- 1Yasar Oncan Secondary School, Ministry of National Education, Malatya, Türkiye
- 2Department of Sport Management, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Inonu University, Malatya, Türkiye
- 3Department of Biostatistics, Faculty of Medicine, Malatya Turgut Ozal University, Malatya, Türkiye
- 4Department of Physical Education and Sport on Disabilities, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Inonu University, Malatya, Türkiye
- 5GSD-HPE Department, Sport Sciences and Diagnostics Research Group, Prince Sultan University, Riyadh, Saudi Arabia
- 6Department of Teacher Education, NLA University College, Oslo, Norway
This meta-analysis examines the effect of Cooperative Learning (CL) interventions, compared to traditional instructional methods, on students’ learning outcomes across affective, cognitive, physical, and social domains in physical education (PE). The review involved a comprehensive search of 12 databases in English, Spanish, and Turkish, with the last search conducted on June 2nd, 2024. Studies included were true experimental or quasi-experimental designs featuring direct CL interventions in PE, covering students of both genders from primary school to university levels. The standardized Cochrane methods were used to identify eligible records, collect and combine data, and assess the risk of bias. Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (CMA) v4 software package was used to yield a summary of quantitative results. Hedges’s g was used as the effect size (ES) measure, calculated from pre- and post-tests in both experimental and control groups. Forty-three studies (comprising 60 reports) were initially included, but three studies were excluded as outliers, leaving 40 studies (56 reports) with a total of 3.985 participants for analysis. The random effects model revealed a moderate positive overall effect of CL interventions (ES = 0.459, 95% CI = [0.324, 0.592], p < 0.001), indicating that CL enhances PE students’ learning across four domains. Subgroup analyses showed small to moderate ESs for affective (ES = 0.304), physical (ES = 0.471), cognitive (ES = 0.589), and social learning (ES = 0.612). Risk of bias was evaluated using Begg and Mazumdar’s rank correlation, the classic fail-safe number, and a funnel plot, all indicating a low risk of bias. Methodological quality was assessed using the Medical Education Research Study Quality Instrument (MERSQI). The study was registered on PROSPERO (ID: CRD42024532607). This meta-analysis underscores the effectiveness of CL as a student-centered pedagogical model in PE, demonstrating its positive effect on various learning outcomes in the affective, cognitive, physical, and social domains. The findings provide instructive data and strategies for researchers, practitioners, and policymakers aiming to integrate, implement, or make context-specific adaptations of CL into educational processes, while ESs in the affective, physical, cognitive, and social learning domains provide domain-based implementation guidance for these stakeholders.
1 Introduction
Cooperative Learning (CL) emerged in the mid-1970s at Johns Hopkins University (Slavin, 1990) as an innovative pedagogical practice aimed at helping students at various grade levels to cultivate shared intellectual development (Johnson and Johnson, 1999). The central tenet of CL is shared success rather than just sharing success (Slavin, 1995, 1996; Johnson and Johnson, 1999, 2009; Zhou et al., 2023) as it reduces social hierarchies and individual conflicts, focuses on collective responsibility and minimizes competition within and between students.
Although CL, as a pedagogical model, has a deep-rooted epistemological heritage in the educational sciences, in recent years it has become an interdisciplinary research focus in the physical education (PE) landscape (Fernandez-Rio and Iglesias, 2024), with the rise of student-centered model-based practices (Casey and Kirk, 2024) and the academic prioritization of the holistic development paradigm (Bailey, 2009; Casey and Kirk, 2024). Last two systematic reviews by Casey and Goodyear (2015) and Bores-García et al. (2021) have shown that the CL model has an important place in PE classes, promoting a range of positive learning outcomes for students. We propose that the CL model in PE, compared to traditional instructional methods, has a statistically significant effect on students’ learning outcomes in the affective, cognitive, physical, and social domains. Furthermore, social, demographic, and assessment factors may moderate the link between CL and learning outcomes. We test this theoretical claim via a meta-analysis of 40 studies (56 effect sizes [ES]).
2 Literature review
2.1 The application of CL in PE
As teaching and learning PE in schools in the 21st century still poses a significant challenge (MacPhail and Lawson, 2020), epistemological and pedagogical debates question the best way of organizing the interdependent elements of curriculum, learning and teaching practices to achieve specific learning outcomes (O’Neil and Boyce, 2018; Murtagh et al., 2023; Fernández-Vázquez et al., 2024; Howley et al., 2024). This inquiry began with a resistance to the didactic hegemony of Mosston (1966) traditional Teaching Styles; since the 1970s, structures have evolved from traditional teacher-centered approaches to novel student-centered models (Casey, 2016; Casey and Kirk, 2024). This educational evolution was triggered by Joyce and Weil’s (1972) Instruction Models, which centered on the active participation of the learner, creating an epistemic break; and by the Curriculum Models formulated by Jewett and Bain (1985), which codified interdisciplinary view as an ontic necessity, moving the paradigm to a pluralist axis. Metzler (2000) consolidated this process theoretically with the Instructional Models that operationalized the episteme of social constructivism, while Haerens et al. (2011) and Kirk (2013) evolved the paradigm from linear pedagogy to a rhizomatic matrix with Pedagogical Models. As a manifesto of the evolution of Models-Based Practices (Casey and Kirk, 2024), Kirk (2013) call for educationally beneficial learning outcomes for students in four domains (affective, cognitive, physical, social), gained empirical reinforcement in an umbrella review conducted by Fernandez-Rio and Iglesias (2024). The study reveals that the most widely recognized, practiced, and investigated pedagogical models (Casey and Kirk, 2024), such as Sport Education (SE), Teaching Games for Understanding (TGfU), Teaching Personal and Social Responsibility (TPSR), and CL (the model under investigation) despite different features of each, support the comprehensive and coherent learning achievements, with strong evidence embodying Kirk’s theoretical argument. Table 1 systematically presents a synthesis of the main advantages, limitations and ESs of these pedagogical models.
The core concept underlying SE is to develop competent, literate and enthusiastic boys and girls through authentic sport experiences strategically designed around six structural features (Siedentop, 2001): seasons, affiliation, formal competition, culminating event, record keeping and festivity (Siedentop et al., 2019). The crux of TGfU is to radically reverse the ‘technical skills first’ paradigm by utilizing modified game play (representation and exaggeration; Mitchell et al., 2013) to create a cognitively demanding learning environment in which students can explore the deeper tactical dynamics of the game play (e.g., zone reading, decision-making chains, collective action strategies; Bunker and Thorpe, 1982; Harvey et al., 2018). The central tenet of TPSR is to provide underserved children with the opportunity to develop personal and social responsibility skills through physical activity practices (Hellison et al., 2000) to apply these skills in other social settings, and to develop a sense of responsible citizenship (Hellison, 2011). CL was designed to unleash the dialectical synergy of learning in collective dialog (with each other), autonomous-interactional agency (by each other) and around common ontological goals (for each other; Dyson and Casey, 2016). This point of view transforms educational practice into a radical interaction by combining the symbiotic relationship of stimulating interactions along the axis of positive interdependence, the dialectics of interpersonal skill dynamics with collective autonomy, the articulation of process-oriented cognitive processing mechanisms with ontological responsibility, and finally the constitutive role of individual accountability within this organic totality (Johnson et al., 1994), all within a context of methodological-epistemological coherence. In this context, CL transforms educational practice into an ontological whole by weaving affective, cognitive, physical, and social layers together (Casey and Goodyear, 2015; Bores-García et al., 2021) and has a great research interest (74 studies superior to TGfU [49 studies] and TPSR [47 studies]; Iglesias et al., 2023).
The existing meta-analyses findings of CL (intrinsic motivation: ES = 0.38; self-determined motivation: ES = 0.95) address its pedagogical effectiveness with an ontological narrowing limited to the affective domain (Sierra-Díaz et al., 2019; Liu and Lipowski, 2021). Connected to this gap, this study develops the first meta-analytic model that reinterprets the mechanisms of CL’s hegemonic influence through the interdisciplinary dialectics of different learning outcomes in four domains (affective, cognitive, physical, social) into a triad of interdisciplinary synergy, epistemological continuity and ontological integrity.
In ‘liquid times’ writings, Bauman (2007), while analyzing contemporary society shaped by spatial and temporal dismemberment, touches on similar themes as other authors (e.g., Giddens, 1991, 1998; Beck, 1992, 1998; Nowotny, 1994; Sennett, 1998; Harvey, 2005), he positions the global age in a darker perspective with the concept of ‘liquid modernity’. Bauman portrays the constant liquefaction of social norms and relations as a dark ontological break where individuals are forced to adapt to this unstable reality. In this context, CL in PE can offer a resilient pedagogical alternative to the fragmentation created in today’s world by the ‘liquid modernity’ that Bauman criticizes. CL’s social structure based on the principle of students learning with, for, and through each other; its collective goal-oriented activities and peer-assessed processes (Dyson and Casey, 2016) can build a socio-pedagogical field of resistance against the individualism of the ‘liquid modernity’. CL, the most studied model in the PE literature after SE (Fernandez-Rio and Iglesias, 2024), also reveals an important shortcoming: the lack of a comprehensive meta-analysis that critically evaluates its product-oriented outcomes. This shortcoming constitutes the main motivation of our study.
CL is a synergistic pedagogical model in PE underpinned by multiple theoretical frameworks (Dyson and Casey, 2016; Metzler, 2017). Johnson and Johnson’s (2009) Social dependency theory emphasizes students’ collective efforts toward common goals (positive social dependency), while Vygotsky (1978) zone of proximal development theory explains the mentoring role of skilled peers in motor skill transfer. Bandura’s (1986) social cognitive theory explains how students gain self-confidence and motivation through peer observation. The constructivist approach (Bruner, 1997) supports the social construction of knowledge through group strategy discussions. Self-determination theory (Deci and Ryan, 2000) explains how CL fosters intrinsic motivation through autonomous role allocation and peer feedback. Motor learning theories (Schmidt, 1975; Newell, 1985) legitimize the collective analysis of teamwork challenges.
The main idea behind CL in PE is to teach students to achieve dynamic, creative, co-experiential learning outcomes through high-quality physical activity practices and sports (Dyson and Casey, 2016; Metzler, 2017). Thus, students experiencing CL applications in PE usually exhibit positive perceptions of the classroom atmosphere (Fernandez-Rio et al., 2017), which accentuate their subjectivity and personal inclusivity. Furthermore, as an effective counterweight to Liquid Modernity’s attribution of isolation and uncertainty, CL equips students with a range of authentic, meaningful, and transferable life skills (Lee, 2014; Dyson and Casey, 2016; Metzler, 2017). Physical fitness (Lee, 2014), social responsibility (Casey and Goodyear, 2015; Bores-García et al., 2021), critical thinking (Huang et al., 2017), motivation and mutual encouragement (Fernández-Espínola et al., 2020; Montoya et al., 2020; Liu and Lipowski, 2021), fairness and inclusivity (Cañabate et al., 2021), and communication and teamwork commitment (Sánchez-Hernández et al., 2018) have been well documented. Particularly, the CL application in PE can empower students to transcend the passive role of observers, a common pitfall in PE classes (Zhou et al., 2023). This principle holds true for the pedagogical landscape of PE across diverse educational tiers, spanning from primary/elementary school (Dyson, 2001) to secondary/middle/high school levels (Casey, 2013), and extending to university settings (Cohen and Zach, 2013).
At present, by leveraging the possibilities provided by digital technologies (Jastrow et al., 2022; Wang et al., 2023), ongoing scholarly investigations, practical applications, and collaborative networks, the CL model can be strategically integrated in synergy with other pedagogical models in PE (e.g., hybridization of the SE model; Fernandez-Rio and Casey, 2021) and offer customizable flexibility to different PE contexts. This integration demonstrates the potential of CL to create a multi-layered and synergetic learning atmosphere that foster students’ all-around development while expanding its worldwide recognition and acceptance (Dyson and Casey, 2016; Zhou et al., 2023).
The CL’s effectiveness in PE is embodied in structured and interactive teaching strategies such as Student Teams-Achievement Divisions (STAD), Learning Team (LT), Teams-Games-Tournament, Think-Pair-Perform, and Jigsaw (Metzler, 2017; Dyson et al., 2022). These strategies allow teachers to adapt learning objectives to the specific requirements of the curriculum and to design pedagogical applications that are sensitive to the individual needs of students. Furthermore, as the foundation of CL, interpersonal skills, processing, positive interdependence, promotive interaction, and individual accountability (Johnson et al., 1994) serve as scaffolding that help students to achieve specific learning outcomes in PE (Goodyear et al., 2014; Casey and Goodyear, 2015; Bores-García et al., 2021; Zach et al., 2023). The successful application of CL not only empowers learners to exercise autonomous agency in the cultivation of self-directed learning trajectories but also strategically positions them as active co-architects in the orchestration of collaborative achievement, thereby engendering a dialectical interplay between individual metacognitive development and collective knowledge synthesis.
2.2 Previous reviews
The existing literature has addressed the role of CL in the context of PE through unidimensional meta-analyses, systematic reviews, bibliometric analyses, and regional studies. While learning outcomes in four domains (affective, cognitive, physical, social) are brought up as legitimate product outcomes in PE (Bailey, 2009; Casey and Kirk, 2024), a systematic review by Casey and Goodyear (2015) revealed a scarcity of quantitative synthesis in a synoptic examination of the effects of CL on learning outcomes in these four domains. Although the study empirically confirmed that CL encourages authentic and transformative learning outcomes in the four domain, the fact that the learning outcome data in the affective domain remained only anecdotal and the learning in these four domains were not explored by quantitative syntheses such as ES prevented a complete understanding of the pedagogical potential of the model. Bridging this research gap could make critical contributions to the development of evidence-based curriculum designs and shaping global physical activity policies, as well as reinforcing the theoretical validity of CL. Six years later, Bores-García et al. (2021) expanded the scope of the literature by analyzing studies between 2014 and 2019 with geographical context, demographic trends and authorship dynamics. However, by adopting a descriptive approach and ignoring measures of ES (also as in Casey and Goodyear’s review), they hampered the understanding of the tangible effects of CL. This weakens the evidence base to support pedagogical decisions.
Regarding meta-analysis development, only three comprehensive studies represent a typical reflection of the reductionist and fragmented point of view in the literature, focusing on the effects of CL on intrinsic motivation (Fernández-Espínola et al., 2020; Liu and Lipowski, 2021) and self-determined motivation (Sierra-Díaz et al., 2019) on the axis of affective learning outcomes. While these studies present results on the effectiveness of CL interventions that exhibit methodological consistency but weaknesses in terms of comprehensive representativeness, the exclusion of an integrative perspective on multidimensional learning outcomes such as cognitive, physical, social domains and components of the affective domain (due to the singularity-focused research paradigm) reveals a critical gap in the conceptualization of CL as a pedagogical model. In this regard, our study attempts to strengthen CL’s model claim through quantitative synthesis by combining learning outcomes in a balanced way across affective, cognitive, physical, and social domains. Thus, we clarify how CL can contribute to holistic educational goals beyond the existing evidence limited to ‘single domain’ meta-analyses.
Dyson et al. (2022) provided a contextual perspective by examining cultural adaptations of CL in Chinese PE curricula. However, their avoidance of cross-cultural comparisons and lack of quantitative synthesis limited the generalizability of their findings. Zhou et al. (2023) mapped the trends in the research themes of CL through bibliometric analysis of 169 studies, but could not make a meta-analytic contribution due to their focus on pattern identification rather than statistical integration. While these studies contributed to the understanding of CL on a global scale, they were insufficient in providing practical guidance as they did not fill quantitative gaps. Most recently, Zach et al. (2023) examined the relationships between CL constructs and students’ learning outcomes in PE using discriminant analysis and illuminated operational dynamics. However, their avoidance of meta-analysis suggests that they missed the opportunity to create cumulative ES metrics. Even such innovative methodologies should be supported by meta-analytic rigor to measure causal relationships or pedagogical effectiveness.
In the existing literature, reviews examining the pedagogical effects of the CL model in PE have not been able to fully reveal the multidimensional potential of the model due to significant methodological and conceptual limitations. Among these limitations, the lack of quantitative synthesis of the learning outcomes in the affective, cognitive, physical and social domains and comparative analysis of these domains’ ES stand out. Moreover, the lack of meta-analytic frameworks limited the integration of cumulative results and statistical model development of interactions across domains. Moreover, the lack of additional analyses of how moderator variables (country, grade level, intervention duration, etc.) shape the effects of CL has made it difficult to understand the context-sensitive dynamics of the model. All these shortcomings may significantly hamper a comprehensive mapping of the pedagogical mechanisms of CL in PE and the development of high-quality evidence-based practices.
2.3 Value of meta-analysis
Meta-analysis is a statistical method that synthesizes results by aggregating study ESs and their variances, providing a quantitative summary of results (McKenzie and Brennan, 2019). This meta-analysis study aimed to show a quantitative summary of results on the effect of the CL model on students’ learning outcomes in the affective, cognitive, physical and social domains within PE context. The study not only comparatively reveals in which developmental domains CL is more effective (e.g., the relative superiority of social ES over affective ES), but also aims to explain the underlying dynamics of pedagogical mechanisms by analyzing ESs from the view of causal relationships. Thus, three broad research questions (RQs) were developed. RQ1: What is the effect of CL on students’ learning outcomes in the affective, cognitive, physical, and social domains in PE? RQ2: When examining the hierarchical effects of CL on students’ learning outcomes in a cross-domain analytical framework (affective, cognitive, physical, social), how does the measurable effect trend? RQ3: How are the ES of CL affected by moderator variables such as publication source, country, grade level, research design, sport content, teaching structure, intervention duration (weeks), publication year, sample size and study quality score (a unique variable)?
3 Materials and methods
3.1 Registration and protocol
This meta-analysis was registered in International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO), an international database for systematic reviews in health and related fields, under the registration number CRD42024532607 (Schiavo, 2019). The protocol was developed based on the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement (Page et al., 2021) for complete, accurate, and transparent reporting.
3.2 Eligibility criteria
The PICOS (Population, Intervention, Comparison, Outcomes, and Study) framework was utilized to develop precise inclusion and exclusion criteria (see Table 2; Amir-Behghadami and Janati, 2020). In order to minimize selection bias in line with Cochrane guidelines (Higgins et al., 2024), studies published in 2000 and later were included. This date represents the period when CL gained methodological consistency in PE and proved its applicability on a global scale (Casey and Goodyear, 2015). Although year of publication was added as a moderator variable to capture model-based developmental changes, temporal heterogeneity did not show statistical significance in preliminary analyses (see, Table 3). Thus, the 25-year timeframe covers the evolution of the pedagogical model while maintaining methodological robustness.

Table 3. CL and learning outcome: univariate analyses of variance for moderator variables (categorical).
3.3 Search strategy
The search strategy used descriptors such as the primary term ‘cooperative learning’ in conjunction with subcategories including ‘school,’ ‘class,’ ‘physical education,’ ‘physical activity,’ and ‘movement.’ The inclusion of ‘physical activity’ and ‘movement’ as related terms aimed to refine the scope of studies to those that, while reporting on CL in PE, also draw on contexts involving physical activity practices and movement to enrich the PE literature. Following this initial search, the identified studies were scrutinized for their relevance and suitability.
3.4 Information sources
The meta-analysis incorporated studies from diverse sources to ensure comprehensive analysis. The included literature comprised published articles, master theses, and doctoral dissertations in English, Spanish, and Turkish languages. The studies in English were sourced from ERIC, Google Scholar, ProQuest Dissertations and Theses, Psych ARTICLES, Psych INFO, Sport DISCUS, and WOS databases. The studies in Spanish were sourced from Dialnet, RECYT (Spanish Repository of Science and Technology), and TESEO (Consultation of the Doctoral Theses) databases. Sources in Spanish, in addition to English and Turkish, were included because, according to the review by Zhou et al. (2023), Spain has produced the highest number of publications on CL in PE, with 71 papers cited up to 404 times (Zhou et al., 2023). Spanish was the primary language of one of the authors (P.P.-G.) in this review. The studies in Turkish were sourced from ULAKBIM (Turkish Academic Network and Information Center) and YOK (Turkish Higher Education Council) Doctoral and Master Theses databases. Turksih was the primary language of six of the authors (H.B., Y.A., S.T., F.H.Y., B.C., and G.N.). Each source was last searched or consulted on June 2nd, 2024.
Although conference proceedings are widespread in the academic literature, the data shared in these sources exhibit significant heterogeneity in terms of methodological reliability, data accuracy and level of detail, making their inclusion in meta-analysis methodologically risky (Liu et al., 2017). Therefore, conference abstracts were excluded to maintain the internal validity of the analysis.
3.5 Selection process
Three authors (H.B., Y.A., and P.P.-G.) independently reviewed the titles and abstracts of the initial records and resolved any discrepancies through discussion until they reached a consensus. Subsequently, the authors worked in pairs to independently screen the titles and abstracts of all individual studies. In instances of disagreement, consensus regarding which studies to assess in full-text was reached through deliberation. This comprehensive review process aimed to verify the eligibility of each study according to the criteria for inclusion and exclusion detailed in Table 2. If needed, Y.A. was consulted for the ultimate decision-making authority. Subsequently, two authors (H.B. and P.P.-G.) conducted independent assessments of the full-text studies for inclusion (Waffenschmidt et al., 2019; Wang et al., 2020). Again, in cases of disagreement, consensus on inclusion or exclusion was reached through deliberation, with the Y.A. being consulted if needed (Bomhof-Roordink et al., 2019). The inter-coder reliability index for Cohen’s Kappa was computed as 0.86 (Cohen, 1960; Landis and Koch, 1977; Viera and Garrett, 2005), demonstrating substantial agreement.
3.6 Data collection process
A study-specific data extraction template was utilized for the data collection process. Two review authors (H.B. and P.P.-G.) employed this template to methodically extract data from eligible studies. The extracted data were systematically compared, with any discrepancies resolved through deliberation. Ş.T. then entered the data into EXCEL spreadsheet with various data items, ensuring accuracy through double-checking.
3.7 Data items
Data were collected regarding the author, country, grade level, intervention characteristics, learning outcomes in the affective, cognitive, phsyical, and social domains, participant characteristics, research design and features, source of publication, sports content, teaching structure, year of publication, and study quality score. Eligible outcomes were broadly categorized as learning outcomes in the four domains (affective, cognitive, physical, social). Any quantitative assessment of learning outcomes was eligible for inclusion.
The identification and standardization of data to be used in meta-analyses is a critical process. As emphasized by Cochrane (Higgins et al., 2024), these data are often reported in heterogeneous formats (mean, standard deviation, ES) and require methodological harmonization prior to analysis. In this study, in order to quantitatively synthesize the effect of the CL model on various learning outcomes in the affective, cognitive, physical and social domains, data on the sub-dimensions of each domain (e.g., motivation, self-efficacy, critical thinking, problem solving, psychomotor abilities, empathy, peer relations) were integrated in a systematic approach through a data combination formula proposed by Cochrane (Higgins et al., 2024). Inverse-variance weighting method, for continuous or dichotomous outcomes was used. The standardized z-scores calculated for each of these sub-dimensions were adopted as the basis for determining the combined ES of the respective domains. The contribution of each sub-dimension was balanced with weighted averages to derive an overall domain-specific effect value. This process ensured methodological consistency and increased comparability across studies.
3.8 Risk of bias assessment
Risk of bias in the result of the synthesis due to missing studies or results within studies was considered (Page et al., 2019). To assess small-study effects and bias in the included studies, a funnel plot was generated (refer to Figure 1). Additionally, the classic fail-safe number (Nfs) method (Khoury et al., 2013), Begg and Mazumdar (1994) rank correlation, and Egger et al.’s (1997) regression intercept were utilized. Two independent reviewers, H.B. and Y.A., conducted assessments for each study to identify potential errors in evaluations (Robson et al., 2019). They independently appraised the risk of bias and provided justifications for their judgments. Any inconsistencies in their assessments were resolved through deliborations, ensuring a consensus was reached.
3.8.1 Study quality assessment
The methodological rigor and reporting quality of each included study were appraised using the Medical Education Research Study Quality Instrument (MERSQI; Reed et al., 2007). The MERSQI involves 10 specific criteria organized into six primary dimensions: study design, sampling methods, data collection procedures, validity of assessment instruments, data analysis techniques, and outcomes assessment. In alignment with the PE context, the MERSQI was refined to specifically omit the assessment of potential healthcare-related outcomes. Following revisions, the instrument achieved a maximum score of 17, contrasting with the original scoring range of 5 to 18. The comprehensive methodological quality scores for each study are delineated in Table 4. The average study methodological quality score for the included studies was assessed to be 13.56, with a range of 12 to 16.
3.9 Effect measures
Standardized mean difference ES and their corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were utilized for continuous data (Hedges and Olkin, 2014). Hedges’s g was selected as the ES measure, calculated using means, sample sizes, and standard deviations derived from both pre- and post-tests of the experimental and control groups. Initially, this meta-analysis included 60 reports from 43 studies: 13 true experiments, 21 quasi-experiments, and 9 studies with unknown classifications, all employing a two-group design. To quantify the magnitude of group differences in standat deviation units, Cohen’s d was employed for classification (Cohen, 1988).
3.10 Synthesis methods
Meta-analysis techniques and procedures, aimed at synthesizing study effect estimates and their variances to generate a quantitative summary of results (Deeks et al., 2019), were conducted using the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (CMA) V4 software package (Borenstein et al., 2021) by two authors, H.B. and Y.A. Both the fixed-effect and random-effects meta-analysis models were also used in the study. The meta-analysis process was expanded to involve subgroup analyses examining learning outcomes in the affective, cognitive, physical, and social domains. Additionally, moderator analyses were conducted on six categorical variables: source of publication, country, grade level, research design, sport content, and teaching structure. Meta-regression analyses were also conducted on four continuous variables: duration of intervention in weeks, year of publication, sample size, and study quality score. These analyses aimed to elucidate the factors contributing to variability in results across included studies, with a specific focus on addressing statistical heterogeneity (Williams, 2012; Deeks et al., 2019). The presence of heterogeneity (variation in ES) across studies was assessed through inspection of a forest plot and calculation of Cochran’s Q statistic along with its corresponding I2 statistic. Confidence in this metric was evaluated using 95% uncertainty intervals (CIs) around the I2. Thresholds of >75% were used to identify significant heterogeneity, with additional consideration given to the uncertainty intervals and the spatial distribution on the forest plot (Huedo-Medina et al., 2006; Page et al., 2018).
4 Results
4.1 Study selection
A total of 10.083 records were identified through database searching. After removing duplicates, 9.803 records remained for screening. 43 studies, including 36 peer-reviewed articles (Huang, 2000; Lafont et al., 2007; Goudas and Magotsiou, 2009; Kiremitci and Dogan, 2010; Pehlivan and Alkan, 2010; Bayraktar, 2011; Mohseen et al., 2011; Fernández-Río et al., 2014; Darnis and Lafont, 2015; Mostafa, 2015; Benkhaled et al., 2015; Bensikaddour et al., 2015; Altınkök, 2017; Fernandez-Rio et al., 2017; Huang et al., 2017; Fernández and González-Mesa, 2018; Chiva-Bartoll et al., 2018; Norito et al., 2019; Cecchini Estrada et al., 2019; Engels and Freund, 2020; Siong and Ali, 2020; Winarni and Lutan, 2020; Fernandez-Rio and Casey, 2021; Salih et al., 2021; Shalal et al., 2021; Yang et al., 2021; Bermejo Díaz et al., 2021; Fadhul, 2022; Palau-Pamies et al., 2022; Bofill-Herrero et al., 2022; García-González et al., 2023; Hu, 2023; Turan et al., 2023; Abbood, 2024; Faozi et al., 2024; Cochon Drouet et al., 2024), 2 master’s theses (Gulay, 2008; Uresin, 2012), and 5 doctoral dissertations (Tuncel, 2006; Bahadir, 2011; Altinkok, 2012; Karavelioglu, 2012; Aka, 2014), were reviewed and included in the meta-analysis, with each source cited accordingly. No additional studies meeting the inclusion criteria (see Table 2) were found. In total, 171 studies were excluded from the meta-analysis, comprising 8 theses and 125 peer-reviewed articles in English, 5 theses and 28 peer-reviewed articles in Spanish, and 3 theses and 2 peer-reviewed articles in Turkish. The rationale for their exclusion is elaborated upon Table 2. Initially (with outliers), this meta-analysis included 43 individual studies and their 60 reports, see Table 4.
In order to prevent outliers from disproportionately affecting the analysis results in meta-analyses (Barnett and Lewis, 1994; Higgins et al., 2024), Hedges and Olkin (2014) stated that studies with standardized residuals outside the range ±3 can be considered as outliers. Accordingly, when some of the effects in the study were evaluated in terms of standardized residual values, it was determined that they deviated significantly from the pattern of the general dataset and their effects were quite high. The studies that were identified as outliers within the framework of the relevant criteria and excluded from the analysis are as follows: Mohseen et al. (2011), ES = 7.454; Palau-Pamies et al. (2022), ES = 9.344 and another effect from the same study, ES = 4.319; and Salih et al. (2021), ES = 6.279. Therefore, the ultimate meta-analysis stage comprised 56 reports from 40 studies, after excluding the outliers. The search and selection process is visually depicted through a flow diagram presented in Figure 2.

Figure 2. The PRISMA flow diagram for the meta-analysis (Page et al., 2021).
4.2 Study characteristics
Table 4 presents details for the included studies and their reports, including the learning outcome, ES, source of publication, country, grade level, sample size, research design, teaching structure, sport content, duration of intervention (in weeks), and study quality score.
4.3 Risk of bias
Upon excluding the outliers (see Table 4), funnel plot assessment and the collective analyses of Egger’s regression intercept, Begg and Mazumdar rank correlation, and Nfs consistently indicated a low likelihood of publication bias. Figure 1 illustrates a funnel plot demonstrating a predominantly symmetric distribution of the 56 effects, well within the expected range and centered around its axis. Egger’s regression intercept was 1.09, with a non-significant two-tailed p-value of 0.25. The rank correlation test reported a Kendall’s tau of 0.11 with a non-significant two-tailed p-value of 0.22. The Nfs value of 3,269 (p = 0.001) far exceeded the fail-safe criterion of 290 ([5 × 56] + 10; Figure 1).
4.4 Results of individual studies
Each report of the included studies for all continuous outcomes provided summary statistics for each group, along with an effect estimate and its precision, visualized in a structured forest plot (see Figure 3). The forest plot displays the summary statistics for each included report of the individual studies in the meta-analysis. These statistics include the ES, standard error, variance, lower limit, upper limit, z-value, and p-value for both the CL intervention and control groups. Additionally, the forest plot presents the risk ratio and its 95% confidence interval for continuous outcomes (Figure 3).
4.5 Results of syntheses
The present meta-analysis of 56 independent ESs, including a total of 3.985 students, yielded an overall ES: 0.459 (95% CI = 0.324 to 0.595; z = 6.64, p < 0.001), indicating a significant moderate positive effect of CL on learning outcomes. Cochrane’s Q was 318 and significant (p < 0.001, I2 = 82.7), suggesting heterogeneous ESs across studies (Table 4).
The subgroup analysis of 18 independent ES, involving a total of 1.919 participants, yielded an overall ES: 0.304 (95% CI [0.71, 1.107], p < 0.05), indicating a significant small positive effect of CL on affective learning. The subgroup analysis of 8 independent ES, involving a total of 962 participants, yielded an overall ES: 0.589 (95% CI [0.71, 1.107], p < 0.05), indicating a significant moderate positive effect of CL on cognitive learning. The subgroup analysis of 24 independent ES, involving a total of 1.859 participants, yielded an overall ES: of 0.471 (95% CI [0.241, 0.700], p < 0.001), indicating a significant moderate positive effect of CL on physical learning. Lastly, the subgroup analysis of 10 independent ES, involving a total of 1.127 participants, yielded an overall ES: 0.612 (95% CI [0.249, 0.974], p < 0.01), indicating a significant moderate positive effect of CL on social learning. A random-effects meta-analysis model was adopted instead of a fixed-effect meta-analysis model due to the I2 statistic exceeding 50% (Page et al., 2018).
4.5.1 Results of additional moderator analyses
The substantial heterogeneity in ESs (as illustrated in Table 5) prompted explorations via moderator and meta-regression analyses to uncover variables potentially influencing the relationship between independent and dependent variables (Table 5). The findings of the moderator and meta-regression analyses are illustrated in Tables 3, 6.

Table 6. CL and learning outcome: univariate analyses of variance for moderator variables (categorical).
4.5.1.1 Source of publication
No significant differences in ESs were observed between articles and theses (QBET = 1.817, df = 1, p > 0.05; see Table 6). Therefore, the source of publication did not moderate the effect of CL.
4.5.1.2 Country
No significant differences in ESs were observed among countries (QBET = 0.761, df = 2, p > 0.05; see Table 6). Therefore, country did not moderate the effect of CL.
4.5.1.3 Grade level
No significant differences in ESs were observed among grade levels. (QBET = 1.008, df = 3, p > 0.05; see Table 6). Therefore, grade level did not moderate the effect of CL.
4.5.1.4 Research design
No significant differences in ESs were observed between true experimental design and quasi-experimental design (QBET = 1.074, df = 1, p > 0.05; see Table 6). Therefore, the research design did not moderate the effect of CL.
4.5.1.5 Sport content
No significant differences in ESs were observed among sports contents (QBET = 4.877, df = 3, p > 0.05; see Table 6). Therefore, sports content did not moderate the effect of CL.
4.5.1.6 Teaching structure
No significant differences in ESs were observed among teaching structures (QBET = 1.257, df = 2, p > 0.05; see Table 6). Therefore, the teaching structure did not moderate the effect of CL.
4.5.1.7 Duration of intervention
Meta-regression of g onto duration of intervention (in weeks) did not show significant effects of CL (QModel [1, k = 52] = 0.36, p > 0.05; see Table 3).
4.5.1.8 Year of publication
Meta-regression of g onto publication year did not show significant effects of CL (QModel [1, k = 54] = 0.36, p > 0.05; see Table 3).
4.5.1.9 Sample size
Meta-regression of g onto sample size did not show significant effects of CL (QModel [1, k = 52] = 0.47, p > 0.05; see Table 3).
4.5.1.10 Study quality score
Meta-regression of g onto study quality score did not show significant effects of CL (QModel [1, k = 52] = 0.64, p > 0.05; see Table 3).
5 Discussion
The purpose of this meta-analysis was to show a quantitative synthesis on the effect of CL on students’ learning outcomes in the affective, cognitive, physical and social domains within PE context. The first research question was: (RQ1) What is the effect of CL on students’ learning outcomes in the affective, cognitive, physical, and social domains in PE? This meta-analysis, including 40 studies with 3.985 PE students and 56 ESs, revealed a significantly positive overall effect of CL at 0.459 on students’ learning outcomes in the affective, cognitive, physical, and social domains. This avant-garde quantitative summary supports the use of CL as a dynamic student-centered model that can contribute to “educationally beneficial” (p. 978) learning outcomes of PE (Kirk, 2013) with a comprehensive point of view. Moving away from meta-analytic evidences of Fernández-Espínola et al. (2020) and Liu and Lipowski (2021), which partially focuses on only one affective parameter (intrinsic motivation) but is limited by univariate methodological narrowing, limited sample population and heterogeneous dataset, this study overcomes the methodological fragmentation, which is frequently criticized in the PE literature. By quantitatively synthesizing evidence of multi-domain effects embeddedness, which has been ignored by all previous studies, the present meta-analysis adds theoretical depth to the potential of CL as a student-centered pedagogical model.
The conceptual ambiguities and implementation challenges pointed out by studies such as Fernandez-Rio and Casey (2021) and Zhou et al. (2023) are partially offset by the consistency of CL’s ESs in this meta-analysis regardless of cultural/contextual factors. For example, while Zach et al. (2023) emphasized the difficulty of implementing CL strategies by teachers, the findings of this study indicate that CL’s capacity to support student-level learning achievements is consistent despite instructional limitations, pointing to the pedagogical flexibility of the model. Furthermore, although most of the reviewed studies were based on short-term interventions (e.g., 6–8 weeks), the moderate ESs in this meta-analysis provide indirect evidence for the existence of CL’s sustained effect and advocate for a necessity of long-term research as accentuated by scholars (Casey and Goodyear, 2015; Bores-García et al., 2021).
The second research question was: (RQ2) When examining the hierarchical effects of CL on students’ learning outcomes in a cross-domain analytical framework (affective, cognitive, physical, social), how does the measurable effect trend? The meta-analytic findings revealed that the effect of CL on students’ learning outcomes exhibited statistically significant differences across learning domains in PE: The highest standardized ES was observed for social domain (0.612), followed by a decreasing trend for cognitive (ES = 0.589), physical (ES = 0.471) and affective (ES = 0.304) domains. This hierarchical distribution reinforces the notion that the collective knowledge construction mechanisms underlying CL’s superiority in social learning and the limitations in affective learning may be due to individual affective architecture deficits. With a particular emphasis on the two learning domains at opposite ends of the ES spectrum, future research should be organized along three axes: (a) dynamic neurocognitive mapping of affective-social interactions in CL processes with EEG/FNIRS-based neuroimaging, (b) scaling the effect weights of moderator variables such as affective literacy and empathic sensitivity with meta-analytic regression models, and (c) translating the affective efficacy of CL into multilayer optimization algorithms through integrated analysis of neural data streams and pedagogical big data.
5.1 Social learning
CL is a multidimensional pedagogical model (Casey and Kirk, 2024) that stands out for its potential to optimize and prioritize social learning outcomes in the PE context (Casey and Goodyear, 2015; Bores-García et al., 2021), but requires in-depth analysis with its methodological and epistemological layers. Meta-analytic evidence (N = 1.127, 10 studies) revealed that CL achieved a moderate ES (0.612) in the social domain, which was significantly superior to affective, cognitive and physical domains. This finding not only reinforces the multi-causal approach of CL in the context of PE, which places the mechanisms of social learning at the epistemic center of the pedagogical model (Casey and Goodyear, 2015; Bores-García et al., 2021; Schulze and Von Huth, 2022; Zach et al., 2023; Zhou et al., 2023); It also empirically validates the functional validity of social constructivist paradigms through (a) the optimization of role allocation in zones of proximal development, (b) the institutionalization of learning environments based on symbolic interactionism, and (c) the cognitive-affective transfer of social capital accumulation (Vygotsky, 1978). The high ES in the social domain can be explained by CL’s ontological capacity to systematically mobilize dynamics such as common goal orientation, role differentiation and social capital accumulation (Goodyear et al., 2014; Casey and Goodyear, 2015; Bores-García et al., 2021; Zach et al., 2023). However, these findings suggest that researchers and practitioners should conceptualize CL as a foundational pedagogical model strategically hybridizable with complementary frameworks, centering social learning outcomes as its core efficacy driver, rather than treating it as a universal solution detached from systemic integration. In line with the ES of 0.612 in the social domain, the team identity emphasis of the SE model (Siedentop, 2001; Siedentop et al., 2019) and the ethical leadership focus of the TPSR model (Hellison, 2011) can deepen CL’s social interaction mechanisms with social participation and social justice dimensions. Moreover, unlike the one-way communication paradigm of traditional approaches such as Mosston’s (1966) Command Style (Style A), CL’s multifocal structure stands out with its potential to automatize students’ communication skills. In this context, the sustainability of CL’s social superiority is directly related to practitioners’ ability to manage group heterogeneity.
According to Zach et al.’s (2023) discriminant analyses, in the social domain, combined strategies showed a moderate positive effect (0.402), while Jigsaw showed an unexpected negative effect (−0.403); although the classification accuracy was 79.5%, the risk of social exhaustion of long-term interventions (−0.367) emphasizes the need for contextual optimization. Furthermore, the highest ES appeared in the social domain reinforced the claim that CL has the potential to promote social capital accumulation in school settings through the synergistic dynamic of peer interaction and collective decision-making processes fostering democratic participation, multidimensional social gains such as multicultural tolerance on the axis of interpersonal and affective development, ethical empathy, and the institutionalization of compassion-oriented behavioral patterns (León et al., 2021; Ortuondo et al., 2022, 2023; Cochon Drouet et al., 2024; Kim and Park, 2024). García-González et al. (2023) demonstrated that high-structured CL significantly increased these prosocial behaviors in younger adolescents, while older adolescents showed minimal changes, underscoring age-specific and structural nuances in CL’s effectiveness. This process is not only limited to in-school interactions, but also facilitates the transfer of communicative flexibility and strategic collaboration skills (Jacobs et al., 2017; Barlett, 2019), which are among the indispensable competencies of the 21st century, to out of school settings based on the principle of interdisciplinary permeability, thus acting as a catalyst for sustainable social transformation. From this point of view, it may be asserted that CL can be used as a pedagogical antidote to problems such as peer bullying. However, a theoretical counter-thesis claiming that the educational value of CL should be sought in motivational dynamics rather than social learning outcomes (Montoya et al., 2020) points to the epistemological paradox of this approach, warning that the instrumentalization of self-determined motivation (Sierra-Díaz et al., 2019) and intrinsic motivation (Fernández-Espínola et al., 2020; Liu and Lipowski, 2021) may undermine pedagogical integrity. This dialectical tension creates a critical debate on how to strike a balance between the social effects of CL and its motivational underpinnings in CL-based program designs. In this context, CL implementation should balance social gains with students’ intrinsic motivation and avoid overly structured group work.
The transformative role of CL in the context of PE goes beyond mere instrumental skills, emerging as a multi-layered pedagogical model intertwined with a sociomoral ethical framework. Embedded within this model-based approach (Casey and Kirk, 2024), as Dyson et al. (2004) emphasize, is the synergy between the dynamics of interpersonal interaction and high-quality social gains. This synergy supports the development of functional skills such as the exchange of ideas, the construction of shared meaning and active listening (André et al., 2011; Bensikaddour et al., 2015), while at the same time building a sociomoral paradigm based on fairness, mutual responsibility and collective belonging, as Wallhead and Dyson (2017) and Sánchez-Hernández et al. (2018) point out. Casey and Goodyear’s (2015) conceptualization of ‘pedagogical empathy’ embodies this dual structure, suggesting that CL functions as a bridge that nurtures mechanisms of mutual care and respect support among students.
In essence, this multidimensional architecture of CL combines the instrumental (skills-based) and ethical (value-oriented) paradigm into a synergistic whole, as suggested by Dyson et al. (2020). This synthesis not only supports affective, cognitive, and physical development in a holistic axis, but also transforms PE into a ‘social microcosm’ by reconstructing learning processes with democratic participation, social justice and ethical sensitivity. Thus, beyond being a methodological tool, CL functions as an epistemological framework that transforms both the individual and collective identity construction of students.
5.2 Cognitive learning
A subgroup analysis of eight studies involving 962 students revealed that CL had a moderate positive effect on cognitive learning outcomes. This impact was evidenced by a modarate ES of 0.589, ranking as the second-highest among the analyzed results. This synthesized quantitative evidence aligns with studies demonstrating that CL facilitates cognitive learning, including the ability to analyze team strategies, enhance decision-making and problem solving skills, and engage in meaning-making activities (Dyson et al., 2016; Gorucu, 2016; Bodsworth and Goodyear, 2017; Fernández and González-Mesa, 2018; Nopembri et al., 2019).
Although the ES of CL in the cognitive domain (0.589) makes sense with peer-mediated knowledge transfer and modeling processes in Bandura’s (1986) social cognitive theory, its low value compared to the social domain (ES = 0.612) may be due to CL’s ontological focus on shared meaning construction while marginalizing individual cognitive deepening. However, this quantitative finding proves its practical relevance in real classroom settings through critical dynamics. CL’s structured group interactions increase the pedagogical intensity of lesson time by enabling multiple simultaneous learning outcomes (social-cognitive; O’Leary and Griggs, 2010; Casey and Goodyear, 2015) compared to teacher-centered approaches. For example, in a basketball tactical analysis activity, CL groups parallel strategic decision-making (cognitive) and peer feedback (social) processes, enabling double gains in a single class period. The heterogeneous group structure (Slavin, 1990) turns students’ cognitive style differences (analytical-intuitive) into an advantage; when hybridized with TGfU, CL’s social dynamics strengthen inclusiveness by enabling tactical problem solving skills (Bunker and Thorpe, 1982; Ortiz et al., 2023), strategic engagement of even low motor-skill students. The significance of ES = 0.589 lies in the fact that CL promotes not only cognitive gains but also 21st century skills such as social–emotional learning and ethical reasoning (Dyson and Casey, 2016). CL practices integrated with Mosston’s (1966) Exploratory Style (Style E) expose students to multi-layered cognitive-social processes such as open-ended hypothesis testing and managing in-group conflicts of opinion. This triple synergy transforms CL’s ES = 0.589 from a mere statistical output to an indicator of instructional sustainability. The critical point is that researchers and practitioners should adopt structured flexibility, adapting the social framework of the CL in such a way as to personalize the levels of cognitive challenge according to student profiles. For example, the cyclical distribution of roles such as observer-strategist and practitioner-critic within CL units in a PE lesson nurtures both motor skill development and meta-cognitive awareness. In this regard, the cognitive ES value of CL is evidence of its capacity to function as a cognitive catalyst of social learning, but this potential only becomes possible when instructional design is realized by practitioners who can manage the dialectic between quantitative data and classroom ontology.
Vygotsky’s (1978) conceptualization of ‘the zone of proximal development’ radically illuminates the cognitive developmental mechanisms of CL: Peer feedback and collective knowledge construction, not only through ‘more knowledgeable others’ that exceed individual capacity, but also by creating distributed cognitive systems (Hutchins, 1995), enable learners to socially negotiate meaning through symbolic means (language, argument structures). In this regard, it can be emphasized that the ‘the zone of proximal development’ is not a static territor but a dynamic space of interaction (De Costa, 2007). For example, conflicting perspectives in CL groups may force students to experience cognitive contradiction (Piaget, 2013), triggering the restructuring of existing schemas. Critical here, however, is how Vygotsky’s emphasis on social interaction synergizes with Flavell’s (1979) model of metacognitive regulation: CL should provide students with not only strategic decision-making (e.g., resource allocation or task division) but also socio-metacognitive awareness (Salomon and Perkins, 1998)—that is, the ability to ‘think about how we think as a group’.
The critical role of cognitive learning in CL was examined by Gorucu (2016) in the context of constructivist pedagogy, emphasizing that problem-solving activities develop students’ ability to build collective intelligence and cognitive adaptation through interdependence dynamics. In the samle line, Bodsworth and Goodyear (2017) study, which draws attention to the potential of digital tools within PE context to instrumentalize reflective learning environments in CL. The researchers argued that technology deepens students’ capacity to ‘learn how to learn’ by triggering not only knowledge transfer but also meta-reflection (high-level analysis of the learning process) and epistemic curiosity (motivation to access knowledge). This dynamic nature of CL is explained in Bores-García et al.’s (2021) systeamtic review through the concepts of active subjectivity and structured reflection. For example, students are forced to question their own thinking strategies and gain cognitive flexibility while solving complex problems through social interaction. This can be explained by the highly reflective components inherent in CL tasks (e.g., argument revision in group discussions or iterative testing of the solution prototype). The student’s participation in the process of CL as an ‘actor’ fosters not only knowledge acquisition but also self-regulated learning (Zimmerman, 2002) and socio-cognitive conflict resolution skills.
In the context of PE, to systematically operationalize the effect of CL on cognitive learning outcomes in a measurable and feasible framework, researchers and practitioners should integrate systemic thinking (Senge, 2006) and epistemic cognition (Chinn et al., 2014) when designing interdisciplinary problem scenarios; and develop scientific logic and social negotiation skills through reasoning diagrams in unconstrained problems such as climate change. In digital hybridization strategies, using the principles of cognitive load (Sweller, 1988), social network analysis (Wasserman and Faust, 1994), and visual collaborative brainstorming with tools such as Padlet or Miro can optimize distributed attention management by visualizing the flow of ideas within the group. Metacognitive reflection mechanisms should adopt the perspective of double loop learning (Argyris and Schön, 1997), focusing on the questions ‘what did we do?’ (first loop) and ‘how did we think?’ (second loop) in group communications and bridging Zimmerman’s (2002) self-regulation cycle (planning-monitoring-evaluation) between individual and collective reflection. However, social epistemology (Goldman, 2019) emphasizes the construction of knowledge in a network of social practices, transforming education into a process in which ‘communities of learners’ (Lave and Wenger, 1991) pursue common goals; this transformation requires understanding CL not as a technical tool but as a pedagogy focused on democratic participation and critical consciousness (Freire, 1970).
5.3 Physical learning
In the subgroup analysis covering 24 studies with a total of 1.859 students, it was found that CL had a low-to-moderate positive effect on learning outcomes in the physical domain, demonstrading with the third-largest ES (0.471). However, this finding is valuable and functional even under realistic school conditions. Under time constraints, CL optimizes multiple gains by integrating physical skill development with social interactions (role distribution, peer feedback; Dyson and Casey, 2016). In heterogeneous classrooms (Slavin, 1996), inclusive participation is promoted with high and low ability students taking on model-leader and observer-learner roles. For non-expert teachers, CL compensates for technical deficiencies with social self-management by providing a pedagogical template with clear role allocations (Casey and Goodyear, 2015). This effect is in line with the goal of sustainable efficiency in less than ideal conditions. Therefore, CL’s moderate effect triggers indirect physical gains such as social capital accumulation and motivational boosts even in resource constraints. In addition, this finding confirms the role of CL in enhancing motor skill development and physical performance, while demonstrating a significant superiority compared to traditional individual and competitive pedagogical approaches. Casey and Goodyear (2015) asserted that within CL-oriented group discussions, students’ deliberative engagement in the co-construction of movement techniques not only stimulates metacognitive consciousness but also transmutes the interactive process into a dynamic, self-reinforcing synergy. This emergent pedagogical phenomenon, they argue, operates through a dialectical interplay wherein the iterative refinement of physical praxis (e.g., kinesthetic precision, proprioceptive calibration) reciprocally scaffolds the evolution of strategic cognitive schemata (e.g., problem-solving heuristics, adaptive decision-making frameworks). In this context, the comparatively attenuated ES of CL in the physical domain (0.471) may be epistemologically rooted in the neurobiological and biomechanical exigencies inherent to motor skill acquisition—specifically, the non-negotiable role of individualized repetition, proprioceptive feedback loops, and task-specific neuromuscular plasticity. This ontological constraint positions CL not as a pedagogical panacea but as a contingent adjunct, necessitating strategic integration within frameworks prioritizing automatization. For instance, the SE model’s symbiotic incorporation of CL (e.g., Fernandez-Rio and Casey, 2021) could theoretically engender a dual-axis developmental matrix, harmonizing technical skill refinement with collective tactical coherence through socially embedded praxis. Analogously, Mosston’s (1966) Practice Style (Stil B), with its regimented iterative scaffolding, might serve as a counterbalance to CL’s dialogic dynamism, creating a dialectical pedagogy oscillating between mechanistic precision and social constructivism. However, such hybridization demands temporal granularity and hierarchical task architecture from educators; failure to calibrate these variables risks metastasizing into pedagogical dissonance, wherein socio-cognitive and sensorimotor objectives engage in zero-sum competition. Crucially, in contexts marred by temporal constraints, heterogeneous skill stratification, and reliance on generalist instructors, this duality becomes exponentially precarious. Without scaffolded curricular blueprints or micro-sequenced progressions, non-specialist educators may inadvertently dilute both social cohesion and skill mastery, rendering CL’s integration a performative compromise rather than a synergistic augmentation.
With regard to physical learning, CL is conceptually grounded in the synergistic interplay of peer interaction that fosters knowledge co-construction (e.g., Dyson et al., 2016), schema optimization that enhances cognitive adaptation (Schmidt, 1975), social motivation that promotes goal-directed engagement (Bandura, 1986; Bandura, 1986), and the mirror-neuron system that activates motor responses through observation (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). In accordance with these theoretical accounts, a meta-analytic review by Stanne et al. (1999) found that peer-assisted learning (not embedded within a direct CL framework) demonstrated a statistically significant superiority in pedagogical effectiveness on movement skill acquisition (ES = 0.53) compared to individual effort-based or competitive instructional models (ES = 0.36). Furthermore, studies examining the effects of CL on physical learning present a panorama shaped by methodological pluralism but with some epistemological gaps. Quantitative approaches (Lafont et al., 2007; Luo et al., 2013; Benkhaled et al., 2015; Darnis and Lafont, 2015; Altınkök, 2017; Huang et al., 2017) have statistically validated the role of CL in social skill development - through dynamics such as dialog, listening and collective understanding building - emphasizing the positive correlation of physical activity with motor skills and social bonds, but compressing the unmeasurable phenomena of body expression (emotional codes of gestures, bodily metaphors) into a reductionist template. In contrast, qualitative studies (Barrett, 2005; Callado et al., 2014; Bensikaddour et al., 2015) have revealed the contextual depth of CL in physical interactions and explained how individuals’ bodily experiences are intertwined with belonging, self-confidence and creative expression through micro-ethnographic analyses, but have been limited to small samples whose generalizability has been questioned. Although mixed-method research (Lee, 2014) blends the structural rigor of quantitative data with the narrative richness of qualitative findings in an attempt to overcome this dichotomy, revealing the multidimensional effects of CL (social learning reflected in motor skills, group synchronization fostering emotional resilience), it fails to place the symbolic language of the body (creative physical communication, performative expression) in a systematic theoretical framework, thus the hegemonic focus on sport and motor skills overshadows the potential of the body as a social-affective signifying tool. This methodological fragmentation coincides with Bores-García et al. (2021) critique, pointing out that CL practices, by constructing the body only as a technical object, neglect its constitutive and transformative role in social interaction, and that this deficit narrows critical dimensions such as creativity, identity construction and cultural diversity in educational processes. This deficiency can be reconsidered with the critical argument from Merleau-Ponty’s (2012) bodily phenomenology perspective that motor skills are not a mere technical acquisition but represent a form of bodily consciousness integrated with social-cultural practices.
5.4 Affective learning
The subgroup analysis of the present meta-analysis covering 18 studies with a total of 1.919 students revealed that CL had a low positive effect on learning outcomes in the affective domain (ES = 0.304). This finding is somewhat consistent with the low ES of intrinsic motivation (0.380) found by Fernández-Espínola et al. (2020) and Liu and Lipowski (2021), indicating that the CL has limited but statistically significant potential to enhance students’ affective learning within different PE settings. We believe that the seemingly low ES is not a superficial weakness, but may be due to the long-term, time-consuming nature of affective development that requires contextual support. This may also stem from a misalignment between collective group dynamics (collective synergy) and individual affective processes (affective architecture). In particular, the autonomous nature of intrinsic motivation, the person-specific trajectory of self-efficacy, and the cognitive isolation of affective self-regulation may play a critical role in determining the ontological boundaries of CL. This paradox may force CL into a radical hybridization with the TPSR model. TPSR’s ethical autonomy and self-esteem-oriented framework can combine CL’s social interaction with layers of affective signification in a fragile synchrony (Hellison, 2011; Shen et al., 2022). Dewey (1986)‘s principles of experiential learning and Rogers’ person-centered approach can provide affective deepening by integrating emotional diaries or self-reflection protocols into CL processes. Mosston’s (1966) Individual Program Style (Style I) brings the group dynamics of CL into dialectical tension with affective individuality, reconciling collective belonging and personal interiority on a fragile equilibrium. However, in contexts where empathic leadership skills are not systematically instilled, this integration may risk turning affective superficiality and social performativity into a symbiotic vicious circle. Especially when working with groups of students with low affective literacy and educators lacking pedagogical sensitivity, the affective integration of CL can be reduced to an artificial ‘theater of emotions’, turning the effect into an ontological illusion.
Wang (2012) explained the transformative effect of CL on dynamics such as motivation and self-confidence by synthesizing Johnson and Johnson’s (2009) and Slavin’s (1996) views, emphasizing that especially collective achievement experiences and synergistic goal settings within the group increase students’ intrinsic motivation. When contextualized within Bandura’s (1986) social cognitive theory it is seen that peer feedback triggers self-efficacy construction and individual motivation is integrated into collective goals through social modeling. However, the way CL triggers affective mechanisms is indirect rather than direct; Zach et al. (2023), while proving the link of CL’s Jigsaw and Combined strategies with affective outcomes, pointed out that Learning Teams produced this association relatively fewer, drawing attention to the need for pedagogical design to be goal-oriented. This is consistent with Deci and Ryan’s (2000) self-determination theory: CL’s low ES may be due to a failure to balance the needs for autonomy, competence and relatedness. For example, the motivation of ‘unenthusiastic’ students in leadership roles observed by Goodyear et al. (2014) can be explained by supporting autonomy, while Slavin’s (1996) goal structuring meets the need for competence and Johnson and Johnson’s (2009) positive dependency principle meets the need for relatedness.
The limited effect potential of CL is further complicated by methodological challenges: Casey and Goodyear’s (2015) critique of ‘anecdotal evidence’ points to the shallowness of studies that neglect the temporal variability of affective learning and the role of cultural context. Although the large sample (N = 1.919) and heterogeneous subgroups of the meta-analysis partially explain the inconsistencies, social desirability bias in self-report measures and cross-sectional designs stand out as factors that suppress ES. These methodological gaps are also related to Vygotsky’s (1978) concept of zone of proximal development: Group interactions allow students to internalize their affective development through ‘peer scaffolding’, but cross-sectional studies cannot capture the temporal dynamics of this process. Dyson et al. (2020) asserted that the developmental dynamics of the sociomoral effects of CL (fairness, active listening) cannot be fully grasped due to the lack of longitudinal evidence.
The role of cultural context on the effectiveness of CL is critical in Hofstede’s (2011) collectivism–individualism tension axis. Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) ecological systems theory further deepens this context: In collectivist cultures, affective learning can be enhanced when the internalization of group goals (macrosystem) is aligned with the learner’s role models in society (mesosystem). Walad et al. (2019) observed that the Jigsaw strategy breaks the dominance of powerful students and ensures equal participation, leading to progress in social communication and attitude development. This finding is in line with Freire’s (1970) critical pedagogy perspective: In-group role distribution can transform power dynamics by offering students who are marginalized in traditional settings (e.g., female students) an ‘emancipatory space for dialog’. Casey and Fernandez-Rio (2019) contended that how this effect differs in the context of cultural heterogeneity should be tested with models of glocal adaptation.
In addition to the CL’s low ES in the affective domain (0.380), the subjective, personal, multidimensional, and complicated nature of affective learning (Pope, 2005) reveals the need for hybrid pedagogies beyond traditional CL models. The integration of social–emotional learning (SEL) and CL can create an ecosystem that nurtures self-evaluation and social justice perspectives; however, the success of this integration depends on restructuring teacher education to support the affective domain of CL. The learning outcomes in the affective domain of CL link with Seligman’s (2011) five domains of well-being: Positive Emotions, Engagement, Relationships, Meaning, and Accomplishment (PERMA): Group synergy (Relationships) and collective achievement (Achievement) can trigger meaningful learning (Meaning) and pleasure or elusive feelings (Positive Emotion) in students. O’Leary and Griggs (2010) and Baena-Morales et al. (2020) emphasized the need for integrated frameworks that center intrinsic motivation, self-efficacy, and perceptions of social justice rather than technical models of collaboration for this optimization. Consequently, the affective potential of CL can only be unlocked through interdisciplinary perspectives, methodological depth, and contextual sensitivity.
The third research question was: (RQ3) How are the ES of CL affected by moderator variables such as publication source, country, grade level, research design, sport content, teaching structure, intervention duration (weeks), publication year, sample size and study quality score (a unique variable)? In this study, the ES of CL did not differ significantly (p > 0.05) in any of the moderator variables examined, suggesting that the structural components of CL’s pedagogical effectiveness are based on the epistemological foundations of social constructivism theory rather than contextual or methodological conditions. As highlighted by Casey and Goodyear (2015) and Bores-García et al. (2021), the effect of CL on learning outcomes in the affective (anecdotally), physical, cognitive and social domains is a reflection of the model’s universal principles. This is consistent with similar study findings being observed across different geographies (e.g., conceptual uncertainties in China, Dyson et al., 2022) or developmental stages. However, the operational reductionist definition of moderators (e.g., broad categorization of ‘sport content’) may have masked the influence of context-specific dynamics (cultural collectivism, implicit teacher competence), creating an artificial impression of consistency.
The non-significant meta-regression results for intervention duration and sample size (Q > 0.05) suggest that the mechanisms of action of CL may be independent of time or scale, but related to critical thresholds (e.g., minimum time required for the formation of social cohesion). This finding is in line with Fernández-Espínola et al.’s (2020) and Liu and Lipowski’s (2021) warnings about heterogeneity in intrinsic motivation, revealing the limits of measuring CL’s non-linear pedagogical dynamics (initial rapid social adaptation, followed by plateaus) with traditional linear models. Consequently, this coherence paradigm highlights the potential of CL as a transdisciplinary pedagogy, necessitating the use of micro-moderators (e.g., teacher-student trust index, cultural collectivism score) and neurocognitive measures (neural correlates of joint attention) in future research. The highly controlled hybrid designs proposed by Zach et al. (2023) and Zhou et al. (2023) could bridge the epistemic gap between theory and practice by testing the contextual flexibility of CL through a dialectical synthesis between the Vygotsky’s (1978) ‘Zone of Proximal Development’ and the Gibson’s (1986) Affordances Theory.
5.5 Limitations and future research directions
The findings from this meta-analysis reveal a methodologically sound body of research on the effect of CL on students’ learning outcomes in PE. However, there are a number of critical issues that need to be identified to fill the gaps in the literature. Due to the authors’ mastery of only English, Spanish and Turkish, this study excludes publications in other languages. The inclusion of multilingual research teams in future systematic reviews and meta-analyses may contribute to enriching the CL literature with interdisciplinary and intercultural perspectives. Furthermore, there is a need for more comprehensive analyses to elucidate the causal relationships underlying the mechanisms of CL’s effect on PE learning outcomes (affective, cognitive, physical, social). In this context, structural equation modeling such as critical path analysis can be used as a methodological tool to uncover causal links between knowledge components and CL interventions. The meta-synthesis method that can be applied to integrate qualitative findings will allow for a multidimensional conceptualization of the phenomenon.
There can be a significant time gap between data collection and publication, which increases the risk of systematic bias in trend analyses based on publication year. To address this methodological weakness, it is essential that researchers transparently report the chronology of data collection and that journal editorial policies enforce this standard. Such regulations would constitute an epistemological step that would increase the validity of literature reviews examining temporal dynamics. Although the CL studies were conducted in 12 different countries (Algeria, China, Egypt, France, Germany, Greece, Indonesia, Iraq, Malaysia, Spain and Türkiye), the limited geographical and cultural diversity makes the universal generalizability of the findings questionable. Comparative studies in different sociocultural contexts are needed to reach globally valid conclusions. However, the current methodological limitations do not invalidate the main arguments of this meta-analysis.
6 Conclusion
The ES hierarchy of the CL model on students’ learning outcomes in the social (ES = 0.612) > cognitive (ES = 0.589) > physical (ES = 0.471) > affective (ES = 0.304) domains within different PE contexts systematically shows how CL’s social constructivism-based epistemology transforms learning processes, particularly through the mechanisms of peer dialog and collective goal orientation. In light of these findings, opportunities emerge for teachers to strengthen collaboration by developing pedagogical designs that center on social interaction; for curriculum developers to build interdisciplinary programs that optimize learning outcomes through the collective synergy of CL; for policymakers to ground school climate policies that support multiple development in a hierarchical context with CL’s empirical findings; and for researchers to deeply analyze the underlying causes of ESs (e.g., high ES in social learning or low ES in affective learning). This essay offers divers stakeholders the possibility to restructure educational ecosystems with strategic interventions based on quantitative data and to integrate the principles of social constructivism at the micro–macro level. Thus, the spectrum of CL’s effect transcends theoretical assumptions and provides the analytical ground for a scalable and sustainable transformation in PE practice.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.
Author contributions
HB: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft. YA: Data curation, Investigation, Resources, Writing – original draft. ST: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. FY: Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft. BC: Investigation, Resources, Writing – original draft. GN: Data curation, Project administration, Resources, Writing – original draft. PP-G: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – original draft. LA: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Prince Sultan University for its support.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Abbood, A. M. H. (2024). Effectiveness of cooperative learning using multimedia in some physical abilities and basic skills for junior players in basketball. ASEAN J. Phys. Educ. Sport Sci. 3, 7–16.
Aka, S. T. (2014). Effect of different educational models on volleyball training. Trabzon: University of Karadeniz Teknik.
Altinkok, M. (2012). Investigating the impact of physical education classes based on cooperative learning method on the development of fundamental motor skills and problem-solving skills of 9–10 year-old children. İstanbul: Univesity of Marmara.
Altınkök, M. (2017). The effect of movement education based on cooperative learning method on the development of basic motor skills of primary school 1st grade learners. J. Balt. Sci. Educ. 16, 241–249. doi: 10.33225/jbse/17.16.241
Amir-Behghadami, M., and Janati, A. (2020). Population, intervention, comparison, outcomes and study (PICOS) design as a framework to formulate eligibility criteria in systematic reviews. Emerg. Med. J. 37:387. doi: 10.1136/emermed-2020-209567
André, A., Deneuve, P., and Louvet, B. (2011). Cooperative learning in physical education and acceptance of students with learning disabilities. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 23, 474–485. doi: 10.1080/10413200.2011.580826
Araújo, R., Mesquita, I., and Hastie, P. A. (2014). Review of the status of learning in research on sport education: future research and practice. J. Sports Sci. Med. 13:846.
Argyris, C., and Schön, D. A. (1997). Organizational learning: a theory of action perspective. Rev. Esp. Invest. Sociol. 77/78:345.
Aygun, Y., Boke, H., Yagin, F. H., Tufekci, S., Murathan, T., and Gencay, E. (2024). Emotional and social outcomes of the teaching personal and social responsibility model in physical education: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Child. Aust. 11:459. doi: 10.3390/children11040459
Baena-Morales, S., Jerez-Mayorga, D., Fernández-González, F. T., and López-Morales, J. (2020). The use of a cooperative-learning activity with university students: a gender experience. Sustain. For. 12:9292. doi: 10.3390/su12219292
Bahadir, Z. (2011). The effect of different teaching methods (cooperative learning and drilling method) used in physical education and sports on students’ attitudes and their social skills. Ankara: University of Gazi.
Bailey, R. (2009). The educational benefits claimed for physical education and school sport. An Acad. Rev. Res Pap Educ. 24, 1–27. doi: 10.1080/02671520701809817
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Barlett, J. (2019). Social-emotional learning, health education best practices, and skills-based health. J. Phys. Educ. Recreat. Dance 90, 58–60. doi: 10.1080/07303084.2019.1548185
Barrett, T. (2005). Effects of cooperative learning on performance of sixth-grade physical education students. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 24, 88–102. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.24.1.88
Bayraktar, G. (2011). The effect of cooperative learning on students’ approach to general gymnastics course and academic achievements. Educ. Res. Rev. 6, 62–71.
Beck, U. (1998). “The politics of risk society” in The politics of risk society. ed. J. Franklin (Cambridge: Polity Press), 9–22.
Begg, C. B., and Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics 50, 1088–1101.
Benkhaled, H., Atallah, A., and Touati, H. (2015). Effects of cooperative and self-learning strategies on physical education and sport class. Int. J. Cross-Disciplinary Subj. Educ. 6, 2340–2347. doi: 10.20533/ijcdse.2042.6364.2015.0321
Bensikaddour, H., Mokrani, D., and Benzidane, H. (2015). The importance of the practice of competitive games kid’s athletics in physical education for college students (11-12 years) using the cooperative learning strategy. Eur. Sci. J. 11, 1857–7881.
Bermejo Díaz, J. M., Pulido Salas, D., Galmes-Panades, A. M., Serra Payeras, P., Vidal-Conti, J., and Ponseti Verdaguer, F. J. (2021). Physical education and university: evaluation of a teaching experience through cooperative learning. Retos 2041, 90–97. doi: 10.47197/retos.v0i39.77834
Bodsworth, H., and Goodyear, V. A. (2017). Barriers and facilitators to using digital technologies in the cooperative learning model in physical education. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 22, 563–579. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2017.1294672
Bofill-Herrero, A., Baena-Morales, S., García-Taibo, O., and Ferriz-Valero, A. (2022). Cooperative learning and SDG 5. An intervention for physical education in secondary schools. J. Phys. Educ. Sport 22, 1570–1580. doi: 10.7752/jpes.2022.07198
Bomhof-Roordink, H., Gärtner, F. R., Stiggelbout, A. M., and Pieterse, A. H. (2019). Key components of shared decision making models: a systematic review. BMJ Open 9:e031763. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-031763
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., and Rothstein, H. R. (2021). Introduction to Meta-analysis. US: John Wiley & Sons.
Bores-García, D., Hortigüela-Alcalá, D., Fernandez-Rio, F. J., González-Calvo, G., and Barba-Martín, R. (2021). Research on cooperative learning in physical education: systematic review of the last five years. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 92, 146–155. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2020.1719276
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Bunker, D., and Thorpe, R. (1982). A model for the teaching of games in secondary schools. Bull. Phys. Educ. 18, 5–8.
Callado, C. V., Aranda, A. F., Pastor, V. M. L., Learning, C., Learning, C., Learning, C., et al. (2014). Cooperative learning in physical education. Porto Alegre, 20, 239–257.
Cañabate, D., Bubnys, R., Nogué, L., Martínez-Mínguez, L., Nieva, C., and Colomer, J. (2021). Cooperative learning to reduce inequalities: instructional approaches and dimensions. Sustain. For. 13:10234. doi: 10.3390/su131810234
Casey, A. (2013). ‘Seeing the trees not just the wood’: steps and not just journeys in teacher action research. Educ. Action Res. 21, 147–163. doi: 10.1080/09650792.2013.789704
Casey, A. (2016). “Models-based practice” in Routledge handbook of physical education pedagogies. ed. C. D. Ennis (London: Routledge), 54–67.
Casey, A., and Fernandez-Rio, J. (2019). Cooperative learning and the affective domain. J. Phys. Educ. Recreat. Dance 90, 12–17. doi: 10.1080/07303084.2019.1559671
Casey, A., and Goodyear, V. A. (2015). Can cooperative learning achieve the four learning outcomes of physical education?: a review of literature. Quest 67, 56–72. doi: 10.1080/00336297.2014.984733
Casey, A., and Kirk, D. (2024). Applying models-based practice in physical education. In: C. D. Blustein. Taylor and Francis.
Cecchini Estrada, J. A., González González-Mesa, C. L. R., Sánchez Martínez, B., and Rodríguez Pérez, C. (2019). The impact of cooperative learning on peer relationships, intrinsic motivation and future intentions to do sport. Psicothema 2, 163–169. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2018.305
Chinn, C. A., Rinehart, R. W., and Buckland, L. A. (2014). “Epistemic cognition and evaluating information: applying the AIR model of epistemic cognition” in Processing inaccurate information: Theoretical and applied perspectives from cognitive science and the educational sciences. eds. D. N. Rapp and J. Braasch (Cambridge: MIT Press), 425–453.
Chiva-Bartoll, Ó., Salvador-García, C., and Ruiz-Montero, P. J. (2018). Teaching games for understanding and cooperative learning: can their hybridization increase motivational climate among physical education students? Croat. J. Educ. 20, 561–584. doi: 10.15516/cje.v20i2.2827
Cochon Drouet, O., Margas, N., Cece, V., and Lentillon-Kaestner, V. (2024). The effects of the jigsaw method on students’ physical activity in physical education: the role of student sex and habituation. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 30, 85–104. doi: 10.1177/1356336X231184347
Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 20, 37–46. doi: 10.1177/001316446002000104
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd Edn. Hillsdale: NJ Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
Cohen, R., and Zach, S. (2013). Building pre-service teaching efficacy: a comparison of instructional models. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 18, 376–388. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2012.690374
Darnis, F., and Lafont, L. (2015). Cooperative learning and dyadic interactions: two modes of knowledge construction in socio-constructivist settings for team-sport teaching. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 20, 459–473. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2013.803528
De Costa, P. I. (2007). J. P. Lantolf and S. L. Thorne: sociocultural theory and the genesis of second language development. Oxford university press, 2006. Appl. Linguis. 28, 477–480. doi: 10.1093/applin/amm027
Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inq. 11, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01
Deeks, J. J., Higgins, J. P. T., and Altman, D. G. (2019). “Analysing data and undertaking meta-analyses” in Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. eds. J. P. Higgins, J. Thomas, J. Chandler, and E. Al (Hoboken, NJ: Cochrane Collaboration).
Dyson, B. (2001). Cooperative learning in an elementary physical education program. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 20, 264–281. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.20.3.264
Dyson, B., and Casey, A. (2016). Cooperative Learning in physical education and physical activity: A practical introduction. London: Routledge.
Dyson, B. P., Colby, R., and Barratt, M. (2016). The co-construction of cooperative learning in physical education with elementary classroom teachers. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 35, 370–380. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.2016-0119
Dyson, B., Griffin, L. L., and Hastie, P. (2004). Sport education, tactical games, and cooperative learning: theoretical and pedagogical considerations. Quest 56, 226–240. doi: 10.1080/00336297.2004.10491823
Dyson, B., Howley, D., and Shen, Y. (2020). ‘Being a team, working together, and being kind’: primary students’ perspectives of cooperative learning’s contribution to their social and emotional learning. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 26, 137–154. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2020.1779683
Dyson, B., Shen, Y., Xiong, W., and Dang, L. (2022). How cooperative learning is conceptualized and implemented in Chinese physical education: a systematic review of literature. ECNU Rev. Educ. 5, 185–206. doi: 10.1177/20965311211006721
Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., and Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 315, 629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629
Engels, E. S., and Freund, P. A. (2020). Effects of cooperative games on enjoyment in physical education-how to increase positive experiences in students? PLoS One 15, 1–14. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243608
Fadhul, A. L. M. S. (2022). Effect of the cooperative learning strategy to reducing psychological stress and learning dribbling and passing skills of football for students. Soc. Sci. Humanit. J.
Faozi, F., Dlis, F., Samsudin, S., Hambali, S., and Riyadi, D. N. (2024). Cooperative learning vs direct teaching in basketball: effects on junior high school students basic techniques. Int. J. Disabil. Sport. Heal. Sci. 7, 132–140. doi: 10.33438/ijdshs.1371249
Fernández, A. D., and González-Mesa, C. (2018). Physical education and cooperative learning: a practical experience. J. Sport Heal. Res. 10, 43–64.
Fernández-Espínola, C., Abad Robles, M. T., Collado-Mateo, D., Almagro, B. J., Castillo Viera, E., and Gimenez Fuentes-Guerra, F. J. (2020). Effects of cooperative-learning interventions on physical education students’ intrinsic motivation: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 44–51. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17124451
Fernandez-Rio, J., and Casey, A. (2021). Sport education as a cooperative learning Endeavour. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 26, 375–387. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2020.1810220
Fernández-Río, J., Cecchini, J. A., and Méndez-Giménez, A. (2014). Effects of cooperative learning on perceived competence, motivation, social goals, effort and boredom in prospective primary education teachers. Infanc. y Aprendiz. 37, 57–89. doi: 10.1080/02103702.2014.881650
Fernandez-Rio, J., and Iglesias, D. (2024). What do we know about pedagogical models in physical education so far? An umbrella review. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 29, 190–205. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2022.2039615
Fernandez-Rio, J., Sanz, N., Fernandez-Cando, J., and Santos, L. (2017). Impact of a sustained cooperative learning intervention on student motivation. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 22, 89–105. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2015.1123238
Fernández-Vázquez, D., Navarro-López, V., Cano-de-la-Cuerda, R., Palacios-Ceña, D., Espada, M., Bores-García, D., et al. (2024). Influence of virtual reality and gamification combined with practice teaching style in physical education on motor skills and students’ perceived effort: a mixed-method intervention study. Sustain. For. 16:1584. doi: 10.3390/su16041584
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area of cognitive developmental inquiry. Am. Psychol. 34, 906–911. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906
Freire, P. (1970). Pedagogy of the oppressed, translated by Myra Bergman Ramos. New York, NY: Continuum.
García-González, L., Santed, M., Escolano-Pérez, E., and Fernández-Río, J. (2023). High- versus low-structured cooperative learning in secondary physical education: impact on prosocial behaviours at different ages. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 29, 199–214. doi: 10.1177/1356336X221132767
Gibson, J. J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Goldman, A. I. (2019). “The what, why, and how of social epistemology” in The Routledge handbook of social epistemology (London: Routledge), 10–20.
Goodyear, V. A., Casey, A., and Kirk, D. (2014). Hiding behind the camera: social learning within the cooperative learning model to engage girls in physical education. Sport Educ. Soc. 19, 712–734. doi: 10.1080/13573322.2012.707124
Gorucu, A. (2016). The investigation of the effects of physical education lessons planned in accordance with cooperative learning approach on secondary school students problem solving skills. Educ. Res. Rev. 11, 998–1007. doi: 10.5897/err2016.2756
Goudas, M., and Magotsiou, E. (2009). The effects of a cooperative physical education program on students’ social skills. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 21, 356–364. doi: 10.1080/10413200903026058
Gulay, O. (2008). The effects of cooperative games in 9th grade physical education lessons on the social skills levels of the students and on the attitudes towards pe lessons. Bolu: University of Abant İzzet Baysal.
Haerens, L., Kirk, D., Cardon, G., and De Bourdeaudhuij, I. (2011). Toward the development of a pedagogical model for health-based physical education. Quest 63, 321–338. doi: 10.1080/00336297.2011.10483684
Harvey, S., and Jarrett, K. (2014). A review of the game-Centred approaches to teaching and coaching literature since 2006. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 19, 278–300. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2012.754005
Harvey, S., Pill, S., and Almond, L. (2018). Old wine in new bottles: a response to claims that teaching games for understanding was not developed as a theoretically based pedagogical framework. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 23, 166–180. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2017.1359526
Hedges, L. V., and Olkin, I. (2014). Statistical methods for Meta-analysis. Cambridge, MA, USA: Academic Press.
Hellison, D. (2011). Teaching personal and social responsibility through physical activity. 3rd Edn. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Hellison, D., Cutforth, N., Kallusky, J., Martinek, T., Parker, M., and Stiehl, J. (2000). Youth development and physical activity: Linking universities and communities. Campaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Higgins, J. P., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M. J., et al. (2024). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 6.5 (updated august 2024). Cochrane. Available online at: www.training.cochrane.org/handbook (Accessed September 17, 2024).
Hofstede, G. (2011). Dimensionalizing cultures: the Hofstede model in context. Online readings Psychol. Cult. 2:8. doi: 10.9707/2307-0919.1014
Howley, D., Dyson, B., and Baek, S. (2024). All the better for it: exploring one teacher-researcher’s evolving efforts to promote meaningful physical education. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 31, 70–86. doi: 10.1177/1356336X241247
Hu, Z. (2023). Virtual reality-based cooperative learning in physical education: ways to improve physical performance and motivation of students. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact. 41, 242–253. doi: 10.1080/10447318.2023.2297331
Huang, C. Y. (2000). The effects of cooperative learning and model demonstration strategies on motor skill performance during video instruction. Proceeding Natl. Sci. Counc. 10, 255–268.
Huang, M., Tu, H., Wang, W. Y., Chen, J. F., Yu, Y. T., and Chou, C. C. (2017). Effects of cooperative learning and concept mapping intervention on critical thinking and basketball skills in elementary school. Think. Skills Creat. 23, 207–216. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2017.01.002
Huedo-Medina, T. B., Sánchez-Meca, J., Marín-Martínez, F., and Botella, J. (2006). Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index? Psychol. Methods 11, 193–206. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.2.193
Iglesias, D., Fernandez-Rio, J., and Rodríguez-González, P. (2023). Cooperative learning in physical education: a research overview. Apunt. Educ. Fis. y Deport. 151, 88–93. doi: 10.5672/apunts.2014-0983.es.(2023/1).151.09
Jacobs, J. M., Lawson, M., Ivy, V. N., and Richards, K. A. R. (2017). Enhancing the transfer of life skills from sport-based youth development programs to school, family, and community settings. J. Amat. Sport 3, 20–43. doi: 10.17161/jas.v3i3.6514
Jastrow, F., Greve, S., Thumel, M., Diekhoff, H., and Süßenbach, J. (2022). Digital technology in physical education: a systematic review of research from 2009 to 2020. Ger. J. Exerc. Sport Res. 52, 504–528. doi: 10.1007/s12662-022-00848-5
Jewett, A. E., and Bain, L. L. (1985). The curriculum process in physical education. Dubuque, IA: William. C. Brown.
Johnson, D. W., and Johnson, R. T. (1999). Making cooperative learning work. Theory Pract. 38, 67–73. doi: 10.1080/00405849909543834
Johnson, D. W., and Johnson, R. T. (2009). An educational psychology success story: social interdependence theory and cooperative learning. Educ. Res. 38, 365–379. doi: 10.3102/0013189X09339057
Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., and Holubec, E. J. (1994). The new circles of Learning: Cooperation in the classroom and school. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.
Karavelioglu, M. B. (2012). Effects of cooperative learning and instruction methods used on soccer-specific skill levels. Ankara: University of Gazi.
Khoury, B., Lecomte, T., Fortin, G., Masse, M., Therien, P., Bouchard, V., et al. (2013). Mindfulness-based therapy: a comprehensive meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33, 763–771. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.05.005
Kim, M., and Park, S. (2024). Better together: promoting cooperative learning in physical education. Strategies 37, 38–40. doi: 10.1080/08924562.2024.2304177
Kiremitci, O., and Dogan, B. (2010). The effect of dance education with the cooperative learning method on the development of students’ problem-solving skills. J. New World Sci. Acad. 5, 179–186.
Kirk, D. (2013). Educational value and models-based practice in physical education. Educ. Philos. Theory 45, 973–986. doi: 10.1080/00131857.2013.785352
Lafont, L., Proeres, M., and Vallet, C. (2007). Cooperative group learning in a team game: role of verbal exchanges among peers. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 10, 93–113. doi: 10.1007/s11218-006-9006-7
Landis, J. R., and Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33, 159–174. doi: 10.2307/2529310
Lave, J., and Wenger, E. (1991). Situated Learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. UK, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lee, T. E. (2014). Effects of a cooperative learning strategy on the effectiveness of physical fitness teaching and constraining factors. Math. Probl. Eng. 2014, 1–6. doi: 10.1155/2014/519291
León, B., Fernandez-Rio, J., Rivera-Pérez, S., and Iglesias, D. (2021). Cooperative classrooms and academic performance in physical education: a multilevel analysis. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 41, 660–668. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.2021-0008
Liu, T., and Lipowski, M. (2021). Influence of cooperative learning intervention on the intrinsic motivation of physical education students: a meta-analysis within a limited range. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 1–11. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18062989
Liu, Z., Saldanha, I. J., Margolis, D., Dumville, J. C., and Cullum, N. A. (2017). Outcomes in Cochrane systematic reviews related to wound care: an investigation into prespecification. Wound Repair Regen. 25, 292–308. doi: 10.1111/wrr.12519
Luo, Y., Sun, Y., and Strobel, J. (2013). The effects of collectivism-individualism on the cooperative learning of motor skill. J. Int. Stud. 3, 41–51. doi: 10.32674/jis.v3i1.517
MacPhail, A., and Lawson, H. (2020). School physical education and teacher education: Collaborative redesign for the 21st century. London: Routledge.
Manninen, M., and Campbell, S. (2022). The effect of the sport education model on basic needs, intrinsic motivation and prosocial attitudes: a systematic review and multilevel meta-analysis. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 28, 78–99. doi: 10.1177/1356336X211017938
McKenzie, J. E., and Brennan, S. E. (2019). “Synthesizing and presenting findings using other methods” in Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. eds. J. P. T. Higgins, J. Thomas, J. Chandler, and E. Al (London: Cochrane Collaboration).
Metzler, M. (2000). Instructional models for physical education. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Miller, A. (2015). Games centered approaches in teaching children & adolescents: systematic review of associated student outcomes. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 34, 36–58. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.2013-0155
Mitchell, S. A., Oslin, J. L., and Griffin, L. L. (2013). Teaching sports concepts and skills: A tactical games approach for ages 7 to 18. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Mohseen, H. S., Omar Fauzee, M. S., Kim, S., and Geok, R. B. B. (2011). Issue of the social dilemmas after wars: a cooperative learning intervention through physical education and it’s effect on social skills development among middle school students’ in Baghdad. Iraq. Aust. J. Basic Appl. Sci. 5, 980–989.
Montoya, A., Simonton, K., and Gaudreault, K. L. (2020). Enhance student motivation and social skills: adopting the sport education and cooperative learning models. J. Phys. Educ. Recreat. Dance 91, 15–20. doi: 10.1080/07303084.2020.1798307
Mosston, M. (1966). Teaching physical education: From command to discovery. Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill.
Mostafa, A. (2015). Effectiveness of cooperative-mastery learning method on basketball skills and moral reasoning at physical education lessons. Int. Sci. J. Phys. Educ. Sport Sci. 1, 9–21. doi: 10.21608/isjpes.2015.233213
Murtagh, E. M., Calderón, A., Scanlon, D., and MacPhail, A. (2023). Online teaching and learning in physical education teacher education: a mixed studies review of literature. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 29, 369–388. doi: 10.1177/1356336X231155793
Newell, K. M. (1985). Coordination, control and skill. Adv. Psychol. 27, 295–317. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62541-8
Nopembri, S., Sugiyama, Y., and Rithaudin, A. (2019). Improving stress coping and problem-solving skills of children in disaster-prone area through cooperative physical education and sports lesson. J. Hum. Sport Exerc. 14, 185–194. doi: 10.14198/jhse.2019.141.15
Norito, T. B., Dlis, F., Hanif, A. S., and Iqbal, M. (2019). Implementing cooperative learning in physical education and sport to improve children fundamental skill. J. Educ. 9, 2391–8306. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.3346970
O’Leary, N., and Griggs, G. (2010). Researching the pieces of a puzzle: the use of a jigsaw learning approach in the delivery of undergraduate gymnastics. J. Furth. High. Educ. 34, 73–81. doi: 10.1080/03098770903477110
O’Neil, K., and Boyce, B. A. (2018). Improving teacher effectiveness in physical education teacher education through field-based supervision. Phys. Educ. 75, 835–849. doi: 10.18666/TPE-2018-V75-I5-7739
Ortiz, M., Meroño, L., Morales-Belando, M. T., Vaquero-Cristóbal, R., and González-Gálvez, N. (2023). Teaching games for understanding in game performance and psychosocial variables: systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized control trial. Child. Aust. 10:573. doi: 10.3390/children10030573
Ortuondo, J., Hortigüela, D., Meroño, L., and Zulaika, L. (2023). Influence of the types of grouping on the social compentence of future physical education teachers when implementing cooperative learning. Movimento 29, 1–20. doi: 10.22456/1982-8918.123333
Ortuondo, J., Hortigüela-Alcalá, D., Bidaurrazaga-letona, I., Zulaika, L. M., Bárcena, J. O., Hortigüela-Alcalá, D., et al. (2022). Effects of a cooperative learning based intervention on the physical self- concept of pre-service physical education tea. Retos Nuevas Perspect. Educ. Física, Deport. y Recreación 44, 827–836.
Page, M. J., Altman, D. G., McKenzie, J. E., Shamseer, L., Ahmadzai, N., Wolfe, D., et al. (2018). Flaws in the application and interpretation of statistical analyses in systematic reviews of therapeutic interventions were common: a cross-sectional analysis. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 95, 7–18. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2017.11.022
Page, M. J., Higgins, J. P. T., and Sterne, J. A. C. (2019). “Assessing risk of Bias due to missing results in a synthesis” in Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. eds. H. JPT, J. Thomas, and J. Chandler (Cochrane: Cochrane Collaboration).
Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., and Mulrow, C. D. (2021). The prisma 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71
Palau-Pamies, M., García-Martínez, S., Ferriz-Valero, A., and Tortosa-Martínez, J. (2022). Impact of cooperative learning on physical education in basic psychological needs. Rev. Int. Med. y Ciencias la Act. Fis. y del Deport. 22, 787–806. doi: 10.15366/rimcafd2022.88.005
Pehlivan, Z., and Alkan, G. (2010). Effect of cooperative teaching method on affective characteristics and motor skill levels in physical education courses. J. New World Sci. Acad. 5, 158–166.
Pope, C. C. (2005). Once more with feeling: affect and playing with the TGfU model. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 10, 271–286. doi: 10.1080/17408980500340885
Pozo, P., Grao-cruces, A., and Pe, R. (2018). Teaching personal and social responsibility model-based programmes in physical education: a systematic review. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 24, 56–75. doi: 10.1177/1356336X16664749
Reed, D. A., Cook, D. A., Beckman, T. J., Levine, R. B., Kern, D. E., and Wright, S. M. (2007). Association between funding and quality of published medical education research. JAMA 298, 1002–1009. doi: 10.1001/jama.298.9.1002
Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230
Robson, R. C., Pham, B., Hwee, J., Thomas, S. M., Rios, P., Page, M. J., et al. (2019). Few studies exist examining methods for selecting studies, abstracting data, and appraising quality in a systematic review. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 106, 121–135. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2018.10.003
Salih, M. M. M., Hashim, R. S., and Kasim, M. A. (2021). Forecasting achievement sports through cooperative learning in handball training in physical education. Ann. Appl. Sport Sci. 9, 1–8. doi: 10.29252/aassjournal.953
Salomon, G., and Perkins, D. N. (1998). Chapter 1: individual and social aspects of learning. Rev. Res. Educ. 23, 1–24. doi: 10.3102/0091732X023001001
Sánchez-Hernández, N., Martos-García, D., and Flintoff, A. (2018). Challenging gender relations in PE through cooperative learning and critical reflection. Sport Educ. Soc. 23, 812–823. doi: 10.1080/13573322.2018.1487836
Sánchez-Miguel, P. A., Álvarez-Sánchez, J. L., Hortigüela-Alcalá, D., Pérez-Pueyo, A., and Tapia-Serrano, M. A. (2025). The effect of the teaching personal and social responsibility model on personal and social responsibility in physical rducation: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Quest 77, 237–266. doi: 10.1080/00336297.2024.2443937
Schiavo, J. H. (2019). PROSPERO: an international register of systematic review protocols. Med. Ref. Serv. Q. 38, 171–180. doi: 10.1080/02763869.2019.1588072
Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychol. Rev. 82, 225–260. doi: 10.1037/h0076770
Schulze, C., and Von Huth, M. (2022). Effects of cooperative learning structures in physical education: a systematic review. Int. J. Phys. Educ. Fit. Sport. 11, 1–11. doi: 10.34256/ijpefs2241
Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and well-being. New York: Simon and Schuster.
Senge, P. M. (2006). The fifth discipline: The art and practice of the Learning organization : Broadway Business. 6, 2635–2645.
Shalal, B. R., Hassan, H. S., and Waheed, M. H. (2021). The effect of using kinetic games exercises in a cooperative learning method in learning the skill of shooting basketball. PalArch’s J. Archaeol. Egypt/Egyptol. 18, 1249–1256.
Shen, Y., Martinek, T., and Dyson, B. P. (2022). Navigating the processes and products of the teaching personal and social responsibility model: a systematic literature review. Quest 74, 91–107. doi: 10.1080/00336297.2021.2017988
Siedentop, D. (2001). Sport education: a retrospective. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 21, 409–418. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.21.4.409
Siedentop, D., Hastie, P., and Van Der Mars, H. (2019). Complete guide to sport education. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Sierra-Díaz, M. J., Gonzalez-Villora, S., Pastor-Vicedo, J. C., and López-Sánchez, G. F. (2019). Can we motivate students to practice physical activities and sports through models-based practice? A systematic review and meta-analysis of psychosocial factors related to physical education. Front. Psychol. 10:2115. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02115
Siong, N. U., and Ali, S. K. B. S. (2020). Effects of STAD and jigsaw cooperative learning methods on badminton backhand low service skill. Int. J. Innov. Creat. Chang. 10, 13–30.
Slavin, R. E. (1996). Research on cooperative learning and achievement: what we know, what we need to know. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 21, 43–69. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1996.0004
Stanne, M. B., Johnson, D. W., and Johnson, R. T. (1999). Does competition enhance or inhibit motor performance: a meta-analysis. Sychological Bull. 125, 133–154.
Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: effects on learning. Cogn. Sci. 12, 257–285.
Tuncel, Z. (2006). The effects of cooperative learning on physical education achievement, cognitive processes, and social behaviors. İzmir: University of Dokuz Eylül.
Turan, S., Zehir, T., Ilgin, O., and Soyer, F. (2023). The effects of cooperative learning model on attitudes in the direction of physical education and self-efficacy. J. ROL Sport. Sci. Cilt 4, 177–194. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.7740964
Uresin, B. (2012). The effect of individual competitive and cooperative goal orientations applied in physical education classes on the attitudes of secondary school students towards physical education classes : University of Ege.
Viera, A. J., and Garrett, J. M. (2005). Understanding interobserver agreement: the kappa statistic. Fam. Med. 37, 360–363
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Waffenschmidt, S., Knelangen, M., Sieben, W., Bühn, S., and Pieper, D. (2019). Single screening versus conventional double screening for study selection in systematic reviews: a methodological systematic review. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 19:132. doi: 10.1186/s12874-019-0782-0
Walad, A. M., Razak, A., Putri, D. H., Walad, A. M., Razak, A., and Putri, D. H. (2019). Implementing jigsaw type of cooperative learning model to improve students’ cognitive, affective and psychomotor domains in learning natural science at grade IX. Int. J. Progress. Sci. Technol. 14, 329–337.
Wallhead, T., and Dyson, B. (2017). A didactic analysis of content development during cooperative learning in primary physical education. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 23, 311–326. doi: 10.1177/1356336X16630221
Wang, M. (2012). Effects of cooperative learning on achievement motivation of female university students. Asian Soc. Sci. 8, 108–114. doi: 10.5539/ass.v8n15p108
Wang, Z., Nayfeh, T., Tetzlaff, J., O’Blenis, P., and Murad, M. (2020). Error rates of human reviewers during abstract screening in systematic reviews. PLoS One 15:e0227742. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0227742
Wang, X., Zhao, C., Huang, T., Chakrabarti, P., and Kurths, J. (2023). Cooperative learning of multi-agent systems via reinforcement learning. IEEE Trans. Signal Inf. Process. over Networks 9, 13–23. doi: 10.1109/TSIPN.2023.3239654
Wasserman, S., and Faust, K. (1994). Social network analysis: Methods and applications : Cambridge University Press.
Williams, R. (2012). Moderator analyses: Categorical models and meta-regression., in annual Campbell collaboration colloquium. Copenhagen: Campbell Collaboration.
Winarni, S., and Lutan, R. (2020). Emphaty and tolerance in physical education: cooperative vs. classical learning. Cakrawala Pendidik. 39, 332–345. doi: 10.21831/cp.v39i2.31851
Yang, C., Chen, R., Chen, X., and Lu, K. H. (2021). The efficiency of cooperative learning in physical education on the learning of action skills and learning motivation. Front. Psychol. 12, 1–17. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.717528
Zach, S., Shoval, E., and Shulruf, B. (2023). Cooperative learning in physical education lessons - literature review. Front. Educ. 8, 1–12. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2023.1273423
Zhou, T., Wang, H., and Li, D. (2023). Focusing on the value of cooperative learning in physical education: a bibliometric analysis. Front. Psychol. 14, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1300986
Keywords: cooperative learning, classroom physical activities, learning outcomes, meta-analysis, physical education
Citation: Boke H, Aygun Y, Tufekci S, Yagin FH, Canpolat B, Norman G, Prieto-González P and Ardigò LP (2025) Effects of cooperative learning on students’ learning outcomes in physical education: a meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 16:1508808. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1508808
Edited by:
Pedro Morouço, Polytechnic Institute of Leiria, PortugalReviewed by:
Jeff Bolles, Francis Marion University, United StatesBaskoro Nugroho Putro, Sebelas Maret University, Indonesia
Norlaila Azura Kosni, Univerisiti Teknologi MARA, Malaysia
Copyright © 2025 Boke, Aygun, Tufekci, Canpolat, Yagin, Norman, Prieto-González and Ardigò. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Yalin Aygun, eWFsaW4uYXlndW5AaW5vbnUuZWR1LnRy; Luca Paolo Ardigò, bHVjYS5hcmRpZ29AbmxhLm5v