Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 02 July 2025

Sec. Environmental Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1481964

This article is part of the Research TopicClimate Change Challenge: Adaptation to Climate ChangeView all 19 articles

Psychological distance and pro-environmental behavior: Insights from wildfire-affected PCT hikers

Lindsay B. Miller
&#x;Lindsay B. Miller*Ronald E. Rice&#x;Ronald E. Rice
  • Department of Communication, University of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, United States

Introduction: As wildfires and extreme weather events increase in frequency and severity, understanding individuals’ psychological and behavioral responses to these rising climate change impacts is necessary to cultivate pro-environmental behavior (PEB). Based on a theoretical model grounded in construal level theory and the theory of reasoned action, we propose that exposure to wildfires is associated with psychological distance of climate change, climate change and wildfire attitudes, and PEB; that psychological distance is associated with climate change attitudes and PEB; and that subjective norms are associated with PEB.

Method: We assess these associations through an a priori content analysis of 66 semi-structured interviews with Pacific Crest Trail hikers during the 2022 wildfire season, illustrated through quotes responding to interview questions asking about such associations.

Results and discussion: The analyses and quotes provide initial support for the proposed model, nuanced insights into the subdimensions of each construct, and a basis for possible wildfire and climate change messaging.

1 Introduction

As climate impacts such as extreme wildfires become increasingly salient (e.g., Diffenbaugh et al., 2021), understanding their psychological and behavioral consequences is necessary to cultivate environmental action. However, how extreme weather events such as wildfires may be leveraged to promote pro-environmental behavior (PEB) remains unclear. One barrier to PEB is the common perception that climate change impacts will occur in the future, in distant locations, to other people, and with uncertainty, which reduce individuals’ motivation to personally combat the climate crisis (Griskevicius et al., 2012). In contrast, wildfires are glaringly proximal on all four of these dimensions, especially for those who directly experience them. Construal level theory (CLT) suggests that these dimensions of psychological distance (temporal, spatial, social, and/or hypothetical) inhibit engagement with climate change issues and other PEB (Maiella et al., 2020; Trope and Liberman, 2010). Thus some research (noted below) indicates that reducing psychological distance can increase such behavior, while others argue this approach by itself is insufficient. These mixed results may be the result of both conceptual and methodological issues.

Conceptually, the relationship between psychological distance and PEB is likely complicated by additional influences (Maiella et al., 2020). Scholars have argued that integrating CLT with constructs from the theory of reasoned action (TRA; Fishbein, 1979) and the later theory of planned behavior (TPB; Ajzen, 1991) can better explain engagement in climate-friendly behavior (Brinkerhoff, 2020; Deng et al., 2017; Tulone et al., 2020). In particular, attitudes and subjective norms—the two antecedents of behavior intention in the TRA—have been shown to influence behavior differently depending on psychological distance (Ledgerwood, 2008). Accordingly, this study integrates the original TRA with CLT, as it offers a more parsimonious fit for our aims. Moreover, the TRA provides a flexible foundation for model extensions—a common approach in environmental behavior research. For example, scholars have extended the TRA/TPB with personal goals and motivation (Islam et al., 2024), natural environment, cultural atmosphere, and emotions (Wang et al., 2025), cost and availability of alternatives (Oludoye et al., 2024), and moral norms and trialability (Valizadeh et al., 2023a), and by introducing additional paths between behavioral antecedents and PEB (Valizadeh et al., 2023b). Thus, the TRA provides a theoretically justified set of variables that can be appropriately integrated with CLT constructs.

Methodologically, many existing studies assess psychological distance using experimental manipulations or retrospective self-reports. However, psychological distance is context-specific and dynamic (Brügger, 2020; Trope and Liberman, 2010; Wang et al., 2021); to produce valid insights, data should be collected during or immediately following real-world events. While agent-based modeling and simulation can propose how climate events may relate to PEB (Ribeiro-Rodrigues and Bortoleto, 2024), this study investigates whether and how personal experiences of wildfires relate to psychological distance of climate change, attitudes, subjective norms, and PEB, based on individuals’ lived experiences.

We take a novel approach to the study of psychological distance through developing a model based on CLT and the TRA, examined through content analysis of 66 semi-structured interviews conducted with Pacific Crest Trail (PCT) hikers during peak wildfire season in the summer of 2022, illustrated through participant quotes about the proposed associations. Through this investigation, we aim to (a) shed light on whether and how individuals’ perceptions of climate change are shaped, reinforced, or change as a result of exposure to wildfires and other extreme weather events, and (b) provide insights into effective mechanisms to increase PEB during extreme weather events. As environmental psychologists increasingly prioritize solutions-focused research, this study helps to diversify the discipline’s methodological toolbox and probe deeper into the lived experiences of those exposed to climate change impacts (Nielsen et al., 2021). Understanding whether and how individuals who experience extreme weather events (here, wildfires) are motivated to engage in climate action (here, pro-environmental behaviors) can facilitate the design, dissemination, and evaluation of behavior interventions during times of climatic disruption.

2 Literature review and theoretical foundations

2.1 Wildfires and climate change

Wildfires and extreme weather events are increasing in frequency and severity, in part due to the drying and heating of atmosphere associated with climate change (Diffenbaugh et al., 2021; Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change, 2021; MacDonald et al., 2023; see also Valiant, 2023, and Goodell, 2023 for books on global fire and heat). Except for 2022, the last half-dozen years have witnessed the highest number of extreme wildfires on Earth, increasing 220% since 2003; they are also becoming more intense (Cunningham et al., 2024). Since 1970, the average length of wildfire season in the western U.S. has increased by more than 100 days, and the number of acres burned has grown 600% (Environmental Defense Fund, 2024).

Although the ways in which forests are managed can affect fire severity, climate change also plays a central role in the recent growth in regional wildfires. Analyzing data from 1979 to 2020, Jain et al. (2022) show that the fire weather index, initial fire spread index, and vapor pressure deficit have all increased around 12%, influenced by lower relative humidity and increased temperature due climate change. Furthermore, these influences are projected to worsen in coming years (Jain et al., 2022). Climate change also affects the jet stream in ways that can divert rain from vulnerable areas while increased heat reduces snowpack and its water melt (Environmental Defense Fund, 2024), which makes areas more susceptible to wildfire. These contributions of climate change to wildfire patterns lead many environmental researchers to conclude that “extreme weather enhanced by climate change is increasing the duration of the fire season and occurrence of extreme fire weather and events” (Schweizer et al., 2020, p. 41; see also Wasserman and Mueller, 2023).

2.2 Exposure to extreme weather events

As climate change intensifies, individuals across the globe are experiencing its impacts. As many as 71% of US residents report having experienced extreme weather in their community in 2022, with 21% experiencing extreme wildfires (Leppert, 2022). A growing literature suggests that individuals who are exposed to these extreme weather events may exhibit higher levels of climate change concern and risk (Lidskog et al., 2019; Spence et al., 2011) and stronger beliefs in climate change (Reser et al., 2012). Experiencing these events has also been associated with behavior change such as disaster preparedness (Dessai and Sims, 2010; Silver and Andrey, 2014; Spialek et al., 2021) and intention to engage in PEB (Rudman et al., 2013; Spialek et al., 2021). Others have found that the relationship between extreme weather experience and PEB was mediated through climate change perception (Deng et al., 2017; Spence et al., 2011). To explain the relationships between direct climate change experiences, environmental attitudes, and pro-environmental behavior, some researchers have turned to construal level theory of psychological distance.

2.3 Psychological distance and construal level theory

The concept of psychological distance includes four interrelated dimensions: temporal distance represents the distance between now and the object’s point in time; spatial distance represents the distance between here and the object’s location; social distance represents the difference between “self” and “other” (Smith and Trope, 2006; Trope and Liberman, 2010); and hypothetical distance represents “the distinction between real and imagined objects and between probable and improbable events” (Trope and Liberman, 2010, p. 7). Many scholars propose that in the absence of direct experience of its impacts, climate change is likely to be perceived as psychologically distant (affecting other locations, the long term, other people, and with uncertainty), and that this psychological distance can impede environmental action (e.g., van der Linden et al., 2015). Indeed, reducing psychological distance of climate change, whether experimentally or naturally as climate change impacts become more apparent, is frequently suggested as a strategy to mobilize PEB (e.g., Jones, 1989; Schuldt et al., 2016; Spialek et al., 2021; van der Linden et al., 2015). However, there is little evidence for the efficacy of this approach on its own (Brügger et al., 2016; Chen, 2020; McDonald et al., 2015; Schuldt et al., 2018; Spence et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2022, 2021). Researchers also caution that many of the findings that support this claim are correlational, and that the relationship between psychological distance of climate change and PEB does not guarantee that interventions designed to decrease psychological distance will inspire more climate action (Wang et al., 2021).

Construal level theory (CLT) is a frequently used framework for understanding the effects of psychological distance on attitudes and behavior (e.g., Wang et al., 2021). CLT argues that when attitude objects are psychologically distant, individuals form abstract mental representations, or construals, of the object, and will therefore be more influenced by other psychologically distant stimuli (Trope and Liberman, 2010). In contrast, when individuals perceive that an attitude object is psychologically proximate (e.g., they directly experience climate change impacts), they will construe the object concretely, and they will be more influenced by other psychologically proximate and concrete stimuli (Spence et al., 2012; Trope and Liberman, 2010). For example, prior research has found subjective norms (based on one’s immediate social context) are more influential on psychologically proximate attitude objects, and individual attitudes (based on broad principles that apply across situations) are more influential on psychologically distant attitude objects (Ledgerwood, 2008; see also Bijani et al., 2017).

Although CLT provides an important basis for understanding how climate change is mentally represented, Brügger et al. (2016) note that “from the perspective of construal level theory, decreasing psychological distance should not itself influence people’s willingness to act but change the processes that underlie individual decision-making” (p. 125). Therefore, recent research suggests that the relationship between psychological distance and behavior may be strengthened by additional explanatory variables (Brinkerhoff, 2020; Chen, 2020; Jia et al., 2021). To better understand behavior, various scholars have integrated the cognitive constructs of the CLT with constructs from the TRA and the later TPB that relate to behavior intentions and change (Brinkerhoff, 2020; Deng et al., 2017; Tulone et al., 2020).

2.4 The theory of reasoned action

Fishbein’s (1979) theory of reasoned action suggests that a person’s behavior is best predicted by their predisposition toward the action, which itself is predicted by both subjective norms and one’s attitudes. Subjective norms refer to the perceived social pressure from important others to perform (or not perform) a behavior (Fishbein, 1979). Environmental attitudes are commonly viewed as stable evaluative tendencies that reflect individuals’ environmental concern and values (Miller et al., 2022), and influence people’s beliefs about, affect toward, and behavioral responses toward the environment (Milfont and Duckitt, 2010). Given CLT findings that subjective norms are more influential than individual attitudes when attitude objects are psychologically proximate (Ledgerwood, 2008), and that environmental attitudes are affected by perceived susceptibility to climate change impacts (Shen et al., 2024), integrating the TRA with CLT may clarify diverging findings in the literature by exploring the constructs that enable or constrain the relationship between psychological distance of climate change and PEB. Following CLT’s tenet that stimuli are more strongly related when matched with construal level, this study examines both climate change attitudes (more abstract) and wildfire attitudes (more concrete).

The original conceptual framework of the TRA specifies that attitudes and subjective norms should each be measured in relation to a specific target behavior, and examined through measures of the beliefs underlying these constructs (Fishbein, 1979). Accordingly, studies based on the TRA “should normally consist of two parts: qualitative (identification of antecedent beliefs) and quantitative (statistical assessment of direct and indirect variables)” (Yuriev et al., 2020). Although many examinations of PEB through the TRA and TPB relate antecedents to a specific target behavior, far fewer examine the underlying attitude and subjective norm beliefs (Yuriev et al., 2020). Further, meta-analyses on TPB antecedents have found that general attitude and norm measures still explain significant variance in PEB (Geiger et al., 2019).

2.5 Pro-environmental behaviors

Pro-environmental behavior is “behavior that consciously seeks to minimize the negative impact of one’s actions on the natural and built world” (Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002, p. 240). Previous research on experiences of extreme weather events suggests that exposure can lead to behavior change to guard against future similar events (more concrete; Deng et al., 2017; Silver and Andrey, 2014; Spialek et al., 2021), but that it can also affect PEB in domains unrelated to the extreme weather (more abstract; López-Feldman and González, 2022; Reser et al., 2012; Spence et al., 2011). Therefore, in the context of extreme weather events, we distinguish between PEBs that relate to the event itself (wildfire PEB; behaviors that relate to helping to prevent wildfires) and other PEB (general PEB; behaviors related to more general environmental contexts). These two categories represent individuals’ general propensities to engage in (general or wildfire-specific) environmental action (Lange, 2024).

3 Model and hypotheses

Based on the above review, Figure 1 presents the overall model of the main associations, indicating the corresponding hypotheses. The model proposes that wildfire exposure is associated with H1 psychological distance of climate change, H2 wildfire attitudes, H3 climate change attitudes, and H4 wildfire PEB. In turn, psychological distance of climate change is associated with H7 climate change attitudes and H8 general PEB. Climate change attitudes H6 are associated with general PEB, and H5 wildfire attitudes are associated with wildfire PEB. Finally, social norms influence H9 wildfire PEB and H10 general PEB. We state these associations as hypotheses, because they are justified by the literature and theoretical foundations, but these are not statistically tested. Rather, they are assessed by the presence of the a priori content codes in the comments; i.e., the associations are not tested in a causal research design but are based on participants’ comments about those associations.

Figure 1

Figure 1. Main relationships. Constructs in A are from construal level theory; constructs in B are from the theory of reasoned action; constructs in C are from both.

4 Method

We content analyzed the qualitative interview data using an a priori and reliable coding scheme (Table 1), an approach used in prior studies (e.g., Baxter and Eyles, 1999; Kleinheksel et al., 2020; Neuendorf, 2019; Weston et al., 2001). This allows us to provide thick descriptions to understand the nuances of the model that are nonetheless firmly grounded and validated in the literature and the overall model. Thus, we do not seek, report, or analyze emergent or inductive codes or themes, nor use the coded content to generate grounded theory.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Operationalization of constructs from the theory of reasoned action and construal level theory.

4.1 Site

The Pacific Crest Trail (PCT; see Figure 2) was selected as a site due to the annual disruption of wildfire. Each year, over 4,000 people receive permits to hike the entire PCT from Mexico to Canada during peak fire season in the Northwest. Each year, sections of the PCT close as wildfires burn across or near the trail (Gerety and Trinca, 2022; Harrell, 2021). For long-distance PCT hikers who spend months preparing to walk the 2,652 miles, the presence of an active wildfire creates physical threats and disruptions, along with emotional disruptions due to the incineration of the trail which has become their home, and the inability to complete this long-sought goal. Feedback on study materials was obtained from a pretest sample of undergraduates and from the Pacific Crest Trail Association, generating a final structured interview guide. The project was publicized on Facebook and Instagram PCT accounts, and on posters and fliers that were delivered and mailed to PCT resupply towns and campsites popular during the early months of the season.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Map of the Pacific Crest Trail, including wildfires, previously burned areas, and interview locations along the PCT. Underlying map source: www.pcta.org. Sections indicate PCT markers. The flame symbol indicates an active fire at the time of the interviews, black trees indicate large previously burned areas, and microphone icons indicate the interview locations.

4.2 Positionality

The first author registered for and hiked the PCT between April–July 2022, hiking over 1,200 miles from the Southern Terminus to Quincy, CA. This included walking and camping with other PCT hikers, being given a trail name, and importantly, being affected by environmental obstacles, wildfire smoke, prior wildfire burn areas, and active fires. Despite designing this project with the intention of studying wildfire impacts, the first author was surprised by the profundity of the physical and emotional impacts of the wildfires when they emerged, both personally and as noted by interviewees. Experiencing the tragedy of having a section of the trail and a yearlong plan go up in smoke overnight lent an understanding of the context of participants’ comments and credibility to the results.

4.3 Sample

The first author conducted semi-structured interviews with hikers willing to participate in the interview while in resupply trail towns. Thus the researcher engaged with individuals who were guaranteed exposure to different levels of wildfire: they saw flames and ash; smelled and saw smoke and haze; were evacuated; had their plans disrupted; walked through burned areas; and communicated with other hikers about their experiences. This method allowed for the collection of rich data from individuals who were experiencing different levels of wildfire in real time.

4.4 Semi-structured on-site interviews

Corresponding to the recommendation noted earlier that CLT data should be collected during or immediately following real-world events, conducting interviews in the context of the PCT—where hikers are more likely to have been or would be exposed to wildfires—both increases the relevance of their comments to CLT and helps to better understand their lived experiences. The semi-structured interview guide (see Table 2, which also provides rationales for each set of questions) represented the project motivations and theoretical foundations of the model. The interviews followed the semi-structured guide, while maintaining flexibility for participants to tell their stories. Questions that had already been addressed in the participant’s prior responses were not asked, and probes were only asked as needed.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Semi-structured interview guide

Sixty-seven interviews were conducted, though one was cut short, resulting in 66. The interviews were conducted in limited number throughout the beginning of the hiking season, and in concentration once wildfires along the PCT began at the end of July 2022. The PCT Map in Figure 2 shows then-active wildfires, burned areas from prior fires, and the interview locations. Respondents were mostly from the United States (US) (46), but some from other countries: Germany (5), Canada (3), Australia, Denmark, England, and the Netherlands (2 each), and one each from Slovenia and Sweden. Sixteen respondents were female, 45 were male, and one respondent identified as non-binary (note that four interviews were conducted with two respondents). Upon completion of the study, the anonymized audio files were transcribed using a professional service.

4.5 Content analysis

Content analysis may be qualitative (thematic) or quantitative (frequency of codes), and a priori (reflecting a developed model or explicit baselines) or emergent/inductive (allowing for codes and subsequent themes to emerge from the data and researcher interpretation; Neuendorf, 2017). Here, we conduct quantitative a priori coding. To analyze the comments, we developed an initial a priori coding scheme spreadsheet reflecting the motivations and theory of the study, as reflected in Figure 1, as well as the wording and sequence of the interview guide. Table 3 lists the main categories and specific codes. The main categories correspond to the concepts in the Figure 1 model; the specific codes were identified by the authors through close reading of the comments as specific instances or forms of the main categories. The coding unit was each entire interview. Two authors and two trained assistants coded the transcripts by first reading the entire interview. Then they returned to the beginning and started coding (entering the transcript line number) following the spreadsheet. However, the two final attitude entries were coded after initial coding and again revisiting the entire interview. All four members coded and discussed questions, ambiguities, or suggestions for improved operationalization through multiple initial sets of five interviews each until we achieved basic stability of the codebook. Early difficulties with several codes and interview ambiguities resulted in dropping these codes. The first author and the two assistants then proceeded with production coding.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Content analysis codes and descriptive statistics.

To assess inter-coder agreement, all coded line numbers were converted to 1 s, with 0 indicating no code. The values for each coder for each code were calculated using Freelon’s ReCal3 (Freelon, n.d.). However, Cohen’s Kappa and Krippendorff’s Alpha produced some nonsensical results due to the low variance in many codings. This is a common phenomenon; Feng (2015) and Zhao et al. (2018) discussed these issues, with the latter especially emphasizing the validity of using agreement percentages for such contexts. Therefore we used the percent agreement across the three coders or across pairs of coders to assess progress toward agreement levels, by coders, by individual codes, and across all codes. Joint coding among the first author and two assistants was formally compared for 11 sets of interviews, and the two assistants independently coded three sets each. The average final unweighted agreement across all 29 joint codings was 85%, with a median of 100%. Remaining disagreements for joint coding were resolved by majority coding.

5 Results

Table 3 presents the descriptive statistics. The interview questions and thus the content analysis codes often are about associations between two variables (e.g., whether wildfire experiences influenced participants’ perceptions of climate change), so most responses describe perceived causal relationships. Table 4 provides illustrative quotes corresponding to the subsections below, to support the validity of the results.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Illustrative quotes for content-analyzed relationships.

5.1 Wildfire exposure

In total, the 66 participants mentioned 136 instances of observing and/or experiencing wildfire impacts and 154 instances of acting in response to wildfires. Participants experienced a wide range of wildfire impacts: some noted only having experienced previous wildfires when walking through burned forests, while some evacuated from the trail due to immediate wildfire danger. However, hiking through a previously burned area was the most prevalent mention of observations/experiences (n = 58), and avoiding a section of trail due to an active wildfire was the most common behavioral response to wildfires (n = 47). Across the diverse wildfire experiences (both those that involved observing and acting in response to wildfires), hikers noted the psychological impact of seeing their temporary “home” (20) now threatened or destroyed. Despite the lesser threat to hikers’ immediate safety, this psychological impact was frequently associated with the 106-mile stretch of forest that burned in 2021 (the “Dixie Fire”), described by one as “really bleak… a morale breaker” (20). Thus, experience observing the impacts of previous wildfires was often equally, or more, poignant to participants as was evading threats of active wildfires.

5.1.1 Association between wildfire exposure and psychological distance of climate change (H1)

Most participants (n = 55) mentioned their experiences with wildfires affecting one or more of the four dimensions of psychological distance. Participants who did not hike or avoided a section of the trail because of an active wildfire-related issue (e.g., smoke or flames) were more likely to perceive climate change as psychologically proximate because of their wildfire experience.

The four dimensions of psychological distance were all represented about equally across the interviews (27, 33, 35, and 34 instances for spatial, temporal, social, and hypothetical, respectively, for a total of 129). Although most participants believed in climate change and were aware of wildfire impacts through the media, they expressed reductions in hypothetical distance through descriptions of their experiences being “eye-opening” as to the reality of climate change (35). Social distance was emphasized through the recurring theme of perceiving climate change affecting hikers personally, or affecting people close to them, with some feeling that “it’s a good reminder of how direct the effects of climate change can be on your life” (50). Reductions in temporal distance were represented through descriptions of climate change feeling like a problem for “our generation now and not so much about the future” (52). Finally, reductions in spatial distance were described by some hikers who no longer felt like climate change only affected other parts of the world, and they could now see that it was “happening here” (63). Thus, across all four dimensions of psychological distance, participants generally perceived climate change as being closer due to their wildfire experiences.

5.1.2 Association between wildfire exposure and attitudes (H2, H3)

5.1.2.1 Wildfire attitudes (H2)

Forty-nine participants described an increase in, reinforcement of, or confirmation of their concern about wildfires, their perception of the seriousness of the problem of wildfires, their worry about wildfires, and/or their general perspective about wildfires. Both hiking through a previously burned area and negative psychological reactions to the wildfires were associated with wildfire attitudes in some of the comments.

Hikers described increased pessimism about the state of wilderness areas after experiencing wildfire impacts on the PCT, with one describing that they “gave me a bit of a grim outlook for the future” (63). Often, they expressed concern that wildfires could make the PCT impossible to hike in just 5 or 10 years, or would prevent future generations from being able to enjoy the outdoors. Not all hikers expressed only pessimistic wildfire attitudes as a result of their experiences, however. Some hikers also explained that hiking through previously burned areas brought them a sense of optimism and beauty, especially at observing the regrowth of the forest—a phenomenon also observed by Lidskog et al. (2019).

5.1.2.2 Climate change attitudes (H3)

Forty-five participants described an increase in, reinforcement of, or confirmation of their concern about climate change, their perception of the seriousness of the problem of climate change, their worry about climate change, and and/or their general perspective about climate change. In some comments, climate change attitudes were associated with hiking through a previously burned area and with negative psychological reactions to the wildfires, similar to those for wildfire attitudes.

Most hikers emphasized that they already believed in climate change (only one hiker stated disbelief), so they often first expressed that their experiences of wildfires did not change their attitudes. However, after probing deeper, many hikers explained that while their beliefs had not changed per se, those were reinforced, confirmed, or intensified after their experiences with wildfires.

Not all hikers attributed the severity of the wildfires to climate change, however. Several hikers pointed to poor forest management as the culprit of increasing wildfires, or described the complex nature of climate change impacts tied up with fire suppression tactics (e.g., 53, 62, 65). The US Forest Service policy of near-total fire suppression (although recently engaging in more controlled burns and underbrush removal), contributed to the build-up of fire fuel, increasing intensity and spread (Busenberg, 2004; Kolbert, 2024). California, a state the PCT transects, is an illustrative case where such policies have contributed to “an increase in large catastrophic fires not typical of these ecosystems” (Busenberg, 2004, p. 41; see especially Schweizer et al., 2020). Therefore, some hikers explained that while the wildfires may be exacerbated by climate change, it was challenging to determine whether, or how much, each wildfire exposure was due to climate change or to improper human intervention.

5.1.3 Associations of wildfire exposure and pro-environmental behavior (H4, H5, H6)

5.1.3.1 General PEB

While the model does not hypothesize a direct association between wildfire exposure and general PEB, the interview guide allowed participants to describe their own interpretation of the ways in which wildfires affected their PEB. Thus, 31 hikers expressed that experiencing wildfires on the PCT affected their current or future engagement in PEB unrelated to wildfires (general PEB). The behaviors hikers described ranged from recycling, driving energy-efficient vehicles or taking alternative modes of transportation, communicating environmental risks to friends and family, and contacting politicians and voting for environmental policies, among others.

5.1.3.2 Wildfire PEB (H4)

Twenty-five hikers expressed that experiencing wildfires on the PCT affected their current (reported) or future (intended) wildfire PEB (e.g., stove safety). Some hikers described being motivated to engage in wildfire PEB because of their perceived increase in the risk of wildfires or of unsafe fire behaviors, such as one hiker who expressed that “you realize that you cannot mess around” (46).

However, other hikers noted that experiencing aspects of the wildfires did not motivate wildfire PEB. Because the most disruptive active wildfires during the 2022 PCT season were not started by human action, it is possible that participants’ exposure to wildfire underscored how little one’s personal PEB can do to alleviate or respond to this multifaceted problem. It may also be difficult for participants to increase their wildfire PEB, which are limited and more likely to experience a ceiling effect.

5.2 Attitudes as a predictor (H5, H6)

The hypothesized model suggested that wildfire attitudes should be associated with wildfire PEB (H5), and that climate change attitudes should be associated with general PEB (H6). However, comments indicated that both attitudes (wildfire and climate change) were associated with general PEB, but not with wildfire PEB specifically. These results align with the lack of support for H4 (that wildfire exposure would be associated with wildfire PEB); see above for potential explanations for this pattern of results.

5.3 Psychological distance of climate change as a predictor (H7, H8)

5.3.1 Associations with climate change attitudes (H7)

Supporting H7, each of the four indicators of psychological distance was associated with climate change attitudes in participant comments. Participants described how the proximity of climate change, brought on by their exposure to wildfires, increased their concern for and awareness of general environmental issues. One participant explained that the immediacy of the wildfires “made me begin to really focus on what is actually happening…facing that hard truth head on” (20). Some hikers also felt that it was easy to have an ambivalent attitude until confronted with climate change impacts “at our front doors” (65).

5.3.2 Associations with PEB (H8)

Some hikers described being motivated to engage in PEB based on the perceived proximity of climate change. Referring to climate change, one hiker described how “Just to see it firsthand is another level. It makes me want to be more active in the change to make climate change better” (32). Some hikers felt that certain dimensions of psychological distance motivated them to engage in PEB. For example, one hiker explained that “when those experiences or threats become impactful for those people surrounded, that to me is a catalyst for change and for action” (4). In this case, the participant was motivated to engage in PEB due to the decreasing social distance of climate change impacts.

However, as mentioned previously, other hikers described increasing apathy and hopelessness in response to the psychological proximity, and a concern that their actions would not continue after the wildfire was no longer top of mind. For example, one hiker stated, “I think it makes me want to do things, but realistically wanting to make changes and actually making the changes is a huge step” (47). Some hikers also expressed that they were already doing all they could to support the environment.

5.4 Association of subjective norms with PEB (H9-H10)

The interview question asked participants to describe how social influence affected their engagement in PEBs, without specifying whether those were wildfire-related or general, though those were distinguished via coding. The comments were also coded for the four possible source(s) (other PCT hikers, friends and family not on the PCT, individuals who live nearby the PCT, and the media) of that influence.

Over half of the participants (n = 38) said that social influence, in the form of subjective norms (especially those from other PCT hikers and individuals local to the PCT), affected their intention to engage in general PEB. Hikers often reported being influenced by multiple sources of those norms. However, few specifically mentioned other people as having much influence on participants’ wildfire PEB (n = 7).

Some hikers described how they had come from a community that was not very environmentally conscious or aware, so spending time with outdoors enthusiasts gave them new motivation to perform (especially general) PEB. These hikers frequently described PCT hikers as “people who really care about the environment” (48). When asked about what kind of norms had the most influence on their PEB, many hikers described how observing people engaging in PEB was more motivational than being publicly shamed for inaction. These findings align with research suggesting that descriptive norms are more powerful behavioral motivators than are injunctive norms (Niemiec et al., 2020). However, some people who were environmentally conscious at home perceived PCT hikers to be less environmentally inclined than their usual social circles. For these participants, subjective norms on the trail had a negative (boomerang; Miller, 2025) effect on their engagement in PEB.

6 Discussion

This study provides specific experienced examples of, and general support for, most of the relationships in the theoretical model (Figure 1). For PCT hikers, many felt that their exposure to wildfire was related to decreasing psychological distance of climate change and to both wildfire and general PEB. In turn, psychological distance of climate change was related to attitudes (climate change and wildfire), and some participants mentioned decreasing psychological distance affecting their PEB. Further, participants described subjective norms from various sources influencing their intended and enacted PEB. These findings have implications for theorizing on CLT and the TRA, individuals exposed to extreme weather, and the communication surrounding salient climate change events.

6.1 Theory implications

First, these results contribute to research on extreme weather and the psychological distance of climate change (e.g., Jones, 1989; Schuldt et al., 2016; Spialek et al., 2021; van der Linden et al., 2015) by indicating that natural reductions in psychological distance to climate change may correspond with increases in attitudes without meaningful changes to PEB. Many hikers expressed increasing levels of climate change and wildfire concern, risk, and belief, but stated that their behaviors are unlikely to change as a result. However, the results also indicate that exposure to wildfires may not have to relate to psychological distance of climate change to affect wildfire attitudes and PEB, which supports previous research demonstrating that exposure to extreme weather is directly associated with higher levels of climate change concern, risk, and belief (Lidskog et al., 2019; Reser et al., 2012; Spence et al., 2011), disaster preparedness (Dessai and Sims, 2010; Silver and Andrey, 2014; Spialek et al., 2021) and intention to engage in PEB. Therefore, the two direct relationships between wildfire exposure and psychological distance of climate change, and between wildfire exposure and PEB, may be more important than the indirect relationship of wildfire exposure on PEB through psychological distance (though some did indicate this pattern). If findings from PCT hikers reflect psychological processes of other individuals who are exposed to climate change impacts, the global increase in extreme weather events may simultaneously promote the perception that climate change is psychologically closer and also a stronger propensity to engage in environmental action.

Importantly, these findings reflect a growing awareness of climate change as a result of direct exposure to climate change impacts. The qualitative exploration of these associations compliments previous quantitative assessments by demonstrating that some individuals who are exposed to wildfires are aware of the impacts these events have on their perception of climate change, and on their intended and enacted behavior. Construal levels and corresponding psychological distance can be primed without participant awareness (Trope and Liberman, 2010), so participants’ abilities to verbalize these changes in the context of a semi-structured interview is a noteworthy contribution, and one that merits further investigation. Although awareness by itself is rarely sufficient to motivate environmental action (Bergquist et al., 2023), the increased salience of climate change through exposure to extreme weather may be an important precursor to PEB (Spence et al., 2011).

The pattern of relationships between exposure to wildfires, psychological distance, attitudes, subjective norms, and behavior, as indicated by the interviewees’ comments, also highlights the utility of integrating CLT with the TRA (Brinkerhoff, 2020; Deng et al., 2017; Tulone et al., 2020). Many scholars find it useful to extend the TRA and TPB to improve their predictive power (Yuriev et al., 2020). Until recently, however, the integration of exposure to extreme weather events or wildfires and psychological distance with these models has been mostly unexplored, and thus is a central contribution of this study. As climate change impacts become more apparent (Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change, 2021), understanding how they relate to these central theories and variables in the literature will become increasingly important.

Beyond providing some general but nuanced support of the theoretical model, the coded content and qualitative quotes illuminate many aspects in the study of exposure to extreme weather events, psychological distance of climate change, and PEB. First, despite participants describing a wide range of wildfire impacts (non-behavioral and behavioral), some wildfire experiences, such as hiking through extensively burned areas, were described as being particularly influential for outcome variables. It is likely that different indicators of extreme weather may differentially affect perceived psychological distance, attitudes, and behavior, as suggested in the literature. Indeed, CLT predicts that concrete representations (such as wildfire) can be abstracted in multiple ways (such as climate change, improper forest management, dangerous landscape, etc.), according to one’s goals (Trope and Liberman, 2010). It may be the case that certain wildfire experiences are more frequently construed as climate change impacts, or as motivational for general pro-environmental behavior change. Given that individuals both on and off the PCT are more likely to experience certain wildfire impacts than others (e.g., seeing or smelling smoke vs. evacuating from an active fire), these possibilities warrant future research.

Beyond our unique sample of PCT hikers, individuals encounter climate change impacts when they are exposed to smoke from an active fire, are forced to evacuate their homes, experience a loss of personal property, or face uncertainty due to changing temperatures and weather patterns. Therefore, while our results are not generalizable beyond the specific PCT population, their implications may be transferable to other contexts in which individuals experience climate change impacts (Jahn and Myers, 2024; Morgan, 2007). However, more research is needed to confirm these relationships among individuals who are exposed to extreme wildfires in other contexts, and to examine the ways in which other extreme weather events relate to the hypothesized model.

6.2 Implications for climate change communication

These results suggest several implications for climate change communication. Wildfires and extreme weather events are accompanied by increased media attention during and immediately following the event (Crow et al., 2017; Johnson et al., 2009). Although media producers are increasing the frequency with which they mention climate change-related issues during their reporting of extreme weather (Hopke, 2020), there is still a long way to go (Cordner and Schwartz, 2019; Crow et al., 2017; Spialek et al., 2021). Furthermore, the media’s normative framing of behavioral responses to extreme weather can influence citizen responses (Nilsson and Enander, 2020), but most do not provide information on risk mitigation, policy solutions, or other actions individuals can take in response to the event (Crow et al., 2017). This study’s preliminary findings suggest that not only should messaging during extreme weather events describe aspects of the event, but that highlighting the proximity (whether physical, temporal, social, or hypothetical) of climate change and relevant (favorable) normative standards around PEB may increase the behavioral impact of the exposure. If the results found within this sample of PCT hikers reflect processes that occur outside of this unique context, they may help media producers tailor their messaging on wildfire and extreme weather to motivate greater pro-environmental behavior. This messaging may be used in combination with other pro-environmental policy mixes to offset negative effects of single interventions (e.g., negative spillover; Alt et al., 2024). See Ettinger et al. (2023) for a framework for using extreme weather events as teachable moments to increase PEB.

Finally, this study contributes to an understanding of why exposure to wildfires may not lead to changes in psychological proximity, attitudes, and PEB (Maiella et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2022). Although not hypothesized, the semi-structured interview guide allowed participants to expand upon this lack of association, and they mentioned several barriers to these associations. The most frequent included a lack of efficacy as a result of understanding the scope of environmental challenges. For example, many described the sense of helplessness that came from watching such a catastrophic impact, with one noting that:

I feel like there are two general ways that you can react to very present examples of climate change like fire. Some people, I think, get very invigorated by that and become very active and have decided this is a time when I choose what I want to be. For myself, I actually find that it’s the cause of a lot of apathy. The scope of things is so incredibly overwhelming for me that it’s hard for me to believe that my actions will make a difference (4).

This apathy was often accompanied by an argument that individual actions are not enough, such as one hiker’s description that “My individual choices are not gonna mean anything if Amazon keeps overnight packages everywhere. I do not know that the wildfires, necessarily, have changed that” (41). The lack of efficacy reflects studies of CLT that demonstrate that psychologically proximate events prompt individuals to think about an activity in terms of how they will perform an action, whereas psychologically distant events promote greater consideration of why they will act (Liberman et al., 2007; McCrea et al., 2008). When wildfires make the psychological presence of climate change feel more immediate, it is possible that reflecting on how one would contribute to climate change mitigation increases the salience of barriers to PEB. If this is the case, individuals exposed to wildfires and other climate change impacts may benefit from information that increases their perceived efficacy to perform PEB.

Other hikers anticipated dissipating urgency once they were removed from the wildfire impacts—a phenomenon that is likely to occur outside of the PCT community as well (Konisky et al., 2016). For example, one hiker acknowledged that his motivation to engage in PEB “might change for a few months, but then I’ll easily slip into the comfy life of just never worrying and not really caring” (32). In developing messages and other interventions to promote PEB during extreme weather events, barriers such as these need to be addressed before meaningful change can be expected (Steg and Vlek, 2009). Thus, a more expansive underlying model based on the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991) which explicitly includes the concept of perceived behavioral control, could motivate greater consideration of these and other barriers in influencing PEB.

6.3 Limitations

This study has several limitations. First, PCT hikers do not represent the general adult population (e.g., partially due to their extensive outdoor recreation; Andre et al., 2017; Baird et al., 2020; Cole, 2018; Wilcer et al., 2018). The respondents are long-distance hikers who, during their hike, may be confronted with significant forms of environmental change. Further, this population also likely encounters a ceiling effect whereby PCT hikers’ attitudes and PEB may already be high (noted in some of the interviews), which could have reduced the associations of wildfire exposure with attitudes and PEB reflected in the comments. However, these PCT hikers had experienced different levels of exposure to wildfires, making them particularly unique but also salient respondents, filling a gap in the literature. Thus this sample is an intentional, purposeful sample, reflecting the construal level theory proposition that psychological distance is more strongly affected by near-immediate and salient experiences. Thus there is no presumption of the sample or results representing more general populations. Second, even among the PCT hiker population, only hikers who were interested in and had time to participate in the interviews were included, which further limits the results. Therefore, the sample has ecological validity, and is purposive, but is a small, non-representative, convenience sample.

Third, coded content can only refer to reported or intended general propensities to engage in PEBs (Lange, 2024) as reflected in responses to the questions and in Figure 2, as there are no measures of observed PEB. However, a much larger sample could use the quantitative counts of each code to statistically test portions of, or the full, model. Fourth, we did not code for all constructs of the TRA as outlined in the theoretical model, so future research may explore more specific attitudes toward the behavior, and the beliefs underlying these behavior-specific attitudes. However, the codes and relationships in this study do represent a common adaptation of the TRA within our theoretically-justified model, and the most salient aspects of relevant participants’ experiences and perceptions. Finally, of course, with all a priori content analysis approaches, other potentially relevant comments about wildfire experiences and PEB that may have emerged through open coding were not represented.

7 Conclusion

This study examined how Pacific Crest Trail hikers’ wildfire exposure shapes their perceptions of climate change and pro-environmental behavior, using a model integrating construal level theory and the theory of reasoned action. A content analysis of 66 interviews identified that:

• Wildfire exposure reduced psychological distance to climate change across all four dimensions, and reinforced concern about both climate change and wildfires.

• Attitudes were more consistently linked to general PEB than to wildfire-specific PEB.

• Subjective norms—especially from fellow hikers—influenced general PEB but had limited influence on wildfire PEB.

• Despite greater perceived proximity, many participants described barriers (e.g., low efficacy, apathy) that limited behavior change.

Although our sample was small and not representative of the general population, studying the relationships between exposure to climate change impacts, psychological distance, attitudes, and PEB in real time clarifies mixed evidence in the literature and suggests opportunities for increasing environmental action. Future research can expand on these findings by studying broader populations, incorporating measures of perceived efficacy, and testing interventions that highlight proximity and social norms to promote climate action.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by UCSB Human Subjects Committee; University of California Santa Barbara. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required from the participants or the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin because participants and the interviewer were Pacific Crest Trail hikers who do not travel with excess paper and pens. A digital copy of the consent form was provided to participants, and verbal consent was obtained before beginning the voice recording and again after voice recording began, as approved by the UCSB Human Subjects Committee.

Author contributions

LM: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. RR: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. The authors are grateful for the second author’s endowed professor account, The Arthur N. Rupe Fund, for funding aspects of the research (partial payment of coding and payment of interview transcriptions).

Acknowledgments

The authors are extremely grateful for the (non-financial) support from The Pacific Crest Trail Association (https://www.pcta.org/), the 2022 PCT cohort, the UC Santa Barbara Graduate Division Academic Services for authorizing an in absentia research academic term for Lindsay Miller, the University Human Subjects Committee for approving the study protocols, Professor Rice’s Arthur N. Rupe Endowed Professorship in the Social Effects of Mass Communication for funding some aspects of the study, the PCT interviewees, the PCT Trail Angels, the many trail friends and family who made the journey possible, and our two totally awesome research assistants, Romi Benasuly and Samuel Liu.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50, 179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alt, M., Bruns, H., DellaValle, N., and Murauskaite-Bull, I. (2024). Synergies of interventions to promote pro-environmental behaviors – a meta-analysis of experimental studies. Glob. Environ. Chang. 84:102776. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2023.102776

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Andre, E. K., Williams, N., Schwartz, F., and Bullard, C. (2017). Benefits of campus outdoor recreation programs: a review of the literature. J. Outdoor Recreat. Educ. Leadersh. 9, 15–25. doi: 10.18666/JOREL-2017-V9-I1-7491

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baird, J., Hutson, G., and Plummer, R. (2020). Examining links between connections to nature and intentions for pro-environmental behavior as outcomes of NOLS. Jorel 12, 367–379. doi: 10.18666/JOREL-2020-V12-I4-9992

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baxter, J., and Eyles, J. (1999). The utility of in-depth interviews for studying the meaning of environmental risk. Prof. Geogr. 51, 307–320. doi: 10.1111/0033-0124.00167

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergquist, M., Thiel, M., Goldberg, M. H., and van der Linden, S. (2023). Field interventions for climate change mitigation behaviors: a second-order meta-analysis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 120:e2214851120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2214851120

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bijani, M., Ghazani, E., Valizadeh, N., and Fallah Haghighi, N. (2017). Pro-environmental analysis of farmers’ concerns and behaviors towards soil conservation in central district of Sari County, Iran. International Soil and Water Conservation Research. 5, 43–49. doi: 10.1016/j.iswcr.2017.03.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brinkerhoff, L. L. (2020). The world needs less plastic: the role of psychological distance and self-efficacy in environmental messages (Thesis). Laramie, Wyoming: University of Wyoming, Department of Communication and Journalism.

Google Scholar

Brügger, A. (2020). Understanding the psychological distance of climate change: the limitations of construal level theory and suggestions for alternative theoretical perspectives. Glob. Environ. Change 60:102023. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2019.102023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brügger, A., Morton, T. A., and Dessai, S. (2016). “Proximising” climate change reconsidered: a construal level theory perspective. J. Environ. Psychol. 46, 125–142. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.04.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Busenberg, G. (2004). Wildfire management in the United States: the evolution of a policy failure. Rev. Policy Res. 21, 145–156. doi: 10.1111/j.1541-1338.2004.00066.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, M.-F. (2020). Effects of psychological distance perception and psychological factors on pro-environmental behaviors in Taiwan: Application of construal level theory. Int. Sociol. 35, 70–89. doi: 10.1177/0268580919881870

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, T. R. (2018). Investigating the thru-hiking experience: a study on the Pacific Northwest National Scenic Trail (Thesis). Missoula, MT: The University of Montana.

Google Scholar

Cordner, A., and Schwartz, E. (2019). Covering wildfires: media emphasis and silence after the Carlton and Okanogan complex wildfires. Soc. Nat. Resour. 32, 489–507. doi: 10.1080/08941920.2018.1530816

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crow, D. A., Berggren, J., Lawhon, L. A., Koebele, E. A., Kroepsch, A., and Huda, J. (2017). Local media coverage of wildfire disasters: an analysis of problems and solutions in policy narratives. Environ. Plan. C, Politics Space 35, 849–871. doi: 10.1177/0263774X16667302

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, C. X., Williamson, G. J., and Bowman, D. M. J. S. (2024). Increasing frequency and intensity of the most extreme wildfires on earth. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 8, 1420–1425. doi: 10.1038/s41559-024-02452-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, Y., Wang, M., and Yousefpour, R. (2017). How do people’s perceptions and climatic disaster experiences influence their daily behaviors regarding adaptation to climate change? — a case study among young generations. Sci. Total Environ. 581-582, 840–847. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2017.01.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dessai, S., and Sims, C. (2010). Public perception of drought and climate change in Southeast England. Environ. Hazards 9, 340–357. doi: 10.3763/ehaz.2010.0037

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diffenbaugh, N. S., Konings, A. G., and Field, C. B. (2021). Atmospheric variability contributes to increasing wildfire weather but not as much as global warming. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 118:e2117876118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2117876118

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Environmental Defense Fund. (2024). What does a hotter planet mean for wildfires? Available online at:https://www.edf.org/climate/heres-how-climate-change-affects-wildfires (Accessed 8.15.24).

Google Scholar

Ettinger, J., Walton, P., Painter, J., Flocke, S. A., and Otto, F. E. L. (2023). Extreme weather events as teachable moments: catalyzing climate change learning and action through conversation. Environ. Commun. 17, 828–843. doi: 10.1080/17524032.2023.2259623

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, C. (2015). Mistakes and how to avoid mistakes in using intercoder reliability indices. Methodol. Eur. J. Res. Methods Behav. Soc. Sci. 1, 1–10. doi: 10.1027/1614-2241/a000086

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fishbein, M. (1979). A theory of reasoned action: some applications and implications. Nebr. Symp. Motiv. 27, 65–116.

Google Scholar

Freelon, D. (n.d.) Recal3: reliability for 3+ coders

Google Scholar

Geiger, J. L., Steg, L., van der Werff, E., and Ünal, A. B. (2019). A meta-analysis of factors related to recycling. J. Environ. Psychol. 64, 78–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.05.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerety, R. M., and Trinca, M. (2022). Heat, water, fire: how climate change is transforming the Pacific Crest Trail : N. Y. Times.

Google Scholar

Goodell, J. (2023). The heat will kill you first: Life and death on a scorched planet. New York, NY: Little, Brown and Company.

Google Scholar

Griskevicius, V., Cantú, S. M., and van Vugt, M. (2012). The evolutionary bases for sustainable behavior: implications for marketing, policy, and social entrepreneurship. J. Public Policy Mark. 31, 115–128. doi: 10.1509/jppm.11.040

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrell, A. (2021). Hiking the Pacific Crest Trail is “almost impossible” due to climate change. Available online at:https://www.sfgate.com/california-parks/article/Pacific-Crest-Trail-thru-hike-climate-change-fires-16364571.php (Accessed 8.15.23).

Google Scholar

Hopke, J. E. (2020). Connecting extreme heat events to climate change: media coverage of heat waves and wildfires. Environ. Commun. 14, 492–508. doi: 10.1080/17524032.2019.1687537

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (2021) IPCC, 2021: summary for policymakers, climate change 2021: the physical science basis. Contribution of working group I. Available online at: https://elib.dlr.de/137584

Google Scholar

Islam, M. H., Anam, M. Z., Islam, M. T., and Sabbir, M. M. (2024). Exploring the impact of goals and motivation on young consumers’ sustainable plastic management behavior using the theory of reasoned goal pursuit. Cleaner Waste Syst. 8:100156. doi: 10.1016/j.clwas.2024.100156

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jahn, J. L. S., and Myers, K. K. (2024). “From mixed-methods to mixed research approaches for qualitative organization communication research” in The sage handbook of qualitative research in organizational communication. eds. B. H. J. M. Brummans, B. C. Taylor, and A. Sivunen (London, United Kingdom: Sage Publications Ltd), 3–22.

Google Scholar

Jain, P., Castellanos-Acuna, D., Coogan, S. C. P., Abatzoglou, J. T., and Flannigan, M. D. (2022). Observed increases in extreme fire weather driven by atmospheric humidity and temperature. Nat. Clim. Chang. 12, 63–70. doi: 10.1038/s41558-021-01224-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jia, Y., Tian, J., and Liu, H. (2021). The roles of mental construal level theory in the promotion of university students’ pro-environmental behaviors. Front. Psychol. 12, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.735837

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, J. F., Bengston, D. N., and Fan, D. P. (2009). Us policy response to the wildfire fuels management problem: an analysis of the news media debate about the healthy forests initiative and the healthy forests restoration act. J. Environ. Policy Plan. 11, 129–142. doi: 10.1080/15239080902732547

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, R. E. (1989). Understanding paper recycling in an institutionally supportive setting: an application of the theory of reasoned action. J. Environ. Syst. 19, 307–321. doi: 10.2190/8KX2-9XQM-RP38-CLPP

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinheksel, A. J., Rockich-Winston, N., Tawfik, H., and Wyatt, T. R. (2020). Demystifying content analysis. Am. J. Pharm. Educ. 84:7113. doi: 10.5688/ajpe7113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolbert, E., (2024). The perverse policies that fuel wildfires. Available online at:https://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2024/02/05/the-perverse-policies-that-fuel-wildfires [Accessed February 17, 2024].

Google Scholar

Kollmuss, A., and Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 8, 239–260. doi: 10.1080/13504620220145401

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Konisky, D. M., Hughes, L., and Kaylor, C. H. (2016). Extreme weather events and climate change concern. Clim. Chang. 134, 533–547. doi: 10.1007/s10584-015-1555-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange, F. (2024). What is measured in pro-environmental behavior research? J. Environ. Psychol. 98:102381. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2024.102381

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ledgerwood, A. (2008). Attitudes in their social context: malleability, stability, and the role of construal (Dissertation). United States, New York: New York University.

Google Scholar

Leppert, R. (2022). Most Americans who have faced extreme weather see a link to climate change – Republicans included. Pew Res. Cent. Available online at:https://www.pewresearch.org/short-reads/2022/08/12/most-americans-who-have-faced-extreme-weather-see-a-link-to-climate-change-republicans-included/ (Accessed 8.15.23).

Google Scholar

Liberman, N., Trope, Y., McCrea, S. M., and Sherman, S. J. (2007). The effect of level of construal on the temporal distance of activity enactment. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 43, 143–149. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.12.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lidskog, R., Johansson, J., and Sjödin, D. (2019). Wildfires, responsibility and trust: public understanding of Sweden’s largest wildfire. Scand. J. For. Res. 34, 319–328. doi: 10.1080/02827581.2019.1598483

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Feldman, A., and González, E. (2022). Extreme weather events and pro-environmental behavior: evidence from a climate change vulnerable country. Appl. Econ. Lett. 31, 1–5. doi: 10.1080/13504851.2022.2138810

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, G., Wall, T., Enquist, C. A. F., LeRoy, S. R., Bradford, J. B., Breshears, D. D., et al. (2023). Drivers of California’s changing wildfires: a state-of-the-knowledge synthesis. Int. J. Wildland Fire 32, 1039–1058. doi: 10.1071/WF22155

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maiella, R., La Malva, P., Marchetti, D., Pomarico, E., Di Crosta, A., Palumbo, R., et al. (2020). The psychological distance and climate change: a systematic review on the mitigation and adaptation behaviors. Front. Psychol. 11:568899. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.568899

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrea, S. M., Liberman, N., Trope, Y., and Sherman, S. J. (2008). Construal level and procrastination. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1308–1314. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02240.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonald, R. I., Chai, H. Y., and Newell, B. R. (2015). Personal experience and the ‘psychological distance’ of climate change: an integrative review. J. Environ. Psychol. 44, 109–118. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.10.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Milfont, T., and Duckitt, J. (2010). The environmental attitudes inventory: a valid and reliable measure to assess the structure of environmental attitudes. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 80–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.09.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, L. B. (2025). From persuasion theory to climate action: insights and future directions for increasing climate-friendly behavior. Sustain. For. 17:2832. doi: 10.3390/su17072832

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, L. B., Rice, R. E., Gustafson, A., and Goldberg, M. H. (2022). Relationships among environmental attitudes, environmental efficacy, and pro-environmental behaviors across and within 11 countries. Environ. Behav. 54, 1063–1096. doi: 10.1177/00139165221131002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, D. L. (2007). Paradigms lost and pragmatism regained: methodological implications of combining qualitative and quantitative methods. J. Mixed Methods Res. 1, 48–76. doi: 10.1177/2345678906292462

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuendorf, K. A. (2017). The content analysis guidebook, 2nd Edn. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc.

Google Scholar

Neuendorf, K. A. (2019). “Content analysis and thematic analysis” in Advanced research methods for applied psychology: Design, analysis and reporting. ed. P. Brough (London, New York: Routledge Taylor and Francis Group), 211–223.

Google Scholar

Nielsen, K. S., Cologna, V., Lange, F., Brick, C., and Stern, P. (2021). The case for impact-focused environmental psychology. J. Environ. Psychol. 74, 1–4. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/w39c5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemiec, R. M., Champine, V., Vaske, J. J., and Mertens, A. (2020). Does the impact of norms vary by type of norm and type of conservation behavior? A meta-analysis. Soc. Nat. Resour. 33, 1024–1040. doi: 10.1080/08941920.2020.1729912

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilsson, S., and Enander, A. (2020). “Damned if you do, damned if you don’t”: media frames of responsibility and accountability in handling a wildfire. J. Contingencies Crisis Manage. 28, 69–82. doi: 10.1111/1468-5973.12284

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oludoye, O. O., Supakata, N., Srithongouthai, S., Kanokkantapong, V., Van den Broucke, S., Ogunyebi, L., et al. (2024). Pro-environmental behavior regarding single-use plastics reduction in urban–rural communities of Thailand: implication for public policy. Sci. Rep. 14:4713. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-55192-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reser, J.P., Graham, B.L., Glendon, A.I., Ellul, M.C., and Callaghan, R. (2012). Public risk perceptions, understandings, and responses to climate change and natural disasters in Australia, 2010 and 2011. National Climate Change Adaptation Research Facility Griffith University, Gold Coast. Available online athttps://www.unisdr.org/preventionweb/files/30470_finalreportreserpublicriskperceptio.pdf

Google Scholar

Ribeiro-Rodrigues, E., and Bortoleto, A. P. (2024). A systematic review of agent-based modeling and simulation applications for analyzing pro-environmental behaviors. Sustain. Prod. Consum. 47, 343–362. doi: 10.1016/j.spc.2024.04.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudman, L. A., McLean, M. C., and Bunzl, M. (2013). When truth is personally inconvenient, attitudes change: the impact of extreme weather on implicit support for green politicians and explicit climate-change beliefs. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2290–2296. doi: 10.1177/0956797613492775

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuldt, J. P., McComas, K. A., and Byrne, S. E. (2016). Communicating about ocean health: theoretical and practical considerations. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 371:20150214. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0214

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuldt, J. P., Rickard, L. N., and Yang, Z. J. (2018). Does reduced psychological distance increase climate engagement? On the limits of localizing climate change. J. Environ. Psychol. 55, 147–153. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.02.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schweizer, D., Nichols, T., Cisneros, R., Navarro, K., and Procter, T. (2020). “Wildland fire, extreme weather and society: implications of a history of fire suppression in California, USA” in Extreme weather events and human health: International case studies. ed. R. Akhtar (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 41–57.

Google Scholar

Shen, T., Rasdi, I. B., Ezani, N. E. B., and San, O. T. (2024). The mediating role of pro-environmental attitude and intention on the translation from climate change health risk perception to pro-environmental behavior. Sci. Rep. 14:9831. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-60418-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Silver, A., and Andrey, J. (2014). The influence of previous disaster experience and sociodemographics on protective behaviors during two successive tornado events. Weather Clim. Soc. 6, 91–103. doi: 10.1175/WCAS-D-13-00026.1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, P. K., and Trope, Y. (2006). You focus on the forest when you’re in charge of the trees: power priming and abstract information processing. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90, 578–596. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.578

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, A., Poortinga, W., Butler, C., and Pidgeon, N. F. (2011). Perceptions of climate change and willingness to save energy related to flood experience. Nat. Clim. Chang. 1, 46–49. doi: 10.1038/nclimate1059

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, A., Poortinga, W., and Pidgeon, N. (2012). The psychological distance of climate change. Risk Anal. 32, 957–972. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2011.01695.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spialek, M. L., Allen, M. W., and Craig, C. A. (2021). The relationship between the disaster communication action context and citizen intent to shape climate-related disaster policy across California wildfire seasons. J. Appl. Commun. Res. 49, 325–346. doi: 10.1080/00909882.2020.1851041

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steg, L., and Vlek, C. (2009). Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: an integrative review and research agenda. J. Environ. Psychol. Environ. Psychol. Move 29, 309–317. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.10.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trope, Y., and Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol. Rev. 117, 440–463. doi: 10.1037/a0018963

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulone, A., Crescimanno, M., Vrontis, D., and Galati, A. (2020). Are coastal communities able to pay for the protection of fish resources impacted by climate change? Fish. Res. 221:105374. doi: 10.1016/j.fishres.2019.105374

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Valiant, J. (2023). Fire weather: A true story from a hotter world. New York, NY: Knopf.

Google Scholar

Valizadeh, N., Bijani, M., Fallah Haghighi, N., Hayati, D., Bazrafkan, K., and Azadi, H. (2023b). Conceptualization of farmers’ water conservation intention and behavior through the lens of economic man worldview: application of structural equation modeling. Water 15:3199. doi: 10.3390/w15183199

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Valizadeh, N., Jalilian, S., Hallaj, Z., Esfandyari Bayat, S., Hayati, D., Bazrafkan, K., et al. (2023a). Encouraging adoption of green manure technology to produce clean rice product. Sci. Rep. 13:8690. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-35964-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Linden, S., Maibach, E., and Leiserowitz, A. (2015). Improving public engagement with climate change: five “best practice” insights from psychological science. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 758–763. doi: 10.1177/1745691615598516

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S., Hurlstone, M. J., Leviston, Z., Walker, I., and Lawrence, C. (2021). Construal-level theory and psychological distancing: implications for grand environmental challenges. One Earth 4, 482–486. doi: 10.1016/j.oneear.2021.03.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Wu, Y., and Han, Z. (2022). Extreme weather and residents’ pro-environmental behaviors. Front. Environ. Sci. 10, 1–17. doi: 10.3389/fenvs.2022.1030010/full

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Yu, H., and Zhou, B. (2025). Formation mechanism of tourists’ pro-environmental behavior in plateau ecotourism destination. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 12:71. doi: 10.1057/s41599-025-04384-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wasserman, T. N., and Mueller, S. E. (2023). Climate influences on future fire severity: a synthesis of climate-fire interactions and impacts on fire regimes, high-severity fire, and forests in the western United States. Fire Ecol. 19:43. doi: 10.1186/s42408-023-00200-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weston, C., Gandell, T., Beauchamp, J., McAlpine, L., Wiseman, C., and Beauchamp, C. (2001). Analyzing interview data: the development and evolution of a coding system. Qual. Sociol. 24, 381–400. doi: 10.1023/A:1010690908200

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilcer, S. R., Larson, L. R., Hallo, J. C., and Baldwin, E. (2018). First day hikes: participation, impacts, and implications for the future. J. Park. Recreat. Adm. 36, 213–226. doi: 10.18666/JPRA-2018-V36-I2-8403

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuriev, A., Dahmen, M., Paillé, P., Boiral, O., and Guillaumie, L. (2020). Pro-environmental behaviors through the lens of the theory of planned behavior: a scoping review. Resour. Conserv. Recycling 155:104660. doi: 10.1016/j.resconrec.2019.104660

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, X., Feng, G. C., Liu, J. S., and Deng, K. (2018). We agreed to measure agreement - redefining reliability de-justifies Krippendorff’s alpha. China Media Res. 14, 1–15.

Google Scholar

Keywords: psychological distance, construal level theory (CLT), wildfire experiences, climate change perceptions, pro-environmental behavior (PEB), Pacific Crest Trail (PCT), semi-structured interviews, environmental psychology

Citation: Miller LB and Rice RE (2025) Psychological distance and pro-environmental behavior: Insights from wildfire-affected PCT hikers. Front. Psychol. 16:1481964. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1481964

Received: 17 August 2024; Accepted: 29 May 2025;
Published: 02 July 2025.

Edited by:

Giuseppe Carrus, Roma Tre University, Italy

Reviewed by:

Naser Valizadeh, Shiraz University, Iran
Victor Briciu, Transilvania University of Brașov, Romania

Copyright © 2025 Miller and Rice. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lindsay B. Miller, bGluZHNheW1pbGxlckB1Y3NiLmVkdQ==

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.