ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 14 February 2025

Sec. Educational Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1519454

Academic performance, self-reported motivation, and affect in higher education: the role of basic psychological need satisfaction

  • 1Departamento de Psicología, Universidad de La Serena, La Serena, Chile
  • 2Departamento de Artes y Letras, Universidad de La Serena, La Serena, Chile
  • 3Departamento de Educación, Universidad de La Serena, La Serena, Chile

Introduction: Higher education is a milestone in students’ lives; however, it often comes with various challenges. In this context, Basic Psychological Needs Theory emerges as a framework to understand a series of significant factors that influence students’ academic experiences, such as motivation and affect. Although there are studies that assess the association between basic psychological needs (BPN), motivation, affect and academic performance separately, there is a lack of research integrating all these variables in a higher education context. The first objective of the study was to evaluate the differences in BPN satisfaction, positive and negative affect, and academic performance between the courses perceived as the most motivating and those perceived as the least motivating. The second objective of the study was to examine the relationship between the studied variables.

Methods: This non-experimental cross-sectional study included a sample of 148 higher education students from Chile. Paired sample t-tests were performed to compare the levels of the study variables between the courses, followed by structural equation modeling (SEM).

Results: Findings for the t-tests reveal that courses considered the most motivating showed higher positive affect, lower negative affect, higher BPN satisfaction, and better academic performance. Results obtained through the SEM show that BPN satisfaction has an indirect effect on academic performance, mediated by affect and self-reported motivation. In addition, a direct effect from negative affect to academic performance was found.

Discussion: These results contribute to a better understanding of how BPN satisfaction influences the academic performance of university students, and reinforce the usefulness of Self-determination Theory (SDT) in explaining motivational and affective phenomena in higher education.

1 Introduction

Higher education represents a significant milestone in students’ lives, marking the beginning of a phase of academic and personal growth (Leow et al., 2023). However, this period also brings challenges for higher education institutions, with student motivation being one of the most prominent (Brahm et al., 2017). Creating a motivating class is of paramount importance because, as recent studies highlight, motivation has a significant effect on students’ engagement (Karimi and Sotoodeh, 2019), self-efficacy (Li et al., 2023), and well-being (Tang et al., 2021).

In this context, Self-Determination Theory (SDT) provides a robust theoretical framework for understanding how the satisfaction of basic psychological needs (BPN)—autonomy, competence, and relatedness—impacts variables such as motivation and academic performance, which are fundamental to academic success (Ryan and Deci, 2001, 2020). In recent years, SDT has been widely studied within the educational context (e.g., Carmona-Halty et al., 2019; Oram and Rogers, 2022; Fierro-Suero et al., 2022), highlighting its significance in understanding academic learning and life satisfaction (Shi et al., 2024). Despite advancements in this field, a significant gap remains in understanding how these mechanisms function among university students in Latin America, particularly in Chile.

Basic Psychological Needs Theory, a sub-theory of SDT, posits the existence of three universal needs—autonomy, competence and relatedness—that are necessary for optimal psychological development (Ryan and Deci, 2017). As Ryan and Deci (2017) propose, autonomy refers to the need to independently regulate one’s actions and experiences, competence relates to the necessity of feeling proficient and effective, and relatedness entails the need for social connection. Satisfying these needs fosters proactive, prosocial and growth-oriented inclinations, promotes healthy adjustment, and contributes to overall well-being (Vansteenkiste et al., 2023).

The relevance of BPN satisfaction for positive educational outcomes is well established. Fulfillment of these needs positively predicts positive affect, negatively predicts negative affect, and enhances motivation, which in turn improves academic engagement and performance (Schutte and Malouff, 2021; Méndez-Aguado et al., 2020; Aydın and Michou, 2020; Liu et al., 2024). In an educational setting, motivation is particularly relevant, as SDT posits that enhancing it can lead to increased student achievement (Ryan and Deci, 2020).

Consistent with Basic Psychological Needs Theory, the association between BPN and motivation has been previously shown (e.g., Del Valle et al., 2025; Basileo et al., 2024; Lombas and Esteban, 2018). This latter construct, defined as the energy that drives people to choose, prioritize and act on their desires (Dörnyei and Ottó, 1998), is a crucial cognitive component for effective learning activities (Hurtado-Bermúdez and Romero-Abrio, 2023).

According to SDT (Ryan and Deci, 2020), motivation is classified into different types along a spectrum based on their degree of self-determination. At the most self-determined end lies intrinsic motivation, defined as the inherent drive to seek new learning experiences and apply previously acquired knowledge. Moving along the spectrum, extrinsic motivation reflects actions driven by the integration of external regulations with personal values and needs. At the least self-determined end is amotivation, characterized by a lack of intention or willingness to act.

Another theory that explores different aspects of motivation is the Achievement Goal Theory (Elliot and Hulleman, 2017). This theoretical framework states that different types of goals lead to different patterns of affect, cognition and behaviors, and thus, different academic outcomes. The mastery goals emphasize developing skills and achieving mastery of a task, while the performance goals center on showcasing competence in comparison to others. In this context, research has shown that mastery goals are associated with an adaptive motivational pattern, marked by a tendency to seek challenges and demonstrate strong persistence when facing obstacles, while performance goals are linked to avoiding challenges and displaying low persistence (Dweck, 1986).

Recent research has demonstrated that students’ motivation within the classroom is significantly influenced by their emotions (Vaculíková, 2021; Florescu et al., 2023; Wang et al., 2024), underscoring the increasing importance of the latter variable in educational contexts (Schutz and Pekrun, 2007). Emotions, as natural reactions that impact an individual’s mental state, not only condition responses to various situations (Méndez-Aguado et al., 2020), but also play a pivotal role in everyday life.

Emotions are a fundamental type of affective state that arise as reactions toward specific goals, or perceiving changes in relation to those goals (Batson et al., 1992). In contrast, affect is a broader construct, defined as a relatively stable inclination to experience specific moods and emotions across various contexts (Grzybowski et al., 2021). Watson et al. (1988) addressed affect through a two-dimensional model, distinguishing between Positive Affect and Negative Affect. Positive Affect is associated with pleasant subjective states, whereas Negative Affect encompasses a broad spectrum of distress, such as guilt, fear, or irritability (Schutz and Pekrun, 2007; Shiota et al., 2021; Watson et al., 1988). This widely validated model (Díaz-García et al., 2020) considers both dimensions as fundamental components of psychological well-being, as both negative affect and positive affect mediate the relationship between psychological need satisfaction and outcomes such as mental health and academic performance (Schutte and Malouff, 2021).

Crucial to understanding affect are their key components. Two pivotal dimensions in the conceptualization of affect are valence and arousal (Russell, 1980, 2003). Valence refers to the level of pleasantness associated with an event, which can be positioned along a continuum ranging from negative to positive; while arousal or intensity is defined as the level of autonomic activation, ranging from low to high (Bestelmeyer et al., 2017). In this sense, it is believed that positive activating emotions, such as enjoyment, increase motivation, while negative deactivating emotions, such as boredom, decrease it (Pekrun et al., 2007).

Affect has been shown to indirectly influence academic performance (Rodríguez-Muñoz et al., 2021), while motivation has a direct impact on this variable (Méndez-Aguado et al., 2020). Academic performance, usually evaluated through grades, is the benchmark by which student competence is measured (Reed, 2009). Poor academic performance not only has psychological consequences for students but also social and familial repercussions (Najimi et al., 2013).

Despite existing research, comprehensive studies integrating basic psychological needs, affect, motivation, and academic performance in higher education are limited. While previous studies have explored these variables individually or in pairs (e.g., Rodríguez-Muñoz et al., 2021; Schutte and Malouff, 2021), few have examined their interactions within a cohesive structural model. This study aims to address this limitation.

The first objective of this investigation was to evaluate the differences in BPN satisfaction, positive and negative affect, and academic performance between the courses perceived as the most motivating and those perceived as the least motivating by university students. Consistent with SDT and previous research, it was hypothesized that the most motivating courses would exhibit higher levels of BPN satisfaction, academic performance, and positive affect, as well as lower levels of negative affect.

The second objective of the study was to explore the relationships among the study variables using a structural model. As depicted in Figure 1, it was hypothesized that BPN satisfaction positively predicts motivation and positive affect, while negatively predicting negative affect. Positive affect was expected to positively predict motivation, whereas negative affect was anticipated to negatively predict to it. Finally, motivation was expected to positively predict academic performance. This model is grounded in evidence supporting the predictive roles of affect and motivation in educational outcomes (Rodríguez-Muñoz et al., 2021; Méndez-Aguado et al., 2020).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. General model representing the second hypothesis of the study. BPNS, basic psychological need satisfaction; PA, positive affect; NA, negative affect; M, self-reported motivation; AP, academic performance. This Figure was created using Mplus 8.11 (Muthen and Muthen, 2024).

Understanding the pathways linking motivation, affect, and performance is essential for designing effective educational interventions. Examining these dynamics in the context of higher education in Chile offers valuable insights into how cultural and contextual factors influence these relationships. Addressing this gap is crucial for developing culturally tailored strategies that enhance student well-being and academic success in Latin America. Additionally, the findings will contribute to theoretical and empirical advancements by shedding light on how psychological needs, emotions, and motivation interact to shape academic outcomes. This integrated approach provides a robust framework for improving educational practices and outcomes in higher education.

2 Methods

2.1 Subjects

The inclusion criteria for participation in the present study were: (1) being a higher education student; (2) being enrolled in a program of humanities, in line with the structure of academic programs in Chile (Pedagogy, Psychology, Music, Arts, Literature, or Law); and (3) being in either the first or fourth year of study. The careers considered were limited to the aforementioned in order to ensure greater homogeneity within the sample, allowing for more meaningful comparisons across participants from different academic backgrounds. Similarly, the sample was intentionally delimited to first- and fourth-year students to minimize potential biases between new and senior students during data collection, and to ensure comparability across academic levels. A convenience sampling method was employed to facilitate participant recruitment, leveraging the goodwill of instructors who granted access to students in their courses.

This study included a nonrandom sample of 148 higher education students from various disciplines: pedagogy (35%), psychology (33%), law (29%). The majority of participants were from the Coquimbo region (94%), with 6% from other regions of Chile. Among the participants, 84 (56.8%) were first-year students and 64 (43.2%) were fourth-year students. In terms of gender distribution, 56 (37.8%) identified as male, 87 (58.8%) as female, and 5 (3.4%) did not disclose their gender. The mean age of the participants was 21 years (SD = 3.21), with ages ranging from 18 to 35 years.

The initial sample consisted of 159 students. However, data quality assurance measures resulted in the exclusion of 11 participants. Three were excluded due to responses deviating more than four standard deviations from the mean, indicating potential outliers. Additionally, eight participants were excluded due to response invariability, thereby enhancing the overall reliability of the data.

2.2 Instruments

The instruments for this study were adapted from previous tools, specifically to meet two important requirements. First, they needed to be contextualized for a course or subject spanning an entire semester. Second, they had to include a small number of items to ensure ease of application and, consequently, the feasibility of the study.

Self-reported motivation throughout the semester in a given course was assessed using a scale from 1 to 10, where 1 represented low self-reported motivation and 10 represented high self-reported motivation (“Indicate how motivating the course was on a scale of 1 to 10, where 1 is the least motivating and 10 is the most motivating”). This item on motivation allowed for the assessment of the magnitude of motivation in each of the two courses previously identified as the most and least motivating. Although single-item measurements have been criticized in the past, they offer advantages when a general and brief assessment is required (Allen et al., 2022). It is important to note that this measurement does not aim to capture a hypothetical construct but rather to provide a general estimate of students’ self-reported motivation, based on their own understanding of this construct. Since it is a single-item measure, it is not possible to obtain psychometric indicators of validity and reliability.

Affect was measured using a modified version of the Chilean validation of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (Dufey and Fernández, 2012). Two psychologists, one specializing in affectivity, and a professor with expertise in teaching, provided judgments for content validity. This adaptation comprised 5 items measuring positive affect (happiness, interest, enthusiasm, confidence, and satisfaction) and 5 items measuring negative affect (embarrassment, fear, sadness, frustration, and anger). Item selection was guided by the relevance of the affect or emotion within the context of various situations encountered in a course. Participants were prompted to indicate the intensity of the affects experienced on average throughout the semester within the course. Responses were scaled from none (0 points) to very much (4 points).

To assess their internal structure, an Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) was conducted using a polychoric matrix in the Mplus 8.11 software (Muthen and Muthen, 2024). The procedure followed the guidelines outlined by Lloret-Segura et al. (2014). The WLSMV estimator was employed with GEOMIN rotation. The resulting solution, which demonstrated both theoretical coherence and satisfactory fit indices, consisted of two factors. The RMSEA was 0.063 (CI 90% 0.042–0.085), CFI was 0.994, TLI was 0.989, and SRMR was 0.023. These factors grouped all positive affect items into one factor and negative affect items into another. The negative affect factor exhibited internal consistencies of 0.850 and 0.852 using Cronbach’s alpha and McDonald’s omega, respectively. Meanwhile, the positive affect factor showed internal consistencies of 0.903 and 0.904 using Cronbach’s alpha and McDonald’s omega, respectively.

BPN satisfaction within the context of a course was measured using an ad hoc scale specifically developed for the university course setting. Initially, the scale comprised a total of 10 items: three items assessing the Competence need (e.g., “The course challenged me to develop my abilities”), three items evaluating Autonomy (e.g., “Throughout the course, I had the opportunity to freely choose certain topics I wanted to explore further”), and four items addressing Relatedness (e.g., “During the course, I felt valued by the instructor”). Content validity was established through the evaluation of two psychologists specializing in self-determination theory and one psychologist experienced in measurement techniques.

To assess its internal structure, an Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) was conducted, following the methodology employed for the previous instrument. This analysis was thus performed using a polychoric matrix with the Mplus 8.11 software, adhering to the guidelines outlined by Lloret-Segura et al. (2014). The WLSMV estimator with GEOMIN rotation was utilized. The best solution consisted of a single factor with 8 items covering the three BPN of the model. The fit indices for this model were as follows: RMSEA of 0.068 (CI 0.021–0.106), CFI of 0.984, TLI of 0.978, and SRMR of 0.060. This factor exhibited an internal consistency of 0.906 for Cronbach’s alpha and 0.910 for McDonald’s omega.

Finally, to assess academic performance, participants were asked to recall and report the average grade obtained in each of the referenced courses: the one they perceived as the least motivating and the one they perceived as the most motivating.

2.3 Procedure

All instruments were administered online using Google Forms. The contacted students were invited to participate and were given a flyer that included a QR code to access the form. Before proceeding with the survey, participants were required to review and agree to an informed consent form detailing the objectives of the research, anticipated outcomes, and any possible minimal risks associated with their involvement. This document also identified the principal investigator along with their contact information, and informed participants of their right to withdraw from the study. No personal information, including names or other identifiers, was requested. The research project was previously evaluated and approved by the Scientific Ethics Committee of Universidad de La Serena, in compliance with the ethical principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki.

The survey included a sociodemographic questionnaire, followed by several key scales: a positive and negative affect scale, a BPN scale, a question regarding the motivation level, and the average grade obtained during the course. Participants were required to complete these scales twice: once thinking about the most motivating course and once thinking about the least motivating course of the last semester.

It is important to clarify that in Chile most academic programs have rigid curricula with a single option for courses and instructors for all students within a given program. This means that students do not have the option to choose their courses or instructors, except in some cases where elective courses are offered. Elective courses were not considered in this study.

2.4 Data analysis

Eleven participants were identified whose responses were either above four standard deviations or exhibited a lack of variability across different items. Consequently, all responses from these participants were excluded. No further data adjustments were made to the matrix. After cleaning the data, descriptive and comparative analyses were conducted using Jamovi 2.3.28 (The Jamovi Project, 2023). Specifically, paired-sample t-tests were performed to compare the levels of the study variables between the courses that participants identified as the most and the least motivating.

To test the hypothesized model, responses from the most motivating and the least motivating courses were combined into a single data set, resulting in a total of 296 data points. This new data set was then subjected to a correlation analysis using Jamovi 2.3.28. Following this, a Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) analysis was conducted using Mplus 8.11, employing the WLSMV estimator.

To evaluate the fit of the hypothesized model, the following indices were used: Chi-square (χ2), which assesses the discrepancy between the observed covariance matrix and the one estimated by the model. An associated p-value greater than 0.05 indicates good fit; however, this statistic is sensitive to sample size. The Comparative Fit Index (CFI) compares the proposed model with a null model assuming independence among variables. CFI values above 0.95 suggest good model fit. The Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), also known as the Non-Normed Fit Index, compares the fit of the specified model to that of a null model while penalizing for model complexity. TLI values above 0.95 indicate good fit. The Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) measures the discrepancy per degree of freedom in the model. Following Steiger (2007) recommendation, values below 0.07 indicate a good model fit. The Weighted Root Mean Square Residual (WRMR) evaluates the weighted discrepancy between the observed and estimated covariance matrices. A WRMR value below 1.0 indicates acceptable model fit (DiStefano et al., 2017).

3 Results

3.1 Description of study variables

Table 1 depicts the means and variability of the studied variables. Overall, as hypothesized, means tend to be higher in the context of the most motivating courses, except for negative affect, which exhibits higher values in the least motivating contexts. The Shapiro–Wilk test reveals that the distribution of the majority of variables deviates from normality.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. General description of the study variables.

3.2 Comparisons between the most motivating courses and the least motivating courses

In order to further examine the comparisons of self-reported motivation, positive affect, negative affect, BPN satisfaction, and academic performance in the most motivating and least motivating courses, a paired samples t-test was employed. The analysis revealed statistically significant differences across all variables, accompanied by a large effect size. As observed, the mean values were consistently higher in the most motivating course context for all variables, except for negative affect, which exhibited lower levels (refer to Table 2 for details).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Study variables comparison in the context of the most and the least motivating course.

3.3 Correlations between study variables

Table 3 presents the Spearman correlations between the study variables. Every variable significantly correlated with each other. Specifically, positive affect had a significant, high-strength positive correlation with motivation and BPN satisfaction. Similarly, motivation also showed a high-strength positive correlation with BPN satisfaction. Weaker correlation coefficients were seen between the rest of the variables.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Spearman correlations between the study variables.

3.4 Structural model of basic psychological need satisfaction, affect, self-reported motivation, and academic performance

The results of the initial model, shown in Figure 1, indicated a reasonable but not optimal fit to the data (χ2 = 462.183, p = 0.0000, CFI =0.976, TLI = 0.973, WRMR = 1.134, RMSEA = 0.079, 90% CI = 0.070 to 0.087). Therefore, it was necessary to evaluate an alternative model (Figure 2). To create a revised structure, new associations between the variables were established. The SEM results indicated that the adjusted model fits the data well, while retaining theoretical consistency with the original model (χ2 = 375.627, p = 0.0000, CFI = 0.983, TLI = 0.980, WRMR = 0.977, RMSEA = 0.068, 90% CI = 0.059 to 0.077).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Adjusted general model of association between the study variables. Standardized coefficients are presented outside the parentheses, while standard errors are shown within the parentheses. BPNS, basic psychological need satisfaction; PA, positive affect; NA, negative affect; M, self-reported motivation; AP, academic performance. This Figure was created using Mplus 8.11 (Muthen and Muthen, 2024).

3.5 Standardized direct, indirect and total effects

In the adjusted model, all direct effects were significant. BPN satisfaction was positively associated with positive affect (β = 0.834, p = 0.000, 95% CI = 0.790 to 0.878), and motivation (β = 0.258, p = 0.000, 95% CI = 0.119 to 0.397), and inversely associated with negative affect (β = −0.447, p = 0.000, 95% CI = −0.546 to −0.347). Additionally, positive affect was positively associated with motivation (β = 0.586, p = 0.000, 95% CI = 0.460 to 0.712), while negative affect was inversely associated with this variable (β = −0.180, p = 0.000, 95% CI = −0.256 to −0.104). In contrast to the initial model, the adjusted model includes an inverse association between negative affect and academic performance (β = −0.332, p = 0.000, 95% CI = −0.454 to −0.209). Finally, as expected, motivation was positively associated with academic performance (β = 0.254, p = 0.000, 95% CI = 0.124 to 0.384).

Significant indirect effects between the study variables were also examined. As seen in Table 4, BPN satisfaction had a significant indirect effect on both academic performance and motivation, with the latter being the strongest association when positive affect served as a mediating variable (β = 0.489, p = 0.000, 95% CI = 0.374 to 0.604). Additionally, motivation mediated the relationship between BPN satisfaction and academic performance, both independently (β = 0.0.66, p = 0.009, 95% CI = 0.017 to 0.115) and in conjunction with positive affect (β = 0.124, p = 0.001, 95% CI = 0.053 to 0.196) and negative affect (β = 0.020, p = 0.004, 95% CI = 0.006 to 0.034). Furthermore, academic performance was also indirectly affected by positive affect (β = 0.149, p = 0.000, 95% CI = 0.065 to 0.233) and negative affect (β = −0.046, p = 0.002, 95% CI = −0.075 to −0.017) through self-reported motivation, with the latter being the only negative indirect effect found.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Indirect relations between the study variables.

Finally, when considering both direct and indirect pathways, BPN satisfaction had a significant total effect on academic performance (β = 0.358, p = 0.000, 95% CI = 0.269 to 0.448).

4 Discussion

The first objective of the study was to assess differences in BPN satisfaction, positive and negative affect, and academic performance between courses perceived as the most and the least motivating by students. As anticipated, the most motivating courses were associated with greater positive affect, reduced negative affect, higher BPN satisfaction, and better academic performance. These findings are consistent with the principles of SDT, highlighting not only the importance of self-reported motivation and academic performance, commonly associated with students’ academic experience, but also the relevance of BPN satisfaction and affect as equally fundamental factors. Furthermore, a considerable effect size was observed in all comparisons made, suggesting a high probability of replicability of these results in future samples.

Overall, these findings align with previous research emphasizing the fundamental role of BPN satisfaction in fostering motivation and academic performance (Ryan and Deci, 2001; Shang et al., 2024). The significant differences observed in BPN satisfaction, affect, and performance between the most and least motivating courses are consistent with predictions derived from Self-Determination Theory (Ryan and Deci, 2017).

To further explore the relationships between the variables studied, the second objective of this research was to examine the relationship between BPN satisfaction, academic performance, positive and negative affect, and self-reported motivation. While the initial model was theoretically robust, it exhibited a reasonable but suboptimal fit to the data, prompting necessary adjustments. In response, the revised model introduced additional associations, including a notable adjustment: the incorporation of a direct inverse relationship between negative affect and academic performance, which had not been hypothesized in the initial model. As anticipated, the findings revealed that BPN satisfaction predicts academic performance both directly and indirectly, with affect and motivation acting as mediators. These results underscore the importance of designing university courses that satisfy students’ BPN, as they are associated with greater self-reported motivation and improved academic performance—a core principle of SDT.

The results of this study also show that BPN satisfaction has a proportional effect on positive affect, while the opposite occurs with negative affect. Likewise, it was found that positive affect positively predicts self-reported motivation, while negative affect has the opposite effect on it. Analyzing this relationship is quite complex due to the diversity of functions and levels that affect has (González-Arias, 2023). Among these functions, there is the informative role of the body to the brain regarding homeostatic self-regulation at a behavioral level (Roth and Benita, 2023) and the motivational role of affects, which provide energy in response to environmental events (Pekrun et al., 2002). This allows to suggest that an environment that favors BPN satisfaction will involve the experimentation of positive affects, which promote approach behaviors with the purpose of taking advantage of the nourishing opportunities that this environment offers.

Similarly, the results show that students’ self-reported motivation has a direct and positive effect on academic performance. At a theoretical level, the relationship found between these variables can be explained by the facilitating role that motivation plays in the execution of academic activities. The effect of self-reported motivation on academic performance found in this study is consistent with that reported in previous studies (see Liu et al., 2024; Gumasing and Castro, 2023; Méndez-Aguado et al., 2020). However, the scientific literature presents contradictory evidence on this relationship, as some studies have not found a correlation between these two variables (e.g., Muñoz and Correa, 2023), while others have shown that motivation only has an indirect effect on academic performance (see Wu et al., 2020; Kusurkar et al., 2013). The lack of consensus on this issue may be due to limitations inherent in measuring academic performance through grades, which may be affected by reliability problems, variability in assessment criteria, and validity issues (Cain et al., 2022).

As it was previously stated, Achievement Goal Theory (Elliot and Hulleman, 2017) explains that different types of goals lead to different academic outcomes. Given the complex and interconnected systems within which classrooms operate—encompassing schools, homes, communities, and broader societal influences—students’ motives and their perceptions of performance may vary widely (Urdan and Kaplan, 2020). These variations are often shaped by cultural and pedagogical practices, which influence how students prioritize mastery or performance goals. In the Chilean educational context, for instance, it has been found that students in the areas of health and education present a tendency to study because of altruistic reasons, focusing in being capable to treat users effectively (Miranda-Ossandón et al., 2023), which can be linked to a better motivational pattern and thus better academic outcomes. This is relevant for explaining the results of the present study, as most of the sample was composed by students of psychology and pedagogies.

On the other hand, a non-expected inverse association between negative affect and academic performance was found. This suggests that negative affect not only indirectly predicts academic performance through motivation but also has a direct predictive effect. At a theoretical level, this result can be explained considering that negative affect has detrimental effects on cognitive processing, leading to greater attentional and psychological resources consumption and worse cognitive performance (Yang et al., 2023), which is key for achieving satisfactory academic outcomes.

There is no clear consensus on the impact of negative emotions on academic performance. For example, anxiety has been associated with both improved and diminished academic outcomes in students (Mirawdali et al., 2018; Al-Qaisy, 2011). However, a review by Alshareef et al. (2024) found that most good-quality studies indicate anxiety is linked to reduced academic performance. This aligns with the findings of the present study, as negative affects, such as anxiety, may impair memory function, interfere with judgment and cognitive processing, and reduce concentration (Alshareef et al., 2024). Consequently, professors face the critical challenge of reducing students’ experiences of shame, guilt, anger, and other negative affects. Addressing this is essential not only for fostering a healthy academic environment but also for supporting optimal academic performance, as negative affect can significantly disrupt the learning process.

Finally, the results of the present study also indicate that BPN satisfaction indirectly and positively predicts academic performance, aligning with the principles of Self-Determination Theory (SDT). However, some literature suggests a negative relationship between BPN satisfaction and academic performance among university students, arguing that satisfying these needs could reduce the interest in improving grades (Nishimura and Joshi, 2021). In addition, another study indicates that poor academic performance can reduce competence and relationship satisfaction, while grades could limit autonomy by leading students to choose courses that guarantee better grades instead of those more relevant to their learning or personal growth (Chamberlin et al., 2018). These antecedents show the inherent complexity of the relationship between BPN satisfaction and academic performance, which highlights the need for further research in this area.

Overall, the results of this study supports BPN satisfaction as a vitally important factor for the learning process of university students, as it fosters positive affects and decreases negative affects while significantly predicting self-reported motivation, which, in turn, translates into improved academic performance. These findings are consistent with the postulates of SDT, which states that when the satisfaction of students’ BPN is facilitated by promoting their feeling of autonomy, competence, connection and value, positive affects are experienced. Additionally, the inclusion of a direct inverse relationship between negative affect and academic performance emphasizes the importance of exploring not only beneficial determinants in a higher education context but also affective factors that may hinder learning when investigating this theoretical framework.

From a practical perspective, these results stress the importance of educational strategies aimed at reducing negative affect in learning environments. Additionally, they underscore the need to design university courses that foster enriching environments capable of promoting the satisfaction of students’ BPN. This idea is supported by numerous studies demonstrating that the effective implementation of active learning strategies positively impacts BPN satisfaction (Mentzer et al., 2023) and enhances academic performance in university settings (Freeman et al., 2014; Lo and Hew, 2019; Kozanitis and Nenciovici, 2023). Likewise, the way in which the teacher provides feedback on students’ performance is also relevant (Deci and Ryan, 1985). It has been found that feedback through descriptive comments on performance, rather than just grades, generates a higher level of perceived competence, motivation, and academic performance in students (Koenka et al., 2019). Similarly, it has been found that promoting a learning climate that encourages student autonomy is associated with greater satisfaction with their BPN (Levesque-Bristol et al., 2020; Zhou et al., 2023) and motivation (Orsini et al., 2018). Implementing these changes has the potential to improve not only academic outcomes but also the overall well-being of students (Ryan and Deci, 2017; Hayat et al., 2020).

In the educational field, there are approaches that emphasize the theoretical selection and transmission of content and skills, which can lead to a lessened attention to the affective and motivational aspects of students, despite their importance in the learning processes. Therefore, the findings of this study highlight the need for higher education institutions to integrate the promotion of BPN satisfaction into the design of their courses, since this will contribute to achieving their objectives of training well-rounded professionals. Additionally, these results provide empirical evidence that supports SDT in the Latin American university context, which helps to identify the most universal aspects of the theory and those that vary according to the cultural context.

5 Limitations and projections

This study has several limitations that must be acknowledged. First, its cross-sectional design restricts the ability to infer causal relationships between the variables. Additionally, the study relied on a small, non-randomized sample composed exclusively of students from a specific region in Chile, potentially limiting the generalizability of the findings to other cultural and educational contexts. Furthermore, the use of self-reported measures introduces the possibility of social desirability bias, which may influence the accuracy of the data. Moreover, data collection was conducted through Google Forms, a method that might have induced selection bias by favoring the inclusion of participants with greater access to digital resources. Finally, the absence of incentives for participants could have affected both the response rate and the quality of the data.

Future studies could adopt longitudinal designs to examine how interactions among BPN satisfaction, affect, and motivation evolve over time. Expanding the sample to include diverse regions and cultural contexts would also be valuable to explore cross-cultural variations. Moreover, future research could employ mixed methods, combining qualitative and quantitative approaches to capture a more comprehensive understanding of student experiences and contextual dynamics in higher education.

The use of a general BPN satisfaction instrument, rather than an instrument that assesses competence, relatedness, and autonomy individually, limited the conclusions obtained in the analysis. It is suggested that future studies use instruments that separately assess BPN satisfaction to determine whether the course context contributes equally to the satisfaction of all BPN satisfaction dimensions or whether certain factors affect these dimensions in a differentiated way. Finally, it would be advisable to conduct studies that more precisely discriminate between different types of motivation, using instruments tailored to the course context.

6 Conclusion

The findings of this study reveal that in courses perceived as most motivating, students experience higher positive affect, lower negative affect, higher BPN satisfaction, and better academic performance. Furthermore, BPN satisfaction was found to both directly and indirectly predict self-reported motivation, mediated by both types of affects, while negative affect and motivation directly predict academic performance. These results contribute to a better understanding of how BPN satisfaction influences the academic performance of university students. They also reinforce the usefulness of SDT in explaining motivational and affective phenomena in higher education, underlining that BPN satisfaction not only improves the academic experience, but also highlights the importance of educational environments incorporating these principles.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Comité Ético Científico de la Universidad de La Serena. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MG-A: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. PD: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. BS-F: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AR-P: Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MA: Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. DÁ-T: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. RC: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was conducted with funding from the DIDULS Regular PR233311 project at Universidad de La Serena.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Allen, M., Iliescu, D., and Greiff, S. (2022). Single item measures in psychological science. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 38, 1–5. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000699

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Qaisy, L. (2011). The relation of depression and anxiety in academic achievement among group of university students. Int. J. Psychol. Counsel. 3, 96–100. doi: 10.5897/IJPC.9000056

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alshareef, N., Fletcher, I., and Giga, S. (2024). The role of emotions in academic performance of undergraduate medical students: a narrative review. BMC Med. Educ. 24:907. doi: 10.1186/s12909-024-05894-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aydın, G., and Michou, A. (2020). Self-determined motivation and academic buoyancy as predictors of achievement in normative settings. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 90, 964–980. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12338

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Basileo, L., Otto, B., Lyons, M., Vannini, N., and Toth, M. (2024). The role of self-efficacy, motivation, and perceived support of students' basic psychological needs in academic achievement. Front. Educ. 9:1385442. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2024.1385442

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, C., Shaw, L., and Oleson, K. (1992). “Differentiating affect, mood, and emotion: toward functionally based conceptual distinctions” in Emotion. ed. M. Clark. 1st ed (Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications, Inc), 294–326.

Google Scholar

Bestelmeyer, P., Kotz, S., and Belin, P. (2017). Effects of emotional valence and arousal on the voice perception network. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1351–1358. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx059

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brahm, T., Jenert, T., and Wagner, D. (2017). The crucial first year: a longitudinal study of students’ motivational development at a Swiss business school. High. Educ. 73, 459–478. doi: 10.1007/s10734-016-0095-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cain, J., Medina, M., Romanelli, F., and Persky, A. (2022). Deficiencies of traditional grading systems and recommendations for the future. Am. J. Pharm. Educ. 86:8850. doi: 10.5688/ajpe8850

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmona-Halty, M., Schaufeli, W., Llorens, S., and Salanova, M. (2019). Satisfaction of basic psychological needs leads to better academic performance via increased psychological capital: a three-wave longitudinal study among high school students. Front. Psychol. 10:2113. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamberlin, K., Yasué, M., and Chiang, I. (2018). The impact of grades on student motivation. Act. Learn. High. Educ. 24, 109–124. doi: 10.1177/1469787418819728

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E., and Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. 1st ed. New York, NY: Springer Science & Business Media.

Google Scholar

Del Valle, M., Valenzuela, J., Muñoz, C., Miranda-Ossandón, J., Vergaño-Salazar, J., and Precht, A. (2025). Prácticas docentes universitarias y motivación orientada al aprendizaje: efecto mediador de la satisfacción de las necesidades psicológicas básicas. Educación XX1 28, 235–256. doi: 10.5944/educxx1.38203

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz-García, A., González-Robles, A., Mor, S., Mira, A., Quero, S., García-Palacios, A., et al. (2020). Positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS): psychometric properties of the online Spanish version in a clinical sample with emotional disorders. BMC Psychiatry 20:56. doi: 10.1186/s12888-020-2472-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

DiStefano, C., Liu, J., Jiang, N., and Shi, D. (2017). Examination of the weighted root mean square residual: evidence for trustworthiness? Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 25, 453–466. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2017.1390394

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dörnyei, Z., and Ottó, I. (1998). “Motivation in action: a process model of L2 motivation” in Working papers in applied linguistics, vol. 4, 43–69 (London: Thames Valley University).

Google Scholar

Dufey, M., and Fernández, A. (2012). Validez y confiabilidad del positive affect and negative affect schedule (PANAS) en estudiantes universitarios chilenos. Revista Iberoamericana de Diagnóstico Evaluación-e Avaliação Psicológica 2, 157–173.

Google Scholar

Dweck, C. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. Am. Psychol. 41, 1040–1048. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.41.10.1040

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliot, A., and Hulleman, C. (2017). “Achievement goals” in Handbook of competence and motivation: Theory and application. eds. A. J. Elliot, C. Dweck, and D. Yeager. 2nd ed (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 43–60.

Google Scholar

Fierro-Suero, S., Almagro, B., Becker, E., and Sáenz-López, P. (2022). Basic psychological needs, class-related emotions and satisfaction with life in Spanish teachers. Int. J. Educ. Psychol. 11, 153–181. doi: 10.17583/ijep.9106

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Florescu, M., Bochiș, L., Barth, K., and Indrieș, M. (2023). Can dysfunctional negative emotions explain Students' level of academic motivation in online learning at university? Revista Romaneasca Educatie Multidimensionala 15, 55–72. doi: 10.18662/rrem/15.2/720

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, S., Eddy, S., McDonough, M., Smith, M., Okoroafor, N., Jordt, H., et al. (2014). Active learning increases student performance in science, engineering, and mathematics. Psychol. Cogniti. Sci. 111, 8410–8415. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1319030111

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

González-Arias, M. (2023). Evolución del concepto de emoción en el contexto científico: Desde la biología y la cultura, hasta el construccionismo psicológico. Logos: Revista de Lingüística, Literatura y Filosofía.

Google Scholar

Grzybowski, S., Wyczesany, M., Cichecka, H., and Tokarska, A. (2021). The words of affectivity. Affect, category, and social evaluation norms for 400 polish adjectives. Front. Psychol. 12:683012. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.683012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gumasing, M., and Castro, F. (2023). Determining ergonomic appraisal factors affecting the learning motivation and academic performance of students during online classes. Sustain. For. 15:1970. doi: 10.3390/su15031970

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayat, A., Shateri, K., Amini, M., and Shokrpour, N. (2020). Relationships between academic self-efficacy, learning-related emotions, and metacognitive learning strategies with academic performance in medical students: a structural equation model. BMC Med. Educ. 20:76. doi: 10.1186/s12909-020-01995-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurtado-Bermúdez, S., and Romero-Abrio, A. (2023). The effects of combining virtual laboratory and advanced technology research laboratory on university students’ conceptual understanding of electron microscopy. Interact. Learn. Environ. 31, 1126–1141. doi: 10.1080/10494820.2020.1821716

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Karimi, S., and Sotoodeh, B. (2019). The mediating role of intrinsic motivation in the relationship between basic psychological needs satisfaction and academic engagement in agriculture students. Teach. High. Educ. 25, 959–975. doi: 10.1080/13562517.2019.1623775

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenka, A., Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Moshontz, H., Atkinson, K., Sanchez, C., and Cooper, H. (2019). A meta-analysis on the impact of grades and comments on academic motivation and achievement: a case for written feedback. Educ. Psychol. 41, 922–947. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2019.1659939

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kozanitis, A., and Nenciovici, L. (2023). Effect of active learning versus traditional lecturing on the learning achievement of college students in humanities and social sciences: a meta-analysis. High. Educ. 86, 1377–1394. doi: 10.1007/s10734-022-00977-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kusurkar, R., Ten Cate, T., Vos, C., Westers, P., and Croiset, G. (2013). How motivation affects academic performance: a structural equation modelling analysis. Adv. Health Sci. Educ. 18, 57–69. doi: 10.1007/s10459-012-9354-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leow, S., Leow, K., and Ean, C. (2023). Satisfaction of basic psychological needs and eudaimonic well-being among first-year university students. Cogent Soc. Sci. 9:2275441. doi: 10.1080/23311886.2023.2275441

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levesque-Bristol, C., Richards, K., Zissimopoulos, A., Wang, C., and Yu, S. (2020). An evaluation of the integrative model for learning and motivation in the college classroom. Curr. Psychol. 41, 1447–1459. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-00671-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, N., Yang, Y., Zhao, X., and Li, Y. (2023). The relationship between achievement motivation and college students’ general self-efficacy: a moderated mediation model. Front. Psychol. 13:1031912. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1031912

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Ma, S., and Chen, Y. (2024). The impacts of learning motivation, emotional engagement and psychological capital on academic performance in a blended learning university course. Front. Psychol. 15:1357936. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1357936

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lloret-Segura, S., Ferreres-Traver, A., Hernández-Baeza, A., and Tomás-Marco, I. (2014). El análisis factorial exploratorio de los ítems: una guía práctica, revisada y actualizada. Anal. Psicol. 30, 1151–1169. doi: 10.6018/analesps.30.3.199361

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lo, C., and Hew, K. (2019). The impact of flipped classrooms on student achievement in engineering education: a meta-analysis of 10 years of research. J. Eng. Educ. 108, 523–546. doi: 10.1002/jee.20293

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lombas, A., and Esteban, M. (2018). The confounding role of basic needs satisfaction between self-determined motivation and well-being. J. Happiness Stud. 19, 1305–1327. doi: 10.1007/s10902-017-9874-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Méndez-Aguado, C., Aguilar-Parra, J., Álvarez, J., Trigueros, R., and Fernández-Archilla, J. (2020). The influence of emotions, motivation and habits in the academic performance of primary education students in French as a foreign language. Sustain. 12:2531. doi: 10.3390/su12062531

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mentzer, N., Krishna, B., Kotangale, A., and Mohandas, L. (2023). HyFlex environment: addressing students’ basic psychological needs. Learn. Environ. Res. 26, 271–289. doi: 10.1007/s10984-022-09431-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miranda-Ossandón, J., Precht, A., Lobos, C., Valenzuela, J., Muñoz, C., and Del Valle, M. (2023). Enablers and barriers to the construction of motives for learning at the university: the student’s perspective. Int. J. Eval. Res. Educ. 12, 1903–1913. doi: 10.11591/ijere.v12i4.25254

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirawdali, S., Morrissey, H., and Ball, P. (2018). Academic anxiety and its effects on academic performance. Int. J. Curr. Res. 10, 70017–70026.

Google Scholar

Muñoz, R., and Correa, R. (2023). Relación entre motivación y rendimiento académico de estudiantes en el idioma inglés. Logos 33, 490–509. doi: 10.15443/RL3326

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthen, L., and Muthen, B. (2024). MPlus (version 8.11) [computer software]. Muthen & Muthen. Available at: https://www.statmodel.com/ (Accessed January 27, 2025).

Google Scholar

Najimi, A., Sharifirad, G., Amini, M., and Meftagh, S. D. (2013). Academic failure and students’ viewpoint: the influence of individual, internal and external organizational factors. J. Educ. Health Promot. 2:22. doi: 10.4103/2277-9531.112698

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishimura, M., and Joshi, A. (2021). (2021): self-determination theory and the happiness and academic achievement of students at a Women’s university. Psychol. Stud. 66, 139–153. doi: 10.1007/s12646-021-00617-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oram, R., and Rogers, M. (2022). Academic procrastination in undergraduate students: understanding the role of basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration and academic motivation. Can. J. Educ. 45, 619–645. doi: 10.53967/cje-rce.v45i3.5293

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Orsini, C., Binnie, V., Wilson, S., and Villejas, M. (2018). Learning climate and feedback as predictors of dental students’ self-determinated motivation: the mediating role of basic psychological needs satisfaction. Eur. J. Dental Educ. 22, 228–236. doi: 10.1111/eje.12277

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., Frenzel, A., Goetz, T., and Perry, R. (2007). “The control-value theory of achievement emotions: An integrative approach to emotions in education” in Emotion in education. eds. P. Schutz and R. Pekrun. 1st ed (Burlington, MA: Elsevier Academic Press), 13–36.

Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., and Perry, R. (2002). Academic emotions in students' self-regulated learning and achievement: a program of qualitative and quantitative research. Educ. Psychol. 37, 91–105. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3702_4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, M. (2009). “Educational assessment” in Developmental-behavioral pediatrics: Expert consult-online and print. eds. W. Carey, A. Crocker, E. Elias, H. Feldman, and W. Coleman. 4th ed (Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier Health Sciences), 811–827.

Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Muñoz, A., Antino, M., Ruiz-Zorrilla, P., and Ortega, E. (2021). Positive emotions, engagement, and objective academic performance: a weekly diary study. Learn. Individ. Differ. 92:102087. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2021.102087

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, G., and Benita, M. (2023). “Integration versus minimization of emotional experiences: addressing adaptive emotion regulation” in The Oxford handbook of self-determination theory. ed. R. Ryan. 1st ed (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 200–214.

Google Scholar

Russell, J. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39, 1161–1178. doi: 10.1037/h0077714

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, J. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol. Rev. 110, 145–172. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R., and Deci, E. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 141–166. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R., and Deci, E. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. 1st ed. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Ryan, R., and Deci, E. (2020). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation from a self-determination theory perspective: definitions, theory, practices, and future directions. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 61:101860. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101860

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutte, N., and Malouff, J. (2021). Basic psychological need satisfaction, affect and mental health. Curr. Psychol. 40, 1228–1233. doi: 10.1007/s12144-018-0055-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutz, P., and Pekrun, R. (2007). Emotion in education. 1st ed. Burlington, MA: Elsevier Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Shang, Z., Lv, J., Jiang, T., Chen, X., and Luo, Y. (2024). Autonomous motivation sustains the happiness from prosocial behavior. Motiv. Sci. 10, 100–109. doi: 10.1037/mot0000319

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, Y., Lin, Y., and Yu, Z. (2024). The effects of need satisfaction on active academic procrastination and the mediating roles of academic buoyancy and self-regulated learning. Curr. Psychol. 43, 21096–21109. doi: 10.1007/s12144-024-05916-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiota, M., Sauter, D., and Desmet, P. (2021). What are ‘positive’ affect and emotion? Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 39, 142–146. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.03.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiger, J. (2007). Understanding the limitations of global fit assessment in structural equation modeling. Personal. Individ. Differ. 42, 893–898. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, M., Wang, D., and Guerrien, A. (2021). The contribution of basic psychological need satisfaction to psychological well-being via autonomous motivation among older adults: a cross-cultural study in China and France. Front. Psychol. 12:734461. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.734461

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

The Jamovi Project (2023). Jamovi (Versión 2.3.28) [Computer software]. Jamovi. Available at: https://www.jamovi.org (Accessed January 27, 2025).

Google Scholar

Urdan, T., and Kaplan, A. (2020). The origins, evolution, and future directions of achievement goal theory. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 61:101862. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101862

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaculíková, J. (2021). Coping strategies and academic motivation: the mediating effect of achievement emotions. Int. J. Psychol. Psychol. Ther. 21, 363–378.

Google Scholar

Vansteenkiste, M., Soenens, B., and Ryan, R. (2023). “Basic psychological needs theory: a conceptual and empirical review of key criteria” in The Oxord handbook of self-Determimation theory. ed. R. Ryan. 1st ed. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 84–123.

Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Jiang, G., Yao, Z., and Liu, L. (2024). The influence of teacher–student relationship on Chinese high school students' academic motivation for the ideological and political subject: the mediating role of academic emotions. Front. Psychol. 14:1329439. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1329439

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, H., Li, S., Zheng, J., and Guo, J. (2020). Medical students’ motivation and academic performance: the mediating roles of self-efficacy and learning engagement. Med. Educ. Online 25, 1–9. doi: 10.1080/10872981.2020.1742964

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, K., Zeng, Y., Tong, L., Hu, Y., Zhang, R., Li, Z., et al. (2023). Extremely negative emotion interferes with cognition: evidence from ERPs and time-varying brain network. J. Neurosci. Methods 396:109922. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2023.109922

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, S., Hiver, P., and Al-Hoorie, A. (2023). Dynamic engagement: a longitudinal dual-process, reciprocal-effects model of teacher motivational practice and L2 student engagement. Lang. Teach. Res. 1–29. doi: 10.1177/13621688231158789

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: self-determination theory, basic psychological needs, affect, higher education, students, motivation, academic performance

Citation: González-Arias M, Dibona P, Soto-Flores B, Rojas-Puelles A, Amato M, Álvarez-Trigo D and Castillo R (2025) Academic performance, self-reported motivation, and affect in higher education: the role of basic psychological need satisfaction. Front. Psychol. 16:1519454. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1519454

Received: 29 October 2024; Accepted: 30 January 2025;
Published: 14 February 2025.

Edited by:

Henri Tilga, University of Tartu, Estonia

Reviewed by:

Huajie Shen, Fujian University of Technology, China
Florin-Vasile Frumos, Alexandru Ioan Cuza University, Romania

Copyright © 2025 González-Arias, Dibona, Soto-Flores, Rojas-Puelles, Amato, Álvarez-Trigo and Castillo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mauricio González-Arias, bWdvbnphbGVAdXNlcmVuYS5jbA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.