- 1Psychology Department, Universidad Central Marta Abreu de Las Villas, Santa Clara, Villa Clara, Cuba
- 2Facultad de Salud y Bienestar, Carrera de Psicología Clínica, Pontificia Universidad Católica del Ecuador, Quito, Ecuador
- 3Prisma Behavioral Center, West Palm Beach, FL, United States
Specifications data
• Subject: Psychology.
• Specific subject area: Developmental and Educational Psychology.
• Data collection: This research was based on a cross-sectional study. An online survey was conducted via Google Forms® from January to March 2024, distributed through WhatsApp groups, Facebook, email lists, and websites. No incentives were offered for participation. All Cuban citizens over 18 years old currently enrolled in university studies in the country were eligible to participate. The Executive Function Scale (UEF-1) for university students, which consists of 31 items and assesses 7 dimensions, was used.
• Data source location: Universidad Central “Marta Abreu” de Las Villas. Villa Clara, Santa Clara, Cuba (22°24'49''N 79°57'58''O).
• Repository name: Mendeley Data.
• Data identification number: doi: 10.17632/j5zjb46tr7.1.
• Direct URL to data: https://data.mendeley.com/datasets/j5zjb46tr7/1.
Value of the data
• This data provides a detailed perspective on executive functions among Cuban university students and helps fill a gap in the academic literature on this specific topic.
• Collected via an online survey and based on a scale with demonstrated psychometric properties, this data offers a methodological model that can be replicated in other studies of executive functions across different cultural contexts.
• With a substantial sample size of 1,092 university students, the results of this survey provide a solid foundation for future comparisons and analyses in similar populations or other educational settings.
• These data can serve as a reference for future research on executive functions in university environments, enabling other researchers to expand knowledge in this specific area.
Introduction
In the university setting, students face a variety of challenges, such as organizing resources to complete tasks, planning their learning, monitoring their progress across subjects, and ensuring their work meets required standards (Díaz-Guerra et al., 2025a). Additionally, they must assess whether their learning strategies are effective and maintain metacognitive awareness of their actions to grow into competent professionals (Wang and Yang, 2021; Ramos-Galarza et al., 2020; Cushman et al., 2022).
This study introduces the dataset from the University Executive Function Scale (UEF-1), a measure originally developed and validated for Spanish-speaking populations in Ecuador and Chile (Ramos-Galarza et al., 2023) and later validated in the Cuban context (Díaz-Guerra et al., 2025a). This scale has been used to develop an interactive model for understanding university students' ability to self-verify their learning in Cuban, Chilean, and Ecuadorian populations (Díaz-Guerra et al., 2024), as well as to propose explanatory models of conscious monitoring of responsibilities and self-regulated learning behaviors among Cuban University students (Díaz-Guerra et al., 2025b).
In this context, studying executive functions is particularly important. Executive functions are a system of higher-order cognitive processes that enable behavioral self-regulation, conscious control of thoughts and actions, and flexible adaptation to complex situations to achieve specific goals. These high-level cognitive skills enable students to consciously monitor and regulate their educational development in academic settings (Ramos-Galarza et al., 2023; Cartwright et al., 2019).
This research focuses on seven specific executive functions (Díaz-Guerra et al., 2025a):
(a) Conscious monitoring of responsibilities: Active tracking of obligations to ensure their fulfillment.
(b) Supervisory attention system: Mechanism regulating focus and allocation of attention.
(c) Conscious regulation of behavior: Deliberate control of actions to align with goals and norms.
(d) Verification of learning-related behavior: Assessment of actions that facilitate knowledge acquisition.
(e) Decision-making: Process of choosing between alternatives based on reasoning and objectives.
(f) Conscious regulation of emotions: Intentional management of affective responses to maintain balance.
(g) Management of resources for task completion: Efficient organization of time, effort, and tools to achieve goals.
The dataset presented in this study explores the executive functions of university students in Cuba, providing insights into how these functions operate within a Latin American context.
Methods
Procedures
To ensure an effective data collection process, the Executive Function Scale (UEF-1) was administered online using Google Forms. This platform enabled efficient, standardized data collection while offering participants the flexibility to complete the survey at their convenience. Participation was entirely voluntary, with students free to withdraw from the study at any time.
Informed consent was obtained with a focus on transparency, ensuring that participants were fully aware of the study's aims and procedures. Participants were made aware of the scope of their involvement and the implications of sharing their information, ensuring that their participation was based on an informed and voluntary decision.
Assessment tools
The study employed the following instruments:
Demographic Information: Demographic data collected included age, gender, and the province in which the students were enrolled in university.
University Executive Function Scale (UEF-1): Developed and validated for Spanish-speaking populations, this scale consists of 31 items that assess seven executive functions relevant to the university setting. These functions are: conscious monitoring of responsibilities, supervisory attention system, conscious regulation of behavior, verification of behavior for learning, decision-making, conscious regulation of emotions, and management of resources for task completion. Responses were measured on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
Data analysis procedure
The UEF-1 was meticulously selected for its relevance in evaluating executive functions within the university environment. Its structure and content were carefully aligned with the research objectives, facilitating a comprehensive and detailed assessment of participants' executive capabilities. Robust data security measures were implemented to maintain participant confidentiality and anonymity. The dataset does not include personally identifiable information, such as names or addresses, which could reveal participant identities. Additionally, explicit consent was obtained from participants for the anonymous sharing of their sociodemographic data and survey responses. Statistical software JASP was utilized for in-depth analysis and to derive meaningful insights from the dataset. JASP is an open-source, free software that provides a broad array of statistical and graphical tools, enabling a thorough exploration of the data and the identification of significant patterns and relationships. The presented data include participants' sociodemographic characteristics and confirmatory factor analysis to evaluate the scale's psychometric properties. This report provides descriptive statistics, item-test correlations (r), and factor loadings for the UEF-1 items.
Data analysis
The study included a sample of 1,092 participants from Cuban universities who completed the Executive Function Scale (UEF-1) (Díaz-Guerra et al., 2025a). Data were collected from January 23 to May 3, 2024, using snowball sampling across 14 higher education institutions nationwide, with voluntary participation and no missing data. Sample characteristics are presented through descriptive statistics (Table 1), including age (M = 20.05, SD = ±1.72 years), gender (60.4% male), and geographic distribution (93.0% Central Region). Reliability estimates were high for the total score (α = 0.91, ω = 0.91), while factor analysis revealed predominantly medium values for conscious monitoring of responsibilities (α = 0.76, ω = 0.77), supervisory attention system (α = 0.83, ω = 0.83), behavior verification for learning (α = 0.76, ω = 0.76), decision-making (α = 0.70, ω = 0.70), conscious regulation of emotions (α = 0.82, ω = 0.82), and task-solving element management (α = 0.76, ω = 0.76), with medium-low values for conscious regulation of behavior (α = 0.65, ω = 0.65).
The scale demonstrated adequate psychometric properties across its seven dimensions (Table 2). Mean scores (M) ranged from 3.30 (behavioral regulation) to 4.53 (social behavior), with standard deviations (SD) indicating moderate variability (0.84–1.90). Acceptable item-test correlations (r) were observed (0.35–0.63), particularly for attention (UEF22: r = 0.63) and emotional regulation items (UEF29: r = 0.53), along with optimal factor loadings (>0.70) for responsibility monitoring (UEF9: 0.79), attentional system (UEF22: 0.81), and learning verification (UEF20: 0.81). However, the behavioral regulation dimension showed the lowest loadings (UEF19: 0.46; UEF18: 0.49), suggesting weaker internal consistency.
Limitations
Among the study's limitations is the geographic bias in the sample, which was predominantly composed of universities from the Central Region of Cuba (93.0%), with limited representation from the Western (6.4%) and Eastern (0.7%) regions. This imbalance may affect the generalizability of the results, as the socio-educational particularities of other regions—such as access to resources, institutional dynamics, or student profiles—are not adequately reflected.
Additionally, the sample's representativeness may be constrained, as it focuses exclusively on universities in Cuba, potentially missing the diversity of university students across Latin America and other regions. The reliance on self-reported data from the Executive Function Scale (UEF-1) may introduce biases, such as social desirability or inaccuracies in self-assessment, which could impact the validity of the results.
Moreover, the study's specific context—conducted within the socio-cultural and educational environment of Cuba—may limit the generalizability of the findings. Differences in educational systems, socio-economic conditions, and cultural norms may affect executive function performance and perceptions differently in other contexts. Finally, external factors, such as peer influence and the presence of instructors, could influence participants' responses and affect the overall data quality.
Data availability statement
The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found in the article/supplementary material.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by the Institutional Review Board (HSIRB) at Central University “Marta Abreu” of Las Villas. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
DD-G: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. CR-G: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MH: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. YB-P: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Cartwright, K. B., Lee, S. A., Taboada Barber, A., DeWyngaert, L. U., and Lane, A. B. (2019). Contributions of executive function and cognitive intrinsic motivation to university students' reading comprehension. Read. Res. Q. 55, 345–369. doi: 10.1002/rrq.273
Cushman, G., West, K., Davis, M., LaMotte, J., Eaton, C., Gutierrez-Colina, A., et al. (2022). The role of executive functioning, healthcare management, and self-efficacy in college students' health-related quality of life. J. Am. Coll. Heal. 70, 2356–2364. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2020.1862128
Díaz-Guerra, D. D., Hernández-Lugo, M. C., and Ramos-Galarza, C. (2024). Integrating executive functions: exploring interrelations and implications for learning in university students. PsyArXiv Preprints. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/t5kqn
Díaz-Guerra, D. D., Hernández-Lugo, M. D. L. C., Ramos-Galarza, C., and Broche-Pérez, Y. (2025a). Validity and reliability of the executive function scale in Cuban university student. Front. Psychol. 16:1484883. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1484883
Díaz-Guerra, D. D., Ramos-Galarza, C., Broche-Pérez, Y., Hernández-Lugo, M. D. L. C., and Fernández-Castillo, E. (2025b). Explanatory models of conscious monitoring of responsibilities and regulation of behavior for learning: decision tree analysis and structural equations of executive functioning in Cuban University Students. PsyArXiv Preprints. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/46n2g_v1
Ramos-Galarza, C., Acosta-Rodas, P., and Bolaños-Pasquel, M. (2020). The role of executive functions in academic performance and behaviour of university students. J. Appl. Res. High. Educ. 12, 444–455. doi: 10.1108/JARHE-10-2018-0221
Ramos-Galarza, C., Ramos, V., Del Valle, M., Lepe-Martínez, N., Cruz-Cárdenas, J., Acosta-Rodas, P., et al. (2023). Executive functions scale for university students: UEF-1. Front. Psychol. 14:1192555. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1192555
Keywords: executive functions, university students, UEF-1, neuropsychological evaluation, self-regulated behavior
Citation: Díaz-Guerra DD, Ramos-Galarza C, Hernández Lugo MdlC and Broche-Pérez Y (2025) Executive Function Scale (UEF-1) in Cuban University students: dataset. Front. Psychol. 16:1483879. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1483879
Received: 21 August 2024; Accepted: 02 May 2025;
Published: 20 May 2025.
Edited by:
Evely Boruchovitch, State University of Campinas, BrazilCopyright © 2025 Díaz-Guerra, Ramos-Galarza, Hernández Lugo and Broche-Pérez. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Carlos Ramos-Galarza, Y2FyYW1vc0BwdWNlLmVkdS5lYw==