- 1Faculty of Policy Studies, Aichi Gakuin University, Nisshin, Japan
- 2Graduate School of Education, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
- 3Research Center for Advanced Science and Technology, the University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
The issue of Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) samples dominating research has been ongoing for decades, and now the emerging trend is to turn to theoretical perspectives from the Majority World. Adopting Western-centric methods based on reductionism can overlook important details and differences between similar cultures, particularly in East Asian cultures, where the Confucian values of relational harmony take many forms. We discuss a novel theoretical perspective on moral constitutions in Confucian heritage cultures. Our central tenet is that divergent moral concepts and ideals are present in Confucian cultures because each culture emphasizes a specific pillar and the self is situated differently in the social relationships that define the scope of interpersonal moral obligations. We consider three Confucian countries: China, Japan, and South Korea as examples. Despite geographical proximity and conventional categorization in cultural psychology, each Confucian country manifests distinct patterns of the self, moral ideals, and behavior in socio-moral contexts. To understand how and why moral worldviews vary within a region, we need to examine how the self in socio-cultural contexts differs and guides interpersonal norms and behaviors across sociocultural contexts. We conclude this paper by offering methodological recommendations for including indigenous moral concepts outside the WEIRD context.
1 Introduction
The nearly exclusive use of Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) samples has been criticized over the last few decades (Arnett, 2008; Henrich et al., 2010). Although many psychologists are aware of this, as evidenced by articles raising this issue having been published in major journals and cited more than 1,000 times, Western hegemony continues to thrive in the psychological field, while non-Western psychology is left behind (Thalmayer et al., 2021). Some methodological approaches focus on identifying universal phenomena (Norenzayan and Heine, 2005); however, non-WEIRD psychology has been missing from mainstream research. Despite the pluralistic stance in dominant theories of moral psychology (Graham et al., 2011; Shweder et al., 1997), Western and non-Western moral psychologists tend to focus on comparing universal moral concepts, reinforcing the prominence of WEIRD psychology while obscuring nuanced cultural differences. This long-standing practice will persist unless novel perspectives are included.
Morality is a sociopsychological topic that has been heavily influenced by Western philosophy. Abstract moral issues and empirical methods have been derived from the moral understanding of researchers in WEIRD cultural contexts (Ellemers et al., 2019; Snarey, 1985). Over the past few decades, a prominent approach in moral psychology has focused on explaining human morality by identifying common patterns and concepts across cultures. While it unifies moral values across borders, one concern is that emphasizing cultural similarities may obscure indigenous views of morality (Obeid et al., 2017). WEIRD people may represent approximately 10–12% of the world’s population, but the rest are simply referred to as a uniform group, such as non-WEIRD or the Majority world. In particular, Eastern perspectives tend to be homogenized in terms of collectivism and interdependence. Moreover, understanding how various moral worldviews prevail within sociocultural contexts is crucial for giving equal weights on the perspectives because intergroup conflicts can arise from perceived dissimilarities in moral ideals (Leach et al., 2007).
As we will see in this paper, East Asian perspectives can provide new insights into variations in the collectivist self and their implications for morality. Western philosophy conceptualizes the self as a separate entity, but morality guides how people interact with others (Rai and Fiske, 2011). Unlike Western emphasis on the self, Eastern emphasis on the interaction between the self and others can explain how moral phenomena are intertwined in the different relational contexts. We posit that, while universal principles exist among all humankind, indigenous standards should be recognized and respected in the study of morality. A critical examination of the sociocultural roots of the people who abide by such standards is essential for understanding morality within a specific context (Rai and Fiske, 2011), and this may only be gained through the lens of the very people who possess implicit knowledge of their norms, values, and ways of life.
1.1 Overview
In this study, we draw on indigenous moral concepts derived from Confucianism in China, Japan, and South Korea to examine the convergence and divergence of moral views within a region that shares a common philosophical background. We will first discuss how and why the traditional assumptions in moral psychology that have driven research may not be applicable to non-Western cultures. Next, we use China, Japan, and South Korea, as countries with Confucian heritage, to illustrate how relational contexts shape moral concepts. Studying the influence of Confucianism in East Asia shows how a philosophical doctrine has evolved to prevail as a moral guide for living a virtuous life (Kim, 2016) and at some point, diverged to produce different versions of the moral worldview (Zhang et al., 2005). Lastly, we offer methodological recommendations for studying morality in non-Western contexts. In doing so, we conclude this paper with suggestions for integrating Western and non-Western perspectives to obtain a more complete picture of human morality.
2 Part 1: assumptions in moral psychology
The cultural construal of the self provides a framework for explaining cultural variations in motivation, cognition, emotional experiences, and behavior (for review, see Kitayama and Uskul, 2011). However, little attention has been paid to the role of the self in constructing socio-moral experiences. In this section, we present as examples two assumptions that have directed research in moral psychology and explain why these assumptions should consider the self as socially constructed to address moral complexity.
2.1 Assumption 1: autonomous thinkers/doers
Western traditions of ethical theory assume that autonomous individuals make moral judgments with the intention of being just (Simpson, 1974). According to Hobbes, moral rules are those that rational, self-interested people would agree to, and those that work to prevent a state of disorganization (Laskar, 2013). In moral judgment tasks, the most common research tool in moral psychology (Ellemers et al., 2019), the researcher assumes that participants make judgments based on their moral sense (Kohlberg, 1984). However, in non-Western cultures, moral judgments may not be private in many instances because the perceived consensus within the larger group may influence individual judgments and behavioral choices (Eom et al., 2016; Vauclair et al., 2015). In collectivistic cultures, the primary regulators of social actions are role expectations within the group and contextual constraints (Hong et al., 1997; Morris and Peng, 1994). Furthermore, members of the community are interdependent, and morally appropriate behavior depends on the context of the relevant interpersonal relationship (Lai, 1995).
Similarly, studies on self-reported traits and moral behavior suggest that a stable moral identity guides moral cognition and behavior. Similar to other social identities, moral identity is considered a core part of an individual’s self-concept and is associated with certain beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors (Aquino and Reed, 2002). Accordingly, Western ethical theories assume that an individual should choose wisely when identifying what virtue outweighs the others in a conflicting situation (Kraut, 2022). In contrast, in non-Western cultures, people may make a moral choice that violates a moral principle with little conflict while still fulfilling interpersonal moral obligations (Miller and Bersoff, 1992). This tolerance for conflicting values in moral choices may be related to the dialectical thinking dominant among people in East Asians and those with East Asian cultural backgrounds. Members of dialectical cultures exhibit more inconsistencies in their self-concept than Euro-Americans (Spencer-Rodgers et al., 2009). Western ethical theories suggest that virtues may conflict in certain situations as individuals strive to stay consistent with their upheld value. In comparison, in non-Western contexts one virtue related to interpersonal responsibilities may override the others (Qingping, 2007). Moreover, people in interdependent cultures have a large set of moral attributes to describe a moral person that can be applied to different contexts, depending on the role and social expectation (Jia et al., 2019), indicating that moral identity is not a stable concept.
2.2 Assumption 2: universal moral principles explain the most patterns of moral thinking and behavior in all cultures
Psychological studies of morality have been subject to reductionism in the quest for universal moral principles. Prominent theories, such as the social domain theory and stage theories of moral development, highlight common moral principles (e.g., justice and fairness) across cultures (Kohlberg, 1984; Turiel, 2002) while more recent theories, from a pluralistic view, suggest that moral ideals are contextualized and expressed differently in different places (Atari et al., 2023; Graham et al., 2011; Shweder et al., 1997). Our endeavor to draw attention to moral diversity also resonates with moral pluralist (Cassaniti and Hickman, 2014) and moral relationalist perspectives (Mascolo and Fasoli, 2020). Although we are concerned about reductionism in morality studies, this is not to say that the observed universal moral concepts are not relevant in non-Western cultures. Nor does the dominant theory neglect moral diversity. Our concern is that both Western and non-Western moral psychologists tend to focus their attention away from the notion that moral concept evolve from social interactions (Mascolo and Fasoli, 2020) and may include those specific to the context (Cassaniti and Hickman, 2014). Instead, they use the theories as a guide to find common patterns of moral thinking and behavior across cultures, leading to reductionism.
The moral foundation theory, for example, explicitly holds the pluralistic stance, but given the self-report questionnaire designed for measuring a unified set of moral foundations, most studies based on the theory leave culture-specific moral concepts buried without developing a culture-sensitive measure. Some studies have reported that the moral foundations are common cross-culturally; however, the relevance of some moral situations to the underlying moral foundation may vary (Doğruyol et al., 2019; Murayama and Miura, 2019). For example, the factor structure of moral foundations may be different in a non-Christian cultural contexts (Akhtar et al., 2023) or cultures in which personal moral values are absorbed into collectives (Chung et al., 2016; Du, 2019). Furthermore, the current models may not capture the moral worldviews of ethnic minorities (Davis et al., 2016). These studies suggest that basic moral ideals may be conceptualized differently across cultures; thus, the assumption that universal moral principles should suffice for human morality may oversimplify the experiences and moral concepts of people outside a Western context.
3 Part 2: Confucianism
Confucius (551–479 B.C.) sought to restore social order and harmony by reviving traditions of ancestor worship in China, filial piety, and a harmonious feudal society with hierarchical ranks, viewing himself as a successor to antiquity (Nakamura, 1991). Confucianism quickly attracted those in high positions who sought to stabilize their sovereign power. This later became recognized as a school of philosophy that spread throughout China as a powerful force for shaping mentalities and guiding ethical behavior. Confucianism was then imported to Korea and then to Japan around the fourth or fifth century. Although Confucianism was never formally recognized as a religion, Confucian moral ideals have influenced much of the moral education and social life in those heritage countries (Wang, 2004).
Although each pillar of Confucianism is weighted differently, the Chinese, Japanese, and South Korean mentalities are deeply rooted in Confucianism. Song, Yuan and Ming dynasties in China, Tokugawa Shogunate in Japan, and Yi dynasty in Korea adopted Confucianism for hundreds of years. Long before the formal education system was modernized by Western influence, textbooks in the three countries were imbued with Confucian teachings (Stowell, 2003). The common themes revolved around social relations. In contrast to the Western approaches to morality that inflate selfhood, personal cultivation in Confucianism means blurring the boundary between the self and others and cultivating social relationships (Sigurðsson, 2018). Today, Confucian ideals of filial piety, harmony, benevolence, and social hierarchy still exert a strong moral influence (Zhang et al., 2005).
In the era of modernization, each heritage country has undergone economic and socio-political reforms, and in recent decades, East Asian individualism–collectivism orientations have shifted dramatically. More people in East Asia are adopting individualistic values as evidenced by their lifestyles (increased divorce rates, rural–urban migration, more freedom in personal choice; Hamamura, 2012; Steele and Lynch, 2013). However, increased autonomy, life choices, and residential mobility do not necessarily indicate that individualism has conquered collectivism. Similarly, Confucianism has been central to moral worldviews. In Japan, for example, no single religion has bound the people for centuries, but a constellation of Confucianism, Shinto, and the Zen sect make up the Japanese mentality, as seen in Bushido: The Soul of Japan.
3.1 Divergence in moral thinking and ideals in Confucian heritage countries
Today, the Confucian ways of thinking continue to flourish in the heritage countries (Hofstede and Bond, 1988), but influence moral cognition and behavior differently, depending on which Confucian value orientation is emphasized in a culture (Chen, 2018). We propose that, within East Asian cultures, the self is situated differently in social relationships and that the extent to which people show moral consideration for others depends on the representation of the self in a given context. Social norms that govern self-other relations, emotions, and communication styles differ among East Asian cultures (Park and Han, 2018), as the ways in which people organize their moral worlds may also differ (Wenxue, 2020). In Confucian heritage countries, harmonious relationships based on trust and mutual obligations to protect one another’s welfare are ubiquitous in daily life, and without explicit transmission from one generation to the next, Confucian virtues are shared among lay people (Lai, 1995). However, people in different East Asian regions differ in how they interpret and emphasize Confucian ethical values (for review, see Chen, 2018; Tamai and Lee, 2002; Zhang et al., 2005). We argue that the divergence originates from the way in which the self is situated in the context, specifically in relation to others.
3.2 The self and moral concepts in Confucian-heritage countries: China, Japan, and South Korea
3.2.1 China: lesser self to a greater self and filial piety
Despite the New Culture Movement in the 1910s that banned Confucianism, at the center of Chinese socio-moral, life is filial piety (xiao), which binds not only the self and family members, but also the self and others in a close circle (Wenxue, 2020; Yue and Ng, 1999). According to Confucian teachings, people who practice ren (benevolence) must love all human beings, and this practice should begin with their parents (Li, 1997, 2008). Originally, filial obligation defined a narrow scope of moral responsibilities, such as caring for one’s parents, showing respect to the elderly, and endorsing the decisions and actions of one’s elders (for a review see, Yeh and Bedford, 2020). Over time, the scope of filial piety has been expanded from parents to others with varying degrees of intimacy, such as friends, neighbors, and colleagues.
The Chinese self-identity is metaphorically viewed as the subordination of a lesser self (xiao wo) to a greater self (da wo), indicating that the individual (lesser self) encompasses the motivations, interests, and behavioral patterns of significant others (greater self), particularly in family relationships (Barbalet, 2014). This relationship between the self and others is specific to the Chinese culture. The Chinese collective mentality is centered on the Confucian ethic of filial piety (xiao), whereas Japanese collective behavior is consistent with loyalty (zhong) to group members, another Confucian pillar (Fukuyama, 1995). In close relationships, Chinese students treat friends as family members and are less concerned about being polite and causing trouble than Taiwanese and Japanese students (Uehara et al., 2011).
Confucian justice is based on filial piety, which prescribes the virtue of respecting parents, elders, and ancestors. For outsiders, Chinese filial piety seems to override the moral principle of “justice.” Rather, the Confucian view of justice is based on rules of social exchange that differ according to the relational status and position (inferior/superior) of the individual with respect to the target person (Hwang, 1999). A study shows that, unlike U.S. students, Chinese college students made harsh judgments about illegal conducts done by strangers, but were forgiving of misconducts by their parents (Study 3, Hwang, 2006). A son should hide his father’s misconduct out of filial love even if it means lying and violating the moral principle of justice (Qingping, 2007). The Confucian concept of yi (justice) does not align with Western ethical philosophy. In Confucian ethics, members of the society should support close allies, just as filial piety favors close others. In resource allocation, equality means that one should favor the intimate (Hwang, 1999). Unlike Western notions of justice, which use “all things considered” to indicate that all alternatives for individual rights have been weighed, Confucian ethics addresses the concerns of significant others because the lesser self shares experiences and intentions of significant others.
3.2.2 Japan: public and private self
The Japanese concept of wa (interpersonal harmony) is reminiscent of Confucian teachings; however, harmony is maintained not only among friends and family members but also among strangers in public. According to the Japanese tradition of dividing the self into private (uchi, in-group) and public (soto, out-group), maintaining harmony means different things for in-groups and out-groups (Sugiyama-Libra, 1978). With respect to the private self, not all ingroup members fall into this category. For the most intimate, Japanese expect one another to freely express their feelings, intentions, and desires (Midooka, 1990). Compared to other Confucian countries, Japanese people define a narrow circle of intimate (uchi) others to whom they apply filial piety. At this point, some may wonder what is characteristic of Japanese Confucianism. To search for uniquely Japanese moral concepts, one should focus on social contexts in which the interaction partner is an outgroup (soto) member or an ingroup (uchi) member with low intimacy, which we discuss below.
Japanese feel uncertain about other people’s inner thoughts unless the interaction partner is the most intimate, and in such situations, the public self drives self-restrained behavior for the collective goal of maintaining peace. Public spaces apply to any situation in which less intimate others are present. Japanese parents teach their children to be respectful of others and obedient in public, and causing inconvenience or annoyance to strangers in public is considered morally unacceptable (Honda et al., 2017; Takagi, 2013). Japanese college students consider it important to exercise self-control so as not to cause annoyance or discomfort to others in public, whereas Korean students are more concerned with protecting autonomous rights, which is a universal moral value (Jeong, 2020). In public spaces, concern for maintaining the social order and peace guides self-restrained behavior (Yamamoto, 1990).
This concern for public peace may not be equal to a genuine concern for the welfare of others. In particular, the Japanese culture reflects a narrow definition of intimacy, and most social relationships fall into out-groups of varying levels of closeness (Midooka, 1990). The public self regulates collective behavior to maintain order; however, reaching out to help others is not morally obligatory. Among 142 countries, Japan ranked lowest in helping a stranger (Charities Aid Foundation, 2023). For the Japanese, interaction with soto others is ideally kept to a minimum because strangers expect one another not to invade their comfort zone. Consequently, Japanese people feel less obligated to help low-intimacy others as long as the public peace is maintained. That is, keeping peace means not interfering in other people’s problems. This behavioral strategy can turn the individual into a bystander. Without understanding the public self, the Japanese may be perceived as distant, rigid, or even cold (Yamamoto, 1990).
3.2.3 South Korea: the influence of familism and social hierarchy roles
South Korea presents a distinctive case for examining the influence of Confucianism on morality, showcasing both continuity and adaptation. The Korean Peninsular has often been described by the general populace and academic scholars as the “most Confucian part of the world,” as it effectively utilizes ancient traditions for the purpose of national development (Koh, 1996, p. 191; Sleziak, 2013). The deep-rooted appreciation for moral values and the endeavor to foster a moral society permeate the educational system, starting from primary school. Here, ethics lessons emphasize hierarchical human relationships, teaching respect for elders, parents, and teachers, as well as care for younger siblings for harmonious community life (Kim, 2014). The role of Confucian ethics is pivotal in shaping perspectives on interpersonal relationships and the dynamics between individuals and society (Moon, 1995).
Geographically positioned between the Chinese continent and the Japanese islands, Korea exhibits a unique amalgamation of mentalities that highlight both filial piety (hyo) and loyalty (chung). Korean morality shares similarities with Chinese principles, emphasizing interpersonal commitments and an expanded form of familial bonding (Park and Han, 2018). Similar to findings in Chinese studies (Hwang, 2006), the moral judgments of Korean individuals often depend on their perceived closeness to the target (Hyun et al., 2009). The prevalent concept of we-ness (woori-seong) operates within close relationships, characterized by a strong sense of bonding, unconditional friendship, mutual altruism, and exclusive favoritism (Choi, 1998). Additionally, akin to Japanese mentalities, loyalty is a fundamental aspect of we-ness. The concept of nunchi, literally translating to “eye-measure,” is a crucial skill in interpersonal contexts, maintaining both bonding and loyalty within the hierarchical structures of groups (Robertson, 2019). Unlike Japan, where a clear distinction exists between public and private spheres, in Korea, this distinction is less pronounced due to extended familism. Jeong, the sense of attachment between individuals based on reciprocal obligations and mutual care, can also be activated in public once mutual bonding is established (Choi, 2011).
Although limited, cross-cultural comparisons reveal culture-specific features of Korean familism, which is more readily activated even within non-familial relationships compared to its counterparts in Chile and France (Hur et al., 2016). In these countries, familism is typically confined to kinship relationships (Valdivieso-Mora et al., 2016). Likewise, familism in South East Asians (e.g., the Philippines) tends to emphasize more traditional values than the Korean familism (Choi et al., 2018). Literature further suggests that the extended form of Korean collectivism differs distinctly from the collectivism found in other East Asian countries (Choi, 1993; Han, 2017). In contrast to the Chinese and Japanese constructs of self, the Korean self is more agentic, aiming to achieve interpersonal harmony by integrating individual selves into a cohesive whole in social contexts (Inumiya et al., 2007). Supported by the strong influence of Confucian disciplines that emphasize human relationships within hierarchical frameworks, Korean familism nurtures the collective as a cohesive and integrative family-like entity.
3.3 Moral disagreement in Confucian countries
Because morality guides how people evaluate others (van der Lee et al., 2017), the real and perceived moral variations within a geographical region may lead to moral dissent. In everyday contexts, perceived and real differences in moral worldviews can impede intercultural exchange. Qingping (2007) argues that Confucian ethics in China prioritize virtues related to family ties over society. This tends to be similar in Korea where the Confucian ethics has been evolved around the family-oriented viewpoint (Kim, 2020). On the other hand, Japanese Confucian ethics focus on public behavior. Japanese people perceive Chinese tourists as being inconsiderate of others because the importance of maintaining public peace differs between the two cultures. However, Chinese students would likely find the Japanese friendship style reserved, distant, and cold because they extend the principle of filial piety (originating from ren) to friends, whom they do not consider separate from family members (Uehara et al., 2011). Research on South Korean foreign residents in Japan report similar impression of Japanese classmates or co-workers to the Chinese (Lee et al., 2016). Unlike in China and South Korea, ren did not prevail in the Japanese mentality where loyalty to the group triumphed (Chen, 2018). Loyalty may be expressed through self-restraint behaviors, such as disguising one’s emotions or intentions by conforming to the group (Honda et al., 2017). International students in Japan (63.9% from China in the cited study) would find it incomprehensible to exercise restraint in close relationships until they are sufficiently acculturated and can understand the moral meaning of self-control within the host culture (Takamatsu et al., 2021).
4 Part 3: recommendations
Morality can be studied from numerous perspectives (Haidt, 2008; Simpson, 1974), implying that any perspectives can be biased if researchers examine an issue solely through their own moral lens. Adopting reductionist research methods can overlook important differences between seemingly similar cultures, particularly those in East Asian cultures, where individuals are more likely to respond to a single context in different ways (Peng and Nisbett, 1999). To show how and why moral worldviews differ among people who are traditionally grouped in the same moral category, we examined how in Confucian cultures (China, Japan, and South Korea) that value interpersonal harmony, the self is situated differently in context, and the scope and content of interpersonal moral responsibilities differ significantly. The same could be said for the other regions of the world, as those in East Asians are not the only non-Western countries (e.g., Cohen et al., 1996). Thus, we provide several recommendations for future studies that aim to elucidate moral diversity among non-WEIRD populations.
To avoid oversimplifying the psychology of people living in non-WEIRD countries, cross-cultural projects must carefully define cultural groups and outline Western and non-Western perspectives at the outset. A minimum requirement for conducting cross-cultural research is to include, in addition to some Western countries, at least several non-Western and non-industrialized countries that do not share a common philosophical, linguistic, and cultural background (Snarey, 1985) for multiple comparisons (Krys et al., 2024). Our examination of the role of Confucianism in the moral worldviews of East Asian countries suggests that classifying cultural groups is not a simple task. Geographical proximity and philosophical background are not reliable guides for categorizing cultures in morality studies (Du, 2019). To categorize cultures, we must examine the historical and sociocultural backgrounds that construct the moral worldview of the people in that place, assuming from the moral pluralist view, that socialization and unique interpersonal experiences within a culture create moral truth (Shweder, 1984). This implies that defining a moral issue involves a conceptual analysis of the issue from the perspectives of the target cultural groups because a moral principle with the same label may carry nuanced conceptual and functional differences across cultures.
Culturally unique morality would remain buried without culturally adapted conceptualizations and tools that take into account the socio-moral practices that govern the place. The pioneering work of Shweder et al. (1997) on the three ethical moral domains is based on the researchers’ ethnographic knowledge of community and family life in the state of Orissa, India, and in-depth interviews with local residents. Using the three ethical codes, Haidt et al. (1993) found that moral concerns among American adults and children and high SES groups are limited to harm and justice, whereas Brazilians and low SES groups have richer moral concepts. Similarly, Vasquez et al. (2001) found that justice was the most salient moral principle for Americans, while Filipinos used the three ethical codes almost evenly depending on the context. More recent studies have unraveled culture-specific morality in East Asian contexts. Jia and Krettenauer (2019) asked participants to describe a highly moral person and found that compared to Canadians, Chinese participants generated more moral attributes including unique descriptions of being moral in the Chinese culture. Similarly, Matsuo (2023) used the situation sampling method to identify the contexts that constitute people’s shared representations of morality and found that, unlike American descriptions of purity that reflect monotheism, Japanese descriptions of purity include some supernatural power (not necessarily an agent) as a motivating force for the moral values of cleanliness and diligence that are essential to Japanese morality. These studies have methodological implications: In order to discover culturally unique moral experiences, the researcher needs native participants to help formulate the moral constructs from the local perspective.
5 Coda
Although never organized as a religion, Confucianism has profoundly influenced political regimes, social relationships, and spirituality in the heritage countries. To delineate an indigenous moral perspective, we present China, Japan, and South Korea as examples of countries where the Confucian pillar of relational harmony has been adapted in different ways. Consequently, people within collectivist cultures vary in the scope and content of their interpersonal moral responsibilities. Understanding different or unfamiliar moral concepts requires effort, as people are typically reluctant to change their existing belief system (Hart et al., 2009). However, this is worth the effort. Without understanding the moral world in which people live, misunderstanding or perceiving others as lacking morality can lead to discrimination (Haslam and Loughnan, 2014). In conclusion, our hope is that the study of indigenous moral psychology will not only lead to the discovery of neglected values in moral psychology, but will also promote understanding among those who are divided.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Author contributions
RT: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Project administration, Conceptualization. JP: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Conceptualization. AM: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Akhtar, N., Francis, L. J., Village, A., Sailer, A. B., Hasan, S. S., and McKenna, U. (2023). Testing the moral foundations questionnaire within a Muslim society: a study among young adults in Pakistan. J. Relig. Educ. 71, 1–18. doi: 10.1007/s40839-023-00195-z
Aquino, K., and Reed, A. II. (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83, 1423–1440. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1423
Arnett, J. J. (2008). The neglected 95%: why American psychology needs to become less American. Am. Psychol. 63, 602–614. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.602
Atari, M., Haidt, J., Graham, J., Koleva, S., Stevens, S. T., and Dehghani, M. (2023). Morality beyond the WEIRD: how the nomological network of morality varies across cultures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 125, 1157–1188. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000470
Barbalet, J. (2014). Greater self, lesser self: dimensions of self-interest in Chinese filial piety. J. Theory Soc. Behav. 44, 186–205. doi: 10.1111/jtsb.12037
Cassaniti, J. L., and Hickman, J. R. (2014). New directions in the anthropology of morality. Anthropol. Theory 14, 251–262. doi: 10.1177/1463499614534371
Charities Aid Foundation (2023). World giving index 2023. Available at: https://www.cafonline.org/docs/default-source/about-us-research/wgi_report_2023_final.pdf?sfvrsn=402a5447_2 (accessed March 21, 2024)
Chen, L. (2018). “Historical and cultural features of Confucianism in East Asia” in Confucianisms for a changing world cultural order. eds. R. T. Ames and P. D. Hershock (Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press), 102–111.
Choi, S. C. (1993). Cultural sense of “we” between Koreans and Japanese. Proceedings of annual conference of Korean psychological association, 229–244
Choi, S. C. (1998). The third-person psychology and the first-person psychology: Two perspectives on human relations. Korean Social Science Journal. 25, 239–264.
Choi, Y., Kim, T. Y., Noh, S., Lee, J., and Takeuchi, D. (2018). Culture and Family Process: Measures of Familism for Filipino and Korean American Parents. Family process. 57, 1029–1048. doi: 10.1111/famp.12322
Chung, E. K., Park, S., Lee, S., and Sohn, Y. W. (2016). The application of moral foundations questionnaire for Korean adults. Korean J. Soc. Person. Psychol. 30, 47–61. doi: 10.21193/kjspp.2016.30.3.003
Cohen, D., Nisbett, R. E., Bowdle, B. F., and Schwarz, N. (1996). Insult, aggression, and the southern culture of honor: an “experimental ethnography”. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 945–960. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.945
Davis, D. E., Rice, K., Van Tongeren, D. R., Hook, J. N., DeBlaere, C., Worthington, E. L. Jr., et al. (2016). The moral foundations hypothesis does not replicate well in black samples. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 110, e23–e30. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000056
Doğruyol, B., Alper, S., and Yilmaz, O. (2019). The five-factor model of the moral foundations theory is stable across WEIRD and non-WEIRD cultures. Personal. Individ. Differ. 151:109547. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.109547
Du, J. (2019). Validation of the moral foundations questionnaire with three Chinese ethnic groups. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 47, 1–12. doi: 10.2224/sbp.8009
Ellemers, N., Van Der Toorn, J., Paunov, Y., and Van Leeuwen, T. (2019). The psychology of morality: a review and analysis of empirical studies published from 1940 through 2017. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 23, 332–366. doi: 10.1177/1088868318811759
Eom, K., Kim, H. S., Sherman, D. K., and Ishii, K. (2016). Cultural variability in the link between environmental concern and support for environmental action. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1331–1339. doi: 10.1177/0956797616660078
Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., and Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 366–385. doi: 10.1037/a0021847
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H., and Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog?. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 65, 613–628. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.4.613
Hamamura, T. (2012). Are cultures becoming individualistic? A cross-temporal comparison of individualism–collectivism in the United States and Japan. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 16, 3–24. doi: 10.1177/1088868311411587
Hart, W., Albarracín, D., Eagly, A. H., Brechan, I., Lindberg, M. J., and Merrill, L. (2009). Feeling validated versus being correct: a meta-analysis of selective exposure to information. Psychol. Bull. 135, 555–588. doi: 10.1037/a0015701
Haslam, N., and Loughnan, S. (2014). Dehumanization and infrahumanization. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65, 399–423. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115045
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature. 466:29. doi: 10.1038/466029a
Hofstede, G., and Bond, M. H. (1988). The Confucius connection: from cultural roots to economic growth. Organ. Dyn. 16, 5–21. doi: 10.1016/0090-2616(88)90009-5
Honda, S., Ishimaru, A., Utsunomiya, S., Yamane, T., Oda, M., Sakamoto, K., et al. (2017). Considering the relation between the Haidt’s moral foundations theory and Japanese morality through developing a new scale. Bull. Fac. Educ. Yamaguchi Univ., 66, 95–106. Available at: https://cir.nii.ac.jp/crid/1520290883590017664?lang=en (accessed March 8, 2023) [Japanese]
Hong, Y., Chiu, C., and Kung, M. (1997). “Bringing culture out in front: effects of cultural meaning system activation on social cognition” in Progress in Asian social psychology, vol. 1. eds. K. Leung, Y. Kashima, U. Kim, and S. Yamaguchi (Singapore: Wiley), 139–150.
Hur, T., Benedvides, P., Lee, C., Park, S. W., and Heo, Y. H. (2016). The meaning of family to Korea: Indigenous analysis of familism in Korea. Presented at the international congress of psychology, Yokohama, Japan, 24–29
Hwang, K. K. (1999). Filial piety and loyalty: two types of social identification in Confucianism. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 2, 163–183. doi: 10.1111/1467-839X.00031
Hwang, K. K. (2006). Moral face and social face: contingent self-esteem in Confucian society. Int. J. Psychol. 41, 276–281. doi: 10.1080/00207590544000040
Hyun, J.-H., Eom, K.-M., and Han, K.-H. (2009). Effects of social distance and mood on moral judgment – focusing on moral dilemmas. Korean J. Sci. Emot. Sensib. 12, 411–424.
Inumiya, Y., Han, M., Lee, D. I., Lee, J. H., and Kim, S. H. (2007). Development of a cultural self-construal scale: subjective, objective, and autonomous selves. Korean J. Soc. Psychol., 21, 17–34 [Korean].
Jeong, J. H. (2020). A comparison of the perception of social annoyance in Korea and Japan (in Korean). J. Japanese Stud. 83, 115–136.
Jia, F., Krettenauer, T., and Li, L. (2019). Moral identity in cultural context: differences between Canadian and Chinese university students. J. Moral Educ. 48, 247–262. doi: 10.1080/03057240.2018.1499504
Kim, R. (2016). Early Confucianism and contemporary moral psychology. Philos Compass 11, 473–485. doi: 10.1111/phc3.12341
Kim, K. (2020). Reflection on “Familism” discourse and “Confucian family” in Korean. Korean Cult. Stud. 86, 391–413.
Kim, K. (2014). Two illustrations from South Korea and some reflections about the public administration studies: Are we granted to pillory the ethics or social justice? Int. J. Philos 2, 48–59.
Kitayama, S., and Uskul, A. K. (2011). Culture, mind, and the brain: current evidence and future directions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 419–449. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145357
Koh, B-I. (1996). Confucianism in Contemporary Korea, in: Confucian Traditions in East Asian Modernity: Moral Education and Economic Culture in Japan and the Four Mini-Dragons. ed. T. Wei-Ming Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.
Kraut, R. (2022) Aristotle’s ethics. In E. N. Zalta and U. Nodelman (eds.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (fall 2022 edition). Available at: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2022/entries/aristotle-ethics/ (accessed March 22, 2024).
Krys, K., de Almeida, I., Wasiel, A., and Vignoles, V. L. (2024). WEIRD–Confucian comparisons: Ongoing cultural biases in psychology’s evidence base and some recommendations for improving global representation. Am. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/amp0001298
Laskar, M. (2013). Summary of social contract theory by Hobbes, Locke and Rousseau. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2410525 (accessed March 12, 2024)
Leach, C. W., Ellemers, N., and Barreto, M. (2007). Group virtue: the importance of morality (vs. competence and sociability) in the positive evaluation of in-groups. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 234–249. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.2.234
Lee, G., Park, J., and Ban, L. (2016). Understanding workplace adaptation as an acculturation process: a qualitative examination of South Korean highly skilled workers in Japan. Int. J. Psychol. Stud. 8, 107–120. doi: 10.5539/ijps.v8n4p107
Li, C. (1997). Shifting perspectives: filial morality revisited. Philos. East West 47, 211–232. doi: 10.2307/1399875
Li, C. (2008). When my grandfather stole persimmons. Reflections on Confucian filial love. Dao 7, 135–139. doi: 10.1007/s11712-008-9059-8
Mascolo, M. F., and Fasoli, A. D. (2020). “The relational origins of morality and its development” in Handbook of integrative psychological development. eds. M. F. Mascolo and T. Bidell (London: Routledge/Taylor & Francis), 392–421.
Matsuo, A. (2023). Morality is in the cultural eye of the beholder: a situation sampling study. J. Cogn. Cult. 23, 127–148. doi: 10.1163/15685373-12340155
Midooka, K. (1990). Characteristics of Japanese-style communication. Media Cult. Soc. 12, 477–489. doi: 10.1177/016344390012004004
Miller, J. G., and Bersoff, D. M. (1992). Culture and moral judgment: how are conflicts between justice and interpersonal responsibilities resolved? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 62, 541–554. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.62.4.541
Moon, Y. (1995). The problems of moral education and the tasks for its improvement. J. Philos. Ideas 2, 167–197.
Morris, M. W., and Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 949–971. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.949
Murayama, A., and Miura, A. (2019). Validation of the Japanese version of the moral foundation questionnaire: investigating the relationship with ideologies. Jpn. J. Psychol. Res. 90, 156–166. doi: 10.4992/jjpsy.90.17234
Nakamura, H. (1991). Ways of thinking of eastern peoples: India, China, Tibet, Japan. New Delhi: Motilal Banarsidass Publisher.
Norenzayan, A., and Heine, S. J. (2005). Psychological universals: what are they and how can we know? Psychol. Bull. 131, 763–784. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.763
Obeid, N., Argo, N., and Ginges, J. (2017). How moral perceptions influence intergroup tolerance: evidence from Lebanon, Morocco, and the United States. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 43, 381–391. doi: 10.1177/0146167216686560
Park, J., and Han, G. (2018). “Collectivism and the development of indigenous psychology in South Korea” in Asia-pacific perspectives on intercultural psychology. eds. W. W. Li, D. Hodgetts, and K. H. Foo (New York, NY: Routledge), 53–74.
Peng, K., and Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. Am. Psychol. 54, 741–754. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.9.741
Qingping, L. (2007). Confucian ethics and social morality: the deep paradox of Confucian ethics. Contemp. Chin. Thought 39, 15–24. doi: 10.2753/CSP1097-1467390101
Rai, T. S., and Fiske, A. P. (2011). Moral psychology is relationship regulation: moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychol. Rev. 118, 57–75. doi: 10.1037/a0021867
Robertson, S. (2019). Korean Nunchi and well-being. Sci. Religion Culture 6, 103–109. doi: 10.17582/journal.src/2019.6.1.103.109
Shweder, R. A. (1984). “Anthropology’s romantic rebellion against the enlightenment, or there’s more to thinking than reason and evidence” in Culture theory: Essays on mind, self and emotion. eds. R. A. Shweder and R. A. LeVine (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 27–66.
Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M., and Park, L. (1997). “The “big three” of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the “big three” explanations of suffering” in Morality and health. eds. A. M. Brandt and P. Rozin (New York: Routledge), 119–169.
Sigurðsson, G. (2018). “Selfhood and morality: East Asian and Western dimensions” in Social theory and Asian dialogues. ed. A. N. Giri (Singapore: Palgrave Macmillan).
Simpson, E. L. (1974). Moral development research: a case study of scientific cultural bias. Hum. Dev. 17, 81–106. doi: 10.1159/000271335
Sleziak, T. (2013). The role of Confucianism in contemporary South Korean society. Yearbook Orient. Stud. 1, 27–46.
Snarey, J. R. (1985). Cross-cultural universality of social-moral development: a critical review of Kohlbergian research. Psychol. Bull. 97, 202–232. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.97.2.202
Spencer-Rodgers, J., Boucher, H. C., Mori, S. C., Wang, L., and Peng, K. (2009). The dialectical self-concept: contradiction, change, and holism in East Asian cultures. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 35, 29–44. doi: 10.1177/0146167208325772
Steele, L. G., and Lynch, S. M. (2013). The pursuit of happiness in China: individualism, collectivism, and subjective well-being during China’s economic and social transformation. Soc. Indic. Res. 114, 441–451. doi: 10.1007/s11205-012-0154-1
Stowell, J. A. (2003). The influence of Confucian values on interpersonal communication in South Korea, compared to China and Japan. Intercult. Commun. Stud., 12, 105–116. Available at: https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/document?repid=rep1&type=pdf&doi=63f87d8d6778b1f18c8b30f379cee01c304daec7 (accessed August 3, 2024)
Sugiyama-Libra, T. (1978). Japanese patterns of behavior. Honolulu, HI: The University Press of Hawaii.
Takagi, A. (2013). Perceived norms and inconsiderate public behavior: a study on the differential impact of descriptive and injunctive norms, and of the causal relationship of perceived norms and behavior. Japanese J. Appl. Psychol., 38, 239–250 [Japanese]
Takamatsu, R., Min, M. C., Wang, L., Xu, W., Taniguchi, N., and Takai, J. (2021). Moralization of Japanese cultural norms among student sojourners in Japan. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 80, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2020.12.001
Tamai, K., and Lee, J. (2002). Confucianism as cultural constraint: a comparison of Confucian values of Japanese and Korean university students. Int. Educ. J. 3, 33–49.
Thalmayer, A. G., Toscanelli, C., and Arnett, J. J. (2021). The neglected 95% revisited: is American psychology becoming less American? Am. Psychol. 76, 116–129. doi: 10.1037/amp0000622
Uehara, A., Cheng, C.-C., and Tsuboi, T. (2011). A comparative study on perceptions of friendship among Japanese, Taiwanese and Chinese university students: based on an inquiry into the concept of “Intersubjectivity”. Intercult. Educ., 34, 120–135. doi: 10.34347/iesj.34.0_120 [Japanese]
Valdivieso-Mora, E., Peet, C. L., Garnier-Villarreal, M., Salazar-Villanea, M., and Johnson, D. K. (2016). A systematic review of the relationship between familism and mental health outcomes in Latino population. Frontiers in Psychology. 7:1632. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01632
van der Lee, R., Ellemers, N., Scheepers, D., and Rutjens, B. T. (2017). In or out? How the perceived morality (vs. competence) of prospective group members affects acceptance and rejection. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 47, 748–762. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2269
Vasquez, K., Keltner, D., Ebenbach, D. H., and Banaszynski, T. L. (2001). Cultural variation and similarity in moral rhetorics: voices from the Philippines and the United States. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 32, 93–120. doi: 10.1177/0022022101032001010
Vauclair, C. M., Fischer, R., Ferreira, M. C., Guerra, V., Hößler, U., Karabati, S., et al. (2015). What kinds of value motives guide people in their moral attitudes? The role of personal and prescriptive values at the culture level and individual level. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 46, 211–228. doi: 10.1177/0022022114557487
Wang, F. Y. (2004). Confucian thinking in traditional moral education: key ideas and fundamental features. J. Moral Educ. 33, 429–447. doi: 10.1080/0305724042000327984
Wenxue, J. (2020). 日韓中の文化の違いをどう乗り越えるか: 歴史問題へのアプローチ. Institute for Peace Policies, No. 140, 1–10. Available at: https://ippjapan.org/pdf/Opinion140_BKin.pdf (accessed March 11, 2024) [Japanese]
Yamamoto, Y. (1990). A morality based on trust: some reflections on Japanese morality. Philos. East West 40, 451–469. doi: 10.2307/1399351
Yeh, K. H., and Bedford, O. (2020). “The psychology of filial piety and moral decision-making in Chinese people” in The Oxford handbook of moral development: An interdisciplinary perspective. ed. L. A. Jensen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 385–401.
Yue, X., and Ng, S. H. (1999). Filial obligations and expectations in China: current views from young and old people in Beijing. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 2, 215–226. doi: 10.1111/1467-839X.00035
Keywords: morality, self, Confucianism, perspective from non-WEIRD psychology, East Asia
Citation: Takamatsu R, Park J and Matsuo A (2024) Morality through the lens of Confucian heritage countries: collective self variations and moral worldviews. Front. Psychol. 15:1454425. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1454425
Edited by:
Fanli Jia, Seton Hall University, United StatesReviewed by:
Danielle P. Ochoa, University of the Philippines Diliman, PhilippinesCopyright © 2024 Takamatsu, Park and Matsuo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Reina Takamatsu, reinat@dpc.agu.ac.jp