BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 06 June 2025

Sec. Sport Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1518495

Sport climbing competence is influenced by training frequency, experience, self-efficacy, flow, and emotional intelligence


Marina TurchettoMarina TurchettoValentina TomaselliValentina TomaselliFrancesca GiorgiFrancesca GiorgiSilvia LeoneSilvia LeoneIrene Leo
Irene Leo*
  • Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy

Sport climbing requires the integration of physical and cognitive abilities. The Embodied Cognition (EC) theoretical framework is increasingly applied in recreational sport research, as it emphasizes the influence of bodily experience on cognition functioning. This exploratory study investigated the relationship between competence in sport climbing and several psychological factors, including both stable traits (e.g., self-esteem, trait emotional intelligence) and trainable capacities (e.g., mindfulness, self-efficacy, flow), as well as climbing experience and training frequency. A sample of 365 climbers (40% female, M = 32.11 years) completed questionnaires. Correlation analysis showed positive associations between climbing competence and training frequency, years of practice, mindfulness, and climbing self-efficacy. A structural equation model (SEM) revealed that climbing confidence was the strongest psychological predictor of competence, while mindfulness unexpectedly emerged as a negative predictor. Trait emotional intelligence was positively associated with mindfulness but not directly with competence. A bidirectional relationship between mindfulness and competence was observed, suggesting a complex interaction. These findings highlight the interplay between psychological dispositions and sport-specific skills in climbing performance.

Introduction

The Embodied Cognition (EC) approach has significantly changed the way researchers conceptualize sensorimotor experience. From this perspective, the body and its sensorimotor patterns play a crucial role in all cognitive processes (Wellsby and Pexman, 2014; Vallet et al., 2016). Mental activity is shaped by the ongoing interaction between the body and its environment (Leo et al., 2022). In this light, EC offers a valuable lens for understanding athletic performance as a dynamic interaction between mind and body (Raab and Araújo, 2019), particularly in activities such as climbing, which require an intricate balance of physical and cognitive skills (Stephan et al., 2011; Wickens et al., 2018).

Sport climbing is a distinct discipline within the broader domain of climbing sports, which also includes traditional climbing and ice climbing. According to the Italian Federation of Sport Climbing (Federazione Arrampicata Sportiva Italiana [FASI], 2025), sport climbing involves climbing for competitive, recreational, or educational purposes on either artificial or natural walls along predefined, equipped routes. Artificial aids for progression are not permitted. There are three types of sport climbing: lead climbing, speed climbing, and bouldering, which are now formally recognized at the international level and were included in the Olympic Games beginning with Tokyo 2021. Lead climbing involves ascending a route using a belayed dynamic rope, either indoors on artificial walls or outdoors on natural rock. Speed climbing uses a standardized route equipped with a timing system and typically employs top-rope belaying, often with automatic belayers. Bouldering consists of short, high-intensity climbs performed without ropes but with crash mats for protection.

Despite the formalization and growing popularity of sport climbing, much of the psychological literature has addressed “rock climbing” in general, often without clearly distinguishing between its subdisciplines. This terminological ambiguity can obscure the specificity of findings and limit their theoretical and pratical utility. Moreover, climbing has frequently been studied through a therapeutic lens, with an emphasis on its benefits for mental health rather than as a performance based acitivity. Research has shown that climbing can reduce anxiety (Aras and Ewert, 2016), alleviate depression (Zieliński et al., 2018), and improve self-esteem and self-confidence (Bahaeloo-Horeh and Assari, 2008; Steimer and Weissert, 2017). While these findings remain important, a growing body of research highlights the role of psychological variables in predicting climbing performance (Mangan et al., 2024).

Classical psychological factors such as personality (Ionel et al., 2023; Steinmetz et al., 2022) and self-efficacy (Llewellyn and Sanchez, 2008; Saul et al., 2019) have been widely examined. Self-efficacy refers to an individual’s belief in their ability to organize and execute the actions required to achieve specific outcomes (Bandura, 1997). It is a strong predictor of the difficulty, frequency, and risk level of climbing activities, with more self-efficacious individuals tending to pursue more challenging goals (Bandura, 1997). Self-efficacy should not be confused with self-esteem, which pertains to an individual’s general evaluation of their own self-worth (Rosenberg, 1965).

Additionally, flow experience appears to be a relevant factor in rock climbing performance (Mangan et al., 2024), especially in outdoor settings (Boudreau et al., 2022). Flow is defined as a state of deep concentration and immersion, characterized by a balance between skill and challenge (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; 2008; Csikszentmihalyi and LeFevre, 1989). More recent studies view flow as a dynamic psychological process rather than a fixed state (Šimleša et al., 2018). Boudreau et al. (2022) found that rock climbers reported experiencing flow in at least one of their climbs in 72% of outdoor sessions. Interestingly, flow states often preceded the phase of route exploration—a critical component of successful climbing (Rucińska, 2021). Nonetheless, the link between flow and sport climbing performance warrants further investigation.

Other psychological variables have received comparatively less attention. For example, Garrido-Palomino and España-Romero (2019) reported a relationship between Emotional Intelligence (EI) and sport climbing performance. EI involves the capacity to pursue long-term goals while employing adaptive emotional regulation strategies (Laborde et al., 2012; Laborde et al., 2014a,b). Variations in EI may differentiate sport climbers across skill levels, suggesting its potential role in performance (Garrido-Palomino and España-Romero, 2019). Although EI’s importance in sports performance is well documented (Laborde et al., 2011; Laborde et al., 2015), further research is needed to clarify its specific role in sport climbing.

Given the stressful nature of sport rock climbing, athletes must maintain focus on the present moment and manage anxiety and fear of falling (Aras and Akalan, 2014). Fear of falling in sport climbing is considered as an element of psychological stress (Garrido-Palomino and España-Romero, 2023) that may impair the performance of lower grade and intermediate climbers, but not in advanced and élite ones (Gajdošík et al., 2020). Mindfulness may enhance sport climbing ability by reducing fear (Ilgner, 2003), improving breathing control, and supporting performance visualization. Mindfulness is defined as “paying attention in a particular way: on purpose, in the present moment, and non-judgmentally” (Kabat-Zinn, 1994). It is inherently embedded in climbing activities (Wheatley, 2023), promoting mental skills necessary to stay focused under pressure (Young and Knight, 2014), manage emotional reactions, and confront fear (Steca et al., 2018). Notably, Wheatley (2023) found that young adults engaged in rock climbing exhibited higher mindfulness levels compared to peers involved in other physical activities. These findings suggest a bidirectional relationship: rock climbing may improve mindfulness, and mindfulness may, in turn, enhance rock climbing performance. However, no studies to date have directly explored this two-way interaction.

Guided by the EC framework, this study focuses on the psychological underpinnings of competence in sport climbing. In contrast to prior research that often conflated distinct climbing disciplines, we specifically examined sport climbers. The psychological variables selected include a mix of enduring traits (e.g., self-esteem, trait emotional intelligence), acquires skills (e.g., self-efficacy), and trainable mental states or capacities (e.g., mindfulness, flow). This distinction reflects the complex nature of psychological functioning in sport contexts.

Based on previous findings, we hypothesized that competence in sport climbing would be positively associated with self-efficacy, flow experience, and self-esteem. Given the limited and mixed findings in the literature, we also explored the roles of trait emotional intelligence and mindfulness, without making strong directional predictions. We further expected training frequency and years of practice to contribute to higher competence, both directly and indirectly, through their influence on psychological functioning.

Materials and methods

A total of 365 Caucasian individuals from Northern Italy participated in this exploratory study, including 135 females (40%) with an age range of 18–72 years (M = 32.11, SD ± 9.92). Over 60% were aged between 18 and 29. Approximately 33% reported 4–9 years of experience in sport climbing (M = 2.99, SD = 1.74), and trained around two times a week (M = 3.76, SD = 1.17). Based on Draper et al. (2011) classification, 46% self-identified at an intermediate level (grades 5b–6b), and 13% reported beginners-level competence (grades 4a–5a) (M = 2.43, SD = 0.94).

Almost all participants reported practicing both lead climbing and bouldering, the two most common sub-disciplines of sport climbing. They typically train in climbing gyms equipped with both indoor bouldering areas and lead walls, the latter often located just outside the gym facility. Recruitment was conducted through word of mouth and social media platforms. Individuals under the age of 18, those who had never practiced climbing, or those who had ceased practicing were excluded from participation. Data were collected between January and April 2021. All participants gave informed consent in accordance with the ethical standards of the Department of Psychology at the University of Padua, Italy.

Measures

The questionnaire was administered online and consisted of six scales.

The Italian translation of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) (Brown and Ryan, 2003) was used to assess mindfulness. The items are presented in reverse form, exploring how often individuals act automatically, without paying attention, or in a worried state. The scale includes 15 items rated on a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (“almost always”) to 6 (“almost never”); higher scores indicate a greater tendency toward mindfulness.

The Italian version of the Dispositional Flow Scale-2 (DFS-2) (Boffi et al., 2012; Jackson and Eklund, 2002) was used to measure individuals’ predisposition to experience flow. The scale comprises 36 items covering the nine dimensions of flow as proposed by Csikszentmihalyi (1990). Each subscale consists of four items rated on a 5-point Likert scale, where 1 corresponds to “never” and 5 to “always.” The flow dimensions include: challenge-skill balance (D1), action-awareness merging (D2), clear goals (D3), unambiguous feedback (D4), concentration on the task at hand (D5), sense of control (D6), loss of self-consciousness (D7), transformation of time (D8), and autotelic experience (D9).

Self-esteem was assessed using the Italian version of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Questionnaire (RSEQ) (Prezza et al., 1997; Rosenberg, 1965), which consists of 10 items—five reflecting positive attributes and five capturing negative self-evaluations. Responses are given on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 3 (“strongly agree”) to 0 (“strongly disagree”), resulting in a total score for self-esteem.

Emotional intelligence was measured using the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire—Short Form (TEIQue-SF) (Petrides, 2009), validated in Italian by Di Fabio and Palazzeschi (2011). The TEIQue-SF consists of 30 items rated on a 7-point Likert scale, where 1 indicates “strongly disagree” and 7 indicates “strongly agree.” It is a condensed version of the original TEIQue (Petrides and Furnham, 2001) and assesses global trait emotional intelligence (TEIQue Tot), along with four subscales: emotionality (TEIQue_EM), sociability (TEIQue_S), self-control (TEIQue_Sc), and well-being (TEIQue_W).

Self-efficacy was measured using the Climbing Self-Efficacy Scale (CSES) (Llewellyn and Sanchez, 2008), which was translated into Italian for this study. The scale includes 10 items aligned with Bandura (1997) theory of self-efficacy. Respondents rate their confidence in climbing-specific skills on a scale from 0% (“not at all confident”) to 100% (“extremely confident”). The total score ranges from 0 to 1000 and provides an indication of the individual’s belief in their ability to perform in the sport.

Participants were also asked to report their climbing competence level and training frequency for the year in which the questionnaire was completed. Climbing competence was defined by the highest climbing grade achieved that year, using the French grading system (from 3 to 9, with subgrades a, b, and c). Draper et al. (2011) scale was used to convert these grades into standardized ability levels.

Data analysis

A descriptive analysis of the raw data, including means and standard deviations, was conducted using a Python program. The reliability of the measures was assessed through correlation analysis using the Spearman-Brown coefficient.

Subsequently, a Structural Equation Model (SEM) was constructed using the statistical software R. The model’s goodness of fit was evaluated using the Chi-Square test, where non-significant values indicate a good fit between the model and the data (Iacobucci, 2010).

In addition, several incremental goodness-of-fit indices were considered. The Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) was evaluated, with values below 0.08 considered acceptable (Hu and Bentler, 1999). The Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) was also used, where values of 0.05 or lower indicate a good model fit (Browne and Cudeck, 1992). The Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) were considered acceptable when values met or exceeded the threshold of 0.90 (Bentler, 1990; Tucker and Lewis, 1973).

Mindfulness, self-efficacy, self-esteem, flow, and emotional intelligence were included as psychological variables hypothesized to influence climbing competence both directly and indirectly. Years of practice and training frequency were also included in the model as additional variables, to more comprehensively capture the factors influencing performance.

Preliminary analyses

Descriptive statistics and internal reliability coefficients for all the factors are provided in Table 1. The Cronbach’s alpha levels for each of the measures exceeded acceptable thresholds, indicating sufficient internal reliability for their inclusion in the analysis. Some subscales of the Teique scale exhibited slightly lower, yet still acceptable, internal consistency, specifically Teique EM (α = 0.68), Teique S (α = 0.66), and similarly, D2 of the flow scale also displayed low internal consistency (α = 0.68). Data related to participant groups categorized based on years of practice (YC), training frequency (TF), and level of expertise (LC) are represented in Table 2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) for study variables.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Participant characteristics by climbing experience, training, and competence.

Table 3 reports all the Spearman-Brown correlations between competence and years of practice, training frequency, and psychological factors. The data reveal positive and statistically significant correlations between competence and training frequency (r = 0.502, p < 0.001), years of practice (r = 0.437, p < 0.001), the CC scale (r = 0.237, p < 0.001), and MAAS (r = 0.161, p < 0.01)

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlation coefficients.

Structural equation model

A structural equation model (SEM) was conducted to test the hypotheses and observe the interaction of psychological factors, directly and indirectly influencing the competence of the climbers’ group. The model is represented in Figure 1. The goodness-of-fit statistics for the SEM model were all within acceptable ranges, with all indices indicating a good fit. Specifically, we evaluated several indices following established guidelines (Kline, 2015), with the following results: RMSEA = 0.08, CFI = 0.97, TLI = 0.90, and SRMR = 0.03. The Chi-square test was significant (χ2 = 19.74, df = 6; p < 0.003). According to widely accepted criteria for SEM model fit, RMSEA and TLI both reached the minimum threshold for an acceptable fit, although values closer to 0.95 are preferred for TLI (Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003) and values below 0.08 are generally considered a better fit for RMSE (MacCallum et al., 1996). Moreover, both CFI and SRMR values (respectively, 0.97 and 0.03) suggested a strong model fit, since the commonly recommended threshold for CFI is 0.95 (or above) and for SRMR is equal or below 0.08 (Hu and Bentler, 1999). On the other hand, the Chi-square statistics resulted significant. However, this result is often found in larger samples and more complex models and given that all other indices suggest a good fit, the significant Chi-square does not necessarily indicate a misfit (Kline, 2015). Climbing confidence (CC), mindfulness (MAAS), years of practice, and training frequency were all significant predictors of competence (CC β = 0.07, MAAS β = −0.15, years of practice β = 0.23, training frequency β = 0.36). Unexpectedly, MAAS was found to be a negative predictor of competence. The CC scale is significantly and positively predicted by self-esteem (β = 0.74), flow (β = 1.20), years of practice (β = 0.11), and training frequency (β = 0.21). Emotional Intelligence (Teique) was also a significant predictor of mindfulness (β = 0.34). The influence of competence on mindfulness was also analyzed and, in this case, it was observed that competence is a significant positive predictor of mindfulness (β = 0.23). The levels of explained variance were significant for mindfulness, competence, and climbing confidence, respectively (R2 = 0.13, 0.39, 0.31).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Structural Equation Model (SEM) of pychological influencing climbing competence. ***p ≤ 0.001, **p ≤ 0.01, *p ≤ 0.05.

Power considerations: Retrospective design analysis

Although we did not conduct an a priori power analysis before data collection, we used a Retrospective Design Analysis (RDA) to evaluate the robustness of our findings post hoc, following the recommendations of Gelman and Carlin (2014). RDA is not a replacement for prospective power analysis, but it can offer insight into which observed effects are likely to be reliably detected and which may be prone to inferential risks. Based on our sample size of 365 participants and hypothesized small-to-medium effect sizes, we examined potential risk associated with low statistical power. The results showed that effects such as Climbing Confidence → Competence (β = 0.07) and Climbing Confidence → Mindfulness (β = 0.06) were below conventional thresholds for robust detection (power < 0.80). Therefore, these small effects may be susceptible to overestimation (Type M error) or misdirection (Type S error), and should be interpreted with caution. In contrast, larger effects such as Flow → Climbing Confidence (β = 1.20, p < 0.001) were associated with high statistical power, suggesting they are more robust and less likely to reflect Type M or S errors.

Overall, while our model appears adequately powered to detect medium-to-large effects (β≈ 0.3–1.20), but the smaller observed effects (β = 0.06–0.07) may be underpowered and require cautious interpretation. Future replication with larger samples may help clarify the stability of these associations and may help to validate the weaker effects.

This section is intended to enhance transparency and should not be considered a substitute for prospective planning in future research.

Discussion

The aim of this exploratory study was to investigate the existing relationship between competence in sport climbing and psychological variables, considering training frequency and years of practice as independent variables. Our hypothesis that self-efficacy, self-esteem, mindfulness, emotional intelligence, and flow would influence competence was partially supported by the findings.

Correlation analysis revealed that competence levels were positively associated with training frequency, years of climbing experience, mindfulness, and domain-specific climbing confidence. To further examine the direction and natura of these relationships, we employed a structural equation model (SEM). Among the psychological dispositions, climbing confidence emerged as the strongest predictor of competence. This aligns with prior research emphasizing the importance of domain-specific self-efficacy in sport climbing performance (Mangan et al., 2024; Saul et al., 2019). Individuals with higher climbing confidence may be more likely to challenge themselves, attempt difficult routes, and build skill through repeated exposure.

Our findings further show that climbing confidence was positively predicted by years of practice, training frequency, general self-esteem, and the disposition to experience flow. Flow, in this context, refers to the optimal psychological state during which a climber becomes fully immersed in the climbing activity. Prior studies have shown that flow is often experienced during phases of route planning and execution (Boudreau et al., 2022), and may facilitate performance by enhancing focus, coordination, and the integration of motor schemas (Saul et al., 2019; Rucińska, 2021).

Interestingly, trait emotional intelligence (EI) was not a significant predictor of climbing confidence, suggesting that general emotional dispositions may not strongly translate to domain-specific performance perceptions. This is consistent with the findings of Garrido-Palomino and España-Romero (2019), who noted that while EI might differentiate among climbers at different skill levels, its predictive power may depend on the type of EI measured used (trait vs. ability-based). Future work may benefit from incorporating ability-based assessments to better capture the dynamic regulation of emotion in performance contexts.

We also found that EI was positively associated with mindfulness. This relationship suggests a potential indirect pathway by which emotional traits enhance sport climbing competence, likely through improved emotional awareness, attention regulation, and resilience. Dispositional mindfulness has been associated with adaptive emotional regulation and the ability to stay focused under pressure (Cerin and Barnett, 2011), both of which are essential when managing stressors such as fear of falling. Moreover, being persistent in pursuing long-term goals despite challenges reflects the psychological trait of grit, which has shown strong associations with sport performance (Duckworth et al., 2007) and climbing in particular (Ionel et al., 2023). These traits may contribute to athletes’ ability to confront emotionally demanding situations in climbing without resorting to avoidance or disengagement, as also observed by Steca et al. (2018) in their examination of emotional stability and self-regulation in athletic contexts.

A noteworthy finding from our model is the bidirectional relationship between mindfulness and climbing competence. Specifically, higher mindfulness levels negatively predicted competence, whereas higher competence positively predicted mindfulness. This may indicate that individuals with greater mindfulness dispositions are not necessarily advanced climbers, perhaps due to lifestyle or personality characteristics unrelated to climbing experience. Conversely, as climbers gain experience and confidence, their ability to maintain focused awareness during complex climbs may increase, reflecting a performance-enhanced development of mindfulness. These interpretations align with findings by Wheatley (2023), who observed that bouldering training, but not strength and conditioning, improved mindfulness among young adults.

This observation is also supported by research framed within the Embodied Cognition (EC) perspective, which emphasizes the feedback loop between sensorimotor engagement and cognitive development (Rucińska, 2021; Carius et al., 2020). Engaging in complex motor activities like sport climbing may enhance athletes’ bodily awareness and attentional control through repetitive, high-demand tasks.

First, the gender distribution in our sample was skewed toward males, limiting the generalizability of findings. This mirrors broader trends in sport climbing research (Boudreau et al., 2022; Diez-Fernández et al., 2023). As female participation grows (Sport e Salute, 2023), future research should strive for greater representation to avoid perpetuating gender bias in this emerging field.

Second, data collection occurred during the semi-lockdown period of the COVID-19 pandemic (November 2020–April 2021), which may have influenced participants’ training patterns, emotional states, and psychological responses. While most research during this period has focused on elite athletes, few studies address the effects of pandemic conditions on non-elite recreational sport participants (Carnevale Pellino et al., 2022).

Third, although we included a Retrospective Design Analysis (RDA), the lack of an a priori power analysis limits the strength of our conclusions, particularly regarding small effect sizes. RDA should be viewed as an exploratory tool rather than a substitute for prospective planning. Future studies should incorporate preregistered protocols and power estimations.

This study offers insights for training approaches that integrate both psychological and physical components. Enhancing domain-specific self-efficacy—climbing confidence—may be a key strategy for improving competence. Moreover, fostering mindfulness and the capacity for flow could further support climbers’ psychological resilience and focus. Coaches and sport psychologists might consider incorporating mindfulness techniques and flow-inducing strategies into training programs, particularly in preparation for complex route navigation and high-pressure scenarios.

Our findings highlight the multifaceted nature of sport climbing competence, shaped by a combination of experience, training frequency, and psychological dispositions. They also underscore the importance of theoretical precision and population specificity in recreational sport psychology research. Future studies should explore how motivational climates and social dynamics shape performance outcomes in sport climbing (Lacerda et al., 2021; Bortoli et al., 2011), contributing to a more nuanced understanding of this rapidly growing discipline.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

Informed consent was obtained during the post-selection phase of participants and in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the Department of Psychology at the University of Padua, Italy. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MT: Conceptualization, investigation, writing – original draft, writing – review & editing, data curation, Formal Analysis. VT: Writing – original draft, writing – review & editing. FG: Data curation, Formal Analysis, methodology, writing – original draft, writing – review & editing. SL: Investigation, writing – original draft. IL: Conceptualization, funding acquisition, investigation, project administration, supervision, writing – original draft, writing – review & editing

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. The publication costs were funded by the University of Padova.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aras, D., and Akalan, C. (2014). The effect of anxiety about falling on selected physiological parameters with different rope protocols in sport rock climbing. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fit. 54, 1–8.

Google Scholar

Aras, D., and Ewert, A. W. (2016). The effects of eight weeks of sport rock climbing training on anxiety. Acta Med. Medit. 32, 223–230. doi: 10.19193/0393-6384_2016_1_35

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bahaeloo-Horeh, S., and Assari, S. (2008). Students experience self-esteem improvement during mountaineering. Wilderness Environ. Med. 19, 181–185. doi: 10.1580/07-WEME-OR-130

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York, NY: Freeman. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.13.2.158

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol. Bull. 107, 238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boffi, M., Diana, B., Riva, E., Inghilleri, P., Argenton, L., Villani, D., et al. (2012). “L’adattamento italiano della Dispositional Flow Scale II-DFS 2,” in Flow, Benessere e Prestazione Eccellente. Dai Modelli Teorici Alle Applicazioni Nello Sport e in Azienda, eds M. Muzio, G. Riva, and L. Argenton (Milan: FrancoAngeli srl), 143–161.

Google Scholar

Bortoli, L., Bertollo, M., Comani, S., and Robazza, C. (2011). Competence, achievement goals, motivational climate, and pleasant psychobiosocial states in youth sport. J. Sports Sci. 29, 171–180. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2010.530679

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boudreau, P., Mackenzie, S. H., and Hodge, K. (2022). Optimal psychological states in advanced climbers: Antecedents, characteristics, and consequences of flow and clutch states. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 60:102155. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2022.102155

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociol. Methods Res. 21, 230–258. doi: 10.1177/0049124192021002005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carius, D., Hörnig, L., Ragert, P., and Kaminski, E. (2020). Characterizing cortical hemodynamic changes during climbing and its relation to climbing expertise. Neurosci. Lett. 715:134604. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2020.134604

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carnevale Pellino, V., Lovecchio, N., Puci, M. V., Marin, L., Gatti, A., Pirazzi, A., et al. (2022). Effects of the lockdown period on the mental health of elite athletes during the COVID-19 pandemic: A narrative review. Sport Sci. Health 18, 1187–1199. doi: 10.1007/s11332-022-00876-w

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cerin, E., and Barnett, A. (2011). Predictors of pre-and post-competition affective states in male martial artists: A multilevel interactional approach. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 21, 137–150. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2009.01006.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). ). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York, NY: Harper. doi: 10.5465/amr.1991.4279513

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2008). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York, NY: Harper Perennial Modern Classics.

Google Scholar

Csikszentmihalyi, M., and LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 56, 815–822. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.56.5.815

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Fabio, A., and Palazzeschi, L. (2011). Proprietà psicometriche del trait emotional intelligence questionnaire short form (TEIQue-SF) nel contesto italiano. Counseling 4, 327–336.

Google Scholar

Diez-Fernández, P., Ruibal-Lista, B., Rico-Díaz, J., Rodríguez-Fernández, J. E., and López-García, S. (2023). Performance factors in sport climbing: A systematic review. Sustainability 15:16687. doi: 10.3390/su152416687

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Draper, N., Dickson, T., Blackwell, G., Fryer, S., Priestley, S., Winter, D., et al. (2011). Self-reported ability assessment in rock climbing. J. Sports Sci. 29, 851–858. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2011.565362

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M. D., and Kelly, D. R. (2007). Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-term goals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 1087–1101. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1087

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Federazione Arrampicata Sportiva Italiana [FASI] (2025). Regolamento di Gara F.A.S.I. Attività Agonistica Anno 2025. Available online at: https://www.federclimb.it/federazione/statuto-e-regolamenti/regolamenti-gare/2025-3.html (accessed February 17, 2025).

Google Scholar

Gajdošík, J., Baláš, J., and Draper, N. (2020). Effect of height on perceived exertion and physiological responses for climbers of differing ability levels. Front. Psychol. 11:997. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00997

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrido-Palomino, I., and España-Romero, V. (2019). Role of emotional intelligence on rock climbing performance. J. Sports Psychol. 28, 123–135. doi: 10.5232/ricyde2019.05706

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrido-Palomino, I., and España-Romero, V. (2023). Fear of falling in women: A psychological training intervention improves climbing performance. J. Sports Sci. 41, 1518–1529. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2023.2281157

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelman, A., and Carlin, J. (2014). Beyond power calculations: Assessing type S (sign) and type M (magnitude) errors. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 9, 641–651. doi: 10.1177/1745691614551642

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equat. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iacobucci, D. (2010). Structural equations modeling: Fit indices, sample size, and advanced topics. J. Consum. Psychol. 20, 90–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2009.09.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ilgner, A. (2003). The Rock Warrior’s Way, 2nd Edn. Rio de Janeiro: Desiderata Institute.

Google Scholar

Ionel, M. S., Ion, A., and Visu-Petra, L. (2023). Personality, grit, and performance in rock climbing: Down to the nitty-gritty. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 21, 306–328. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2022.2044368

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. A., and Eklund, R. C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: The flow state scale–2 and dispositional flow scale–2. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 24, 133–150. doi: 10.1123/jsep.24.2.133

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness Meditation in Everyday Life. London: Hachette Books.

Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 4th Edn. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Laborde, S., Brüll, A., Weber, J., and Anders, L. S. (2011). Trait emotional intelligence in sports: A protective role against stress through heart rate variability? Pers. Individ. Differ. 51, 23–27. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laborde, S., Dosseville, F., and Allen, M. S. (2015). Emotional intelligence in sport and exercise: A systematic review. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 26, 862–874. doi: 10.1111/sms.12510

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laborde, S., Dosseville, F., Guillén, F., and Chávez, E. (2014a). Validity of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire in sports and its links with performance satisfaction. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 15, 481–490. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.05.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laborde, S., Lautenbach, F., Allen, M. S., Herbert, C., and Achtzehn, S. (2014b). The role of trait emotional intelligence in emotion regulation and performance under pressure. Pers. Individ. Diff. 57, 43–47. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.09.013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laborde, S., You, M., Dosseville, F., and Salinas, A. (2012). Culture, individual differences, and situation: Influence on coping in French and Chinese table tennis players. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 12, 255–261. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2011.566367

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacerda, A., Filgueiras, A., Campos, M., Keegan, R., and Landeira-Fernández, J. (2021). Motivational climate measures in sport: A systematic review. Spanish J. Psychol. 24:e27. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2021.27

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leo, I., Leone, S., Dicataldo, R., Vivenzio, C., Cavallin, N., Taglioni, C., et al. (2022). A non-randomized pilot study on the benefits of baby swimming on motor development. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:9262. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19159262

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Llewellyn, D. J., and Sanchez, X. (2008). Individual differences and risk taking in rock climbing. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 9, 413–426. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2007.07.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., and Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychol. Methods 1, 130–149. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.130

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mangan, K., Andrews, K., Miles, B., and Draper, N. (2024). The psychology of rock climbing: A systematic review. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 76:102763. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2024.102763

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V. (2009). Technical Manual for the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaires (TEIQue), 1st Edn. London: London Psychometric Laboratory, doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-88370-0_5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V., and Furnham, A. (2001). Trait emotional intelligence: Psychometric investigation with reference to established trait taxonomies. Eur. J. Personal. 15, 425–448. doi: 10.1002/per.416

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prezza, M., Trombaccia, F. R., and Armento, L. (1997). La scala dell’autostima di Rosenberg. Traduzione e validazione italiana [Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Italian Translation and Validation]. Bollett. Psicol. Appl. 223, 35–44.

Google Scholar

Raab, M., and Araújo, D. (2019). Embodied cognition with and without mental representations: The case of embodied choices in sports. Front. Psychol. 10:1825. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01825

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. doi: 10.1126/science.148.3671.804

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rucińska, Z. (2021). Enactive planning in rock climbing: Recalibration, visualization, and nested affordances. Synthese 199, 5285–5310. doi: 10.1007/s11229-021-03025-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saul, D., Steinmetz, G., Lehmann, W., and Schilling, A. F. (2019). Determinants for success in climbing: A systematic review. J. Exerc. Sci. Fit. 17, 91–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jesf.2019.04.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H., and Müller, H. (2003). Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: Tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods Psychol. Res. Online 8, 23–74.

Google Scholar

Šimleša, M., Guegan, J., Blanchard, E., Tarpin-Bernard, F., and Buisine, S. (2018). The flow engine framework: A cognitive model of optimal human experience. Eur. J. Psychol. 14, 232–253. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v14i1.1370

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sport e Salute (2023). Arrampicata Sportiva: Boom di Praticanti E Quasi La Metà è donna. Venerdì al Foro Italico si sfidano i Più Grandi Campioni Europei. Available online at: https://www.sportesalute.eu/primo-piano/arrampicata-sportiva-boom-di-praticanti-e-quasi-la-meta-e-donna-venerdi-al-foro-italico-si-sfidano-i-piu-grandi-campioni-europei.html (accessed September 12, 2023)

Google Scholar

Steca, P., Baretta, D., Greco, A., D’Addario, M., and Monzani, D. (2018). Associations between personality, sports participation, and athletic success: A comparison of big five in sporting and non-sporting adults. Pers. Individ. Differ. 121, 176–183. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.09.043

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steimer, J., and Weissert, R. (2017). Effects of sport climbing on multiple sclerosis. Front. Physiol. 8:1021. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2017.01021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinmetz, G., Assmann, M., Hubert, J., and Saul, D. (2022). Recreational climbers are more conscientious than recreational athletes: A case control study. BMC Sports Sci. Med. Rehabil. 14:94. doi: 10.1186/s13102-022-00491-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephan, M. A., Krattinger, S., Pasquier, J., Bashir, S., Fournier, T., Ruegg, D. G., et al. (2011). Effect of long-term climbing training on cerebellar ataxia: A case series. Rehabil. Res. Pract. 2011:525879. doi: 10.1155/2011/525879

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker, L. R., and Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika 38, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/BF02291170

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallet, G. T., Brunel, L., Riou, B., and Vermeulen, N. (2016). Dynamics of sensorimotor interactions in embodied cognition. Front. Psychol. 6:1929. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01929

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wellsby, M., and Pexman, P. M. (2014). Developing embodied cognition: Insights from children’s concepts and language processing. Front. Psychol. 5:506. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00506

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheatley, K. A. (2023). Exploring the relationship between mindfulness and rock-climbing: A controlled study. Curr. Psychol. 42, 2680–2692. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-01593-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickens, C. D., McLaughlin, A. C., Keller, J., and Tan, J. (2018). “Task switching in rock climbing: Validation of a computational model for different skill levels,” in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, Vol. 62, (London: SAGE Publications), 651–655. doi: 10.1177/1541931218621149

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, P. R., and Knight, E. L. (2014). Use of psychological skills by risk sport athletes. J. Hum. Perform. Extreme Environ. 11:2. doi: 10.7771/2327-2937.1061

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zieliński, G., Byś, A., Baszczowski, M., Ginszt, M., Suwała, M., and Majcher, P. (2018). The influence of sport climbing on depression and anxiety levels: Literature review. J. Educ. Health Sport 8, 336–344. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.131822

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: embodied cognition, emotional intelligence, self-esteem, flow, mindfulness, climbing, self-efficacy

Citation: Turchetto M, Tomaselli V, Giorgi F, Leone S and Leo I (2025) Sport climbing competence is influenced by training frequency, experience, self-efficacy, flow, and emotional intelligence. Front. Psychol. 16:1518495. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1518495

Received: 28 November 2024; Accepted: 14 May 2025;
Published: 06 June 2025.

Edited by:

Katrina Kessler, Independent Researcher, Seattle, United States

Reviewed by:

Antonia Ioana Vasile, National University of Physical Education and Sport, Romania
Kat Wheatley, King’s College London, United Kingdom
Martin Barry, University of Central Lancashire, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2025 Turchetto, Tomaselli, Giorgi, Leone and Leo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Irene Leo, aXJlbmUubGVvQHVuaXBkLml0

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.