Hirvikoski, T., and Lindefors, N. (2010). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) among longer-term prison inmates is a prevalent, persistent and disabling disorder. BMC Psychiatry 10, 112. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-10-112

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Golden, C. J., and Freshwater, S. M. (2002). Stroop Color and Word Test: Revised Examiner's Manual. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting Co.

Google Scholar

Gregory, C., Lough, S., Stone, V., Erzinclioglu, S., Martin, L., Baron-Cohen, S., et al. (2002). Theory of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease: theoretical and practical implications. Brain 125, 752–764. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf079

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guilmette, T. J., Sweet, J. J., Hebben, N., Koltai, D., Mahone, E. M., Spiegler, B. J., et al. (2020). American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology consensus conference statement on uniform labeling of performance test scores. Clin. Neuropsychol. 34, 437–453. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2020.1722244

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haarsma, G., Davenport, S., White, D. C., Ormachea, P. A., Sheena, E., and Eagleman, D. M. (2020). Assessing risk among correctional community probation populations: predicting reoffense with mobile neurocognitive assessment software. Front. Psychol. 10, 2926. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02926

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Habibi, A., Sarafrazi, A., and Izadyar, S. (2014). Delphi technique theoretical framework in qualitative research. Int. J. Eng. Sci. 3, 8–13.

Google Scholar

Happé, F. G. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 129–154. doi: 10.1007/BF02172093

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, J. C. (2006). Intellectual Disability: Understanding its Development, Causes, Classification, Evaluation, and Treatment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., and Curtis, G. (1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) Manual, Revised and Expanded. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Google Scholar

Hellenbach, M., Karatzias, T., and Brown, M. (2017). Intellectual disabilities among prisoners: prevalence and mental and physical health comorbidities. J. Appl. Res. Intellect. Disabil. 30:230–241. doi: 10.1111/jar.12234

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hetland, H., Eikeland, O.-J., Manger, T., Diseth, Å., and Asbjørnsen, A. (2007). Educational background in a prison population. J. Correct. Educ. 58, 145–156.

Google Scholar

Hilsenroth, M. J., and Segal, D. L. (2004). Comprehensive Handbook of Psychological Assessment: Volume 2 Personality Assessment. New York: John Wiley and Sons.

Google Scholar

Humes, G. E., Welsh, M. C., Retzlaff, P., and Cookson, N. (1997). Towers of Hanoi and London: reliability and validity of two executive function tasks. Assessment 4, 249–257. doi: 10.1177/107319119700400305

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutten, J. C., Ziermans, T. B., Geurts, H. M. G., and Van Horn, J. E. (preprint). Cognitive functioning in offenders compared to non-offending controls: a multi-level meta-analysis. [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/s48rn

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., et al. (2010). Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiat. 167 748–751. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iqbal, S., and Pipon-Young, l. (2009). The Delphi Method. Psychol. Methods 22.

Google Scholar

Jorm, A. F. (2015). Using the Delphi expert consensus method in mental health research. Aust. New Zeal. J. Psychiat. 49, 887–897. doi: 10.1177/0004867415600891

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Karoglu, N., Ferguson, H. J., and ÓCiardha, C. (2022). Theory of mind in offending: a systematic review. Trauma Viol. Abuse, 23, 1610–1628. doi: 10.1177/15248380211013143

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasten, E., Barbosa, F., Kosmidis, M. H., Persson, B. A., Constantinou, M., Baker, G. A., et al. (2021). European clinical neuropsychology: role in healthcare and access to neuropsychological services. Healthcare 9, 734. doi: 10.3390/healthcare9060734

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kempes, M., Berends, I., Duits, N., and Brink, W. V. D. (2019). Neurobiological information and consideration in dutch pre-trial forensic reports of juvenile criminal offenders. Int. J. Forensic Ment. Health 18, 212–219. doi: 10.1080/14999013.2018.1525776

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessels, R. P. C., Montagne, B., Hendriks, A. W., Perrett, D. I., and De Haan, E. H. F. (2014). Assessment of perception of morphed facial expressions using the Emotion Recognition Task: Normative data from healthy participants aged 8-75. J. Neuropsychol. 8, 75–93. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosmidis, M. H. (2018). Challenges in the neuropsychological assessment of illiterate older adults. Lang. Cognit. Neurosci. 33, 373–386. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2017.1379605

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Köstering, L., Schmidt, C. S. M., Egger, K., Amtage, F., Peter, J., Klöppel, S., et al. (2015). Assessment of planning performance in clinical samples: Reliability and validity of the Tower of London task (TOL-F). Neuropsychologia 75, 646–655. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.07.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krahé, B. (2013). The Social Psychology of Aggression: 2nd Edition (0 ed.). New York: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Kuhlmann, B., and Margraf, J. (2023). A new short version of the Facial Expressions of Emotions: Stimuli and Test (FEEST) including prototype and morphed emotional stimuli. Front. Psychol. 14, 1198386. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1198386

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuin, N., Masthoff, E., Kramer, M., and Scherder, E. (2015). The role of risky decision-making in aggression: a systematic review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 25, 159–172. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2015.07.018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Langton, L., Truman, J., and BJS Statisticians. (2014). Socio-Emotional Impact of Violent Crime (NCJ 247076). Washington, D. C.: U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, Bureau of Justice Statistics.

Google Scholar

Lawrence, A. J., Luty, J., Bogdan, N. A., Sahakian, B. J., and Clark, L. (2009). Problem gamblers share deficits in impulsive decision-making with alcohol-dependent individuals. Addiction 104, 1006–1015. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02533.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lezak, M. D. (Ed.). (2012). Neuropsychological assessment (5th ed). Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Li, F., Li, X., and Kou, H. (2023). Emotional recognition training enhances attention to emotional stimuli among male juvenile delinquents. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manage. 16, 575–586. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S403512

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lilienfeld, S. O., Wood, J. M., and Garb, H. N. (2000). The Scientific Status of Projective Techniques. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 1, 27–66. doi: 10.1111/1529-1006.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Linstone, H. A., and Turoff, M. (1975). The Delphi Method: Techniques and Applications. Boston: Addison-Wesley Publishing.

Google Scholar

Logan, G. D. (1994). “On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A user's guide to the stop signal paradigm,” in Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory, and Language, ed. D. Dagenbach and T. H. Carr (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 189–239.

Google Scholar

Manning, K. E. (2020). Seeing red? A systematic review of the evidence for attentional biases to threat-relevant stimuli in propensity to reactive aggression. Aggress. Violent Behav. 50, 101359. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2019.101359

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, A. A., and Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: A meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 454–465. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.08.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matheson, F. I., Dastoori, P., Whittingham, L., Calzavara, A., Keown, L. A., Durbin, A., et al. (2022). Intellectual/developmental disabilities among people incarcerated in federal correctional facilities in Ontario, Canada: examining prevalence, health and correctional characteristics. J. Appl. Res. Intellect. Disabil. 35, 900–909. doi: 10.1111/jar.12995

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McMurran, M., Egan, V., Richardson, C., Street, H., Ahmadi, S., and Cooper, G. (2000). Referrals for anger and aggression in forensic psychology outpatient services. J. Forens. Psychiat. 11, 206–213. doi: 10.1080/095851800362481

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Merk, W., Orobio De Castro, B., Koops, W., and Matthys, W. (2005). The distinction between reactive and proactive aggression: utility for theory, diagnosis and treatment? Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2, 197–220. doi: 10.1080/17405620444000300

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyers, J. E., and Meyers, K. R. (1995). Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial: Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Google Scholar

Monahan, J., and Skeem, J. L. (2014). Risk Redux: the resurgence of risk assessment in criminal sanctioning. Federal Sentenc. Report. 26, 158–166. doi: 10.1525/fsr.2014.26.3.158

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Montagne, B., Kessels, R. P. C., De Haan, E. H. F., and Perrett, D. I. (2007). The emotion recognition task: a paradigm to measure the perception of facial emotional expressions at different intensities. Percept. Mot. Skills 104, 589–598. doi: 10.2466/pms.104.2.589-598

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, C. D., and Murray, H. A. (1935). A method for investigating fantasies: the thematic apperception test. Arch. Neurol. Psychiat. 34, 289. doi: 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1935.02250200049005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Muñoz García-Largo, L., Martí-Agustí, G., Martin-Fumadó, C., and Gómez-Durán, E. L. (2020). Intellectual disability rates among male prison inmates. Int. J. Law Psychiat. 70, 101566. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2020.101566

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

National Institute of Mental Health (2023). RDoC Matrix. National Institute of Mental Health. Available online at: https://www.nimh.nih.gov/research/research-funded-by-nimh/rdoc/constructs/rdoc-matrix (accessed February 9, 2024).

Google Scholar

Nauta-Jansen, L. M. C. (2022). The neurobiology of antisocial behavior in adolescence; current knowledge and relevance for youth forensic clinical practice. Curr. Opini. Psychol. 47, 101356. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101356

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

NIP (n.d.). COTAN Documentatie. Available online at: https://www.cotandocumentatie.nl/beoordelingen/ (accessed September 21 2023).

Google Scholar

Ogilvie, J. M., Stewart, A. L., Chan, R. C. K., and Shum, D. H. K. (2011). Neuropsychological measures of executive function and antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis. Criminology 49, 1063–1107. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2011.00252.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogonah, M. G. T., Seyedsalehi, A., Whiting, D., and Fazel, S. (2023). Violence risk assessment instruments in forensic psychiatric populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiat. 10, 780–789. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(23)00256-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Osterhaus, C., and Bosacki, S. L. (2022). Looking for the lighthouse: a systematic review of advanced theory-of-mind tests beyond preschool. Dev. Rev. 64, 101021. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2022.101021

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Papalia, N., Spivak, B., Daffern, M., and Ogloff, J. R. P. (2019). A meta-analytic review of the efficacy of psychological treatments for violent offenders in correctional and forensic mental health settings. Clin. Psychol.: Sci. Pract. 26, e12282. doi: 10.1111/cpsp.12282

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parrott, D. J., and Giancola, P. R. (2007). Addressing “The criterion problem” in the assessment of aggressive behavior: Development of a new taxonomic system. Aggress. Violent Behav. 12, 280–299. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2006.08.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, D. M., and Taylor, R. M. (2012). Social and Economic Costs of Violence: Workshop Summary. Washington, D.C.: National Academies Press, 13254.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Pearson NL (2012). WAIS-IV-NL Psychometrische Eigenschappen. Boston: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Qualtrics (2023). Qualtrics Software. Available online at: https://www.qualtrics.com (accessed February 9, 2024).

Google Scholar

Raskin, M. (1994). The Delphi study in-field instruction revisited—expert consensus on issues and research priorities. J. Soc. Work Educ., 30: 75–89. doi: 10.1080/10437797.1994.10672215

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayens, M. K., and Hahn, E. J. (2000). Building consensus using the policy Delphi method. Policy, Polit. Nursing Pract. 1, 308–315. doi: 10.1177/152715440000100409

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivara, F., Adhia, A., Lyons, V., Massey, A., Mills, B., Morgan, E., et al. (2019). The effects of violence on health. Health Aff. 38, 1622–1629. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2019.00480

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, R. (1999). Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 20, 322–339. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00091-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, E. H., and Hoaken, P. N. S. (2011). Executive cognitive functioning abilities of male first time and return canadian federal inmates. Can. J. Criminol. Crimi. Just. 53, 377–403. doi: 10.3138/cjccj.53.4.377

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sánchez de Ribera, O., Trajtenberg, N., and Cook, S. (2022). Executive functioning among first time and recidivist inmates in Uruguay. Appl. Neuropsychol.: Adult 29, 1242–1249. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2020.1864634

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitz, F., Kunina-Habenicht, O., Hildebrandt, A., Oberauer, K., and Wilhelm, O. (2020). Psychometrics of the Iowa and Berlin gambling tasks: unresolved issues with reliability and validity for risk taking. Assessment 27, 232–245. doi: 10.1177/1073191117750470

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, K., Fan, J., Magidina, O., Marks, D., Hahn, B., and Halperin, J. (2007). Does the emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition? Convergence with measures on a non-emotional analog. Arch. Clini. Neuropsychol. 22, 151–160. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.12.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Serafim, A. D. P., Saffi, F., Silva, T. G. B. D., Almeida, C. V. D., Hokama, E., Barros, D. M. D., et al. (2015). Forensic neuropsychological assessment: a review of its scope. Arch. Clini. Psychiat. 42, 63–67. doi: 10.1590/0101-60830000000049

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shumlich, E. J., Reid, G. J., Hancock, M., and Hoaken, P. (2019). Executive dysfunction in criminal populations: comparing forensic psychiatric patients and correctional offenders. Int. J. Forensic Ment. Health 18, 243–259. doi: 10.1080/14999013.2018.1495279

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shunk, A. W., Davis, A. S., and Dean, R. S. (2006). Test review: Dean C. Delis, Edith Kaplan and Joel H. Kramer, Delis Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS), The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX, 2001. $415.00 (complete kit). Appl. Neuropsychol. 13: 275–27. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1304_9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, L. M. J. (1997). Do criminal offenders specialize in crime types? Appl. Prevent. Psychol. 6, 35–53. doi: 10.1016/S0962-1849(05)80064-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smeijers, D. (2017). Severe Aggressive Behavior. Towards Understanding some of the Underlying Characteristics and Treatment Responsivity. Available online at: http://rgdoi.net/10.13140/RG.2.2.12181.42728

Google Scholar

Söderstrand, P., and Almkvist, O. (2012). Psychometric data on the eyes test, the faux pas test, and the dewey social stories test in a population-based swedish adult sample. Nordic Psychology 64, 30–43. doi: 10.1080/19012276.2012.693729

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soothill, K., Francis, B., Sanderson, B., and Ackerley, E. (2000). Sex offenders: specialists, generalists—or both? Br. J. Criminol. 40, 56–67. doi: 10.1093/bjc/40.1.56

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steward, K. A., and Kretzmer, T. (2022). Anosognosia in moderate-to-severe traumatic brain injury: a review of prevalence, clinical correlates, and diversity considerations. Clin. Neuropsychol. 36, 2021–2040. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2021.1967452

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., and Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 640–656. doi: 10.1162/089892998562942

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M., and Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary, Third Edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Sullivan, C. J., Mcgloin, J. M., Pratt, T. C., and Piquero, A. R. (2006). Rethinking the “norm” of offender generality: investigating specialization in the short-term. Criminology 44, 199–233. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2006.00047.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuominen, T., Korhonen, T., Hamalainen, H., Temonen, S., Salo, H., Katajisto, J., et al. (2014). Functional illiteracy and neurocognitive deficits among male prisoners: implications for rehabilitation. J. Foren. Pract. 16, 268–280. doi: 10.1108/JFP-10-2013-0044

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turoff, M. (1975). “The policy Delphi,” in The Delphi Method. Techniques and Applications, eds H. A. Linstone, and M. Turoff (London: Addison-Wesley), 84–101.

Google Scholar

Van De Kant, T., Boers, S. F., Kempes, M., and Egger, J. I. M. (2020). Neuropsychological subtypes of violent behaviour: differences in inhibition between affective and instrumental violence. J. Foren. Psychol. 5, 7. doi: 10.35248/2475-319X.19.5.158

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Venturi Da Silva, L., and Cavalheiro Hamdan, A. (2022). Neuropsychological assessment in the forensic context: a scoping review. Brazil. J. Foren. Sci. Med. Law Bioethics 12, 53–74. doi: 10.17063/bjfs12(1)y202253-74

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Völlm, B. A., Edworthy, R., Huband, N., Talbot, E., Majid, S., Holley, J., et al. (2018). Characteristics and pathways of long-stay patients in high and medium secure settings in england; a secondary publication from a large mixed-methods study. Front. Psychiatry 9, 140. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00140

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1997). WMS-III Administration and Scoring Manual. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (2012). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition. London, UK: Pearson.

Google Scholar

White, B. A., Gordon, H., and Guerra, R. C. (2015). Callous–unemotional traits and empathy in proactive and reactive relational aggression in young women. Pers. Individ. Dif. 75, 185–189. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.031

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wigham, S., McGovern, R., Kaner, E., and Hackett, S. S. (2022). A review of recent innovation in psychosocial interventions for reducing violence and aggression in adults using a horizon scanning approach. Aggress. Violent Behav. 62, 101685. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2021.101685

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Willner, P., Bailey, R., Parry, R., and Dymond, S. (2010). Evaluation of executive functioning in people with intellectual disabilities. J. Intell. Disab. Res. 54, 366–379. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01249.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, A., Perrett, D., Calder, A., Sprengelmeyer, R., and Ekman, P. (2002). Facial Expressions of Emotion: Stimuli and Tests (FEEST). Suffolk: Thames Valley Test Company.

Google Scholar

Yukhnenko, D., Farouki, L., and Fazel, S. (2023). Criminal recidivism rates globally: A 6-year systematic review update. J. Crim. Justice 88, 102115. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2023.102115

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Z., Wang, Q., Liu, X., Song, P., and Yang, B. (2017). Differences in inhibitory control between impulsive and premeditated aggression in juvenile inmates. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11, 373. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00373

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zijlmans, J., Marhe, R., Bevaart, F., Van Duin, L., Luijks, M.-J. A., Franken, I., et al. (2021). The predictive value of neurobiological measures for recidivism in delinquent male young adults. J. Psychiat. Neurosci. 46, 10. doi: 10.1503/jpn.200103

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: neuropsychological tests, forensic psychology, aggression, violence, Delphi technique, Research Domain Criteria (RDoC)

Citation: Hutten JC, van Horn JE, Hoppenbrouwers SS, Ziermans TB, Geurts HM and the Forensic Neuropsychology Consortium (2024) Neuropsychological assessment of aggressive offenders: a Delphi consensus study. Front. Psychol. 15:1328839. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1328839

Received: 27 October 2023; Accepted: 05 February 2024;
Published: 23 February 2024.

Edited by:

Heng Choon Chan, University of Birmingham, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Alexander Hochdorn, University of Brasilia, Brazil
Ezequiel Norberto Mercurio, University of Buenos Aires, Argentina

Copyright © 2024 Hutten, van Horn, Hoppenbrouwers, Ziermans, Geurts and the Forensic Neuropsychology Consortium. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Juliette C. Hutten, jhutten@dewaagnederland.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.