Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 June 2025

Sec. Psychology of Aging

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1546443

Traumatic brain injury and anger proneness: results from the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study

Connor A. Law
Connor A. Law1*Holly ElserHolly Elser2Alexa E. WalterAlexa E. Walter1Thomas H. MosleyThomas H. Mosley3Keenan WalkerKeenan Walker4Rebecca F. GottesmanRebecca F. Gottesman5Andrea L. C. Schneider,Andrea L. C. Schneider1,6
  • 1Department of Neurology, Perelman School of Medicine at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, United States
  • 2Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, United States
  • 3The MIND Center, University of Mississippi Medical Center, Jackson, MS, United States
  • 4National Institute of Aging Intramural Research Program, Baltimore, MD, United States
  • 5National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Intramural Research Program, Bethesda, MD, United States
  • 6Department of Biostatistics, Epidemiology, and Informatics, Perelman School of Medicine at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, United States

Background/objective: Associations of traumatic brain injury (TBI) with subsequent increased anger proneness have been studied in younger populations, but less is known about potential bidirectional associations between TBI and anger proneness among older populations. This study aimed to investigate bidirectional associations between anger proneness and TBI among community-dwelling participants in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study.

Methods: TBI was defined by self-report and ICD-9/10 codes. Anger proneness was defined using the Spielberger Trait Anger Scale. We performed 3 analyses: cross-sectional associations of prior TBI with anger proneness (Visit 2, 1990–1992, N = 13,694), associations of interval TBI with change in anger proneness (Visit 2, 1990–1992 to Visit 4, 1996–1998, N = 9,022), and prospective associations of baseline anger proneness with incident TBI (Visit 2, 1990–1992 to 12/31/2020, N = 11,713). Adjusted Tobit, linear, and Cox-proportional hazards regression models estimated associations, respectively.

Results: Overall, participants were a mean age of 57 years at Visit 2, 55% were female, and 24% were Black. In cross-sectional analyses, prior TBI was associated with slightly higher anger proneness (β = 0.35, 95% CI = 0.17, 0.54). In change analyses, interval TBI was not significantly associated with change in anger proneness score over time (β = 0.16, 95% CI = −0.16, 0.48). In prospective analyses, increasing baseline anger proneness was not significantly associated with incident TBI (moderate anger proneness: HR = 1.05, 95% CI = 0.95, 1.15; high anger proneness: HR = 1.15, 95% CI = 0.97, 1.37).

Conclusion: In conclusion, this study did not find evidence for associations between TBI and anger proneness in this older population. Further research regarding relationships between anger proneness and TBI may not be warranted in older populations.

Introduction

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is associated with significant short- and long-term morbidity and mortality (Schneider et al., 2021; Schneider et al., 2018). In particular, TBI is associated with cognitive (i.e., memory and executive functioning impairments) (Haarbauer-Krupa et al., 2021; Howlett et al., 2022), psychiatric (i.e., depression and post-traumatic stress disorder) (Challakere Ramaswamy et al., 2023), and personality-related (i.e., impulsivity and affective instability) (Challakere Ramaswamy et al., 2023) sequelae.

There is also evidence that TBI can change an individual’s propensity for anger (Murphy et al., 2022). In the past half century, anger has been conceptualized using the state–trait model, which splits the concept into state anger–the transitory emotional continuum of being annoyed, irritated, or otherwise angered–and trait anger, which is defined by an individual’s more stable temperamental vulnerability or proneness to becoming angered (Richard et al., 2022). While anger has historically been conceptualized as a personality trait that remains relatively static over the lifecourse (Chida and Steptoe, 2009), more recent research suggests that there are effective interventions, such as cognitive behavioral therapy and training avoidance tendencies towards threatening situations, which can be used to decrease anger proneness (Veenstra et al., 2018). Anger proneness, or trait anger, has been associated with multiple negative health outcomes (Veenstra et al., 2018), including coronary heart disease (Chida and Steptoe, 2009), diabetes (Golden et al., 2006), and stroke (Williams et al., 2002). Behaviorally, high anger proneness is associated with increased risk-taking behaviors (Deffenbacher et al., 2003) and impulsivity (Richard et al., 2022), as well as an impairment in effortful control (Wilkowski and Robinson, 2010). Additionally, high anger proneness is associated with an increased frequency in motor vehicle crashes (Deffenbacher et al., 2003), intimate partner violence, and other types of assault (Stith et al., 2004) all of which are common mechanisms for TBI (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, 2023).

While prior literature has reported increases in anger proneness and other personality changes following TBI (Kim et al., 1999; Tateno et al., 2003; Baguley et al., 2006; Rao et al., 2009), less work has investigated anger proneness as a risk factor for TBI, particularly among middle-age and older individuals. Indeed, the highest incidence of TBI occurs among older individuals, with unintentional falls being the most common mechanism of injury in this age group, followed by motor vehicle crashes (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2022; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2021). Leveraging data from participants in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study collected both before and after the occurrence of TBIs, the objective of the present study is to examine the bidirectional associations between TBI and anger proneness among community dwelling middle-aged and older adults. We hypothesized that prior TBI would be associated with greater anger proneness at baseline, that interval TBI would be associated with a greater increase in anger proneness over time, and that greater anger proneness at baseline would be associated with increased risk of incident TBI.

Methods

Study population

The ARIC Study is a prospective cohort of community-dwelling individuals. Participants were recruited by probability sampling of the following three communities, resulting in individuals of mainly self-reported White racial identity from Washington County, Maryland and selected suburbs of Minneapolis, Minnesota, and of mainly self-reported White and Black racial identity from Forsyth County, North Carolina. In the fourth community (Jackson, Mississippi), only individuals of self-reported Black racial identity were recruited. Participants were enrolled in 1987–1989 when they were 45–64 years old and have participated in subsequent follow-up visits, as well as annual (through 2011) and semi-annual (starting in 2012) telephone interviews. Hospitalization surveillance in all ARIC Study communities began in 1987 and is ongoing. Hospital records from hospitalizations occurring outside of ARIC Study communities are obtained if reported by participants during telephone interviews. In addition, linked Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services (CMS) data were available for participants aged ≥65 years who were enrolled in fee-for-service part B from 1991 to 2018. All participants or their legally authorized representative provided written consent at each study visit and the ARIC Study was approved by institutional review boards at each participating institution.

ARIC Visit 2 (1990–1992) served as the baseline for all analyses as this was the first visit during which anger proneness was assessed (Figure 1). Of the 14,314 participants who attended Visit 2, 42 were excluded for self-identifying as Asian or American Indian racial identity, and 49 were excluded for self-identifying as Black racial identity within the Minnesota or Maryland communities due to small numbers and racial identity/center aliasing (Figure 2). Additionally, 159 were excluded for missing anger proneness data at Visit 2, and 370 were excluded for missing covariates included in statistical models. Among the 13,694 participants included in the cross-sectional analysis, 1,981 were excluded from the change and prospective and change analyses for having a prevalent TBI at Visit 2. Finally, of the 11,713 participants included in the prospective analysis, 2,523 participants were excluded from the change analysis for not attending Visit 4, with an additional 168 excluded for missing Visit 4 anger proneness data; this resulted in 9,022 participants in the change analysis population.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Study design. *Available for ARIC participants from January 1, 1987 through December 31, 2020. **Available for ARIC participants aged 65 years and older enrolled in Medicare fee-for-service Part B from January 1, 1991 through December 31, 2018. +Questions asked in a subgroup of ARIC participants selected for brain magnetic resonance imaging scans.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Study population definitions and inclusion/exclusion criteria.

TBI definition

TBI was defined based on data obtained from self-report questions asked during study visits and from diagnostic codes from the International Classification of Diseases, Ninth/Tenth Revisions (ICD-9/10) from ARIC Study hospitalization surveillance and linked CMS data. The self-report questions inquired about prior TBIs which required medical attention or were associated with loss of consciousness, number of prior TBIs, and year of prior injuries (Supplementary Table 1). Month and day of self-reported TBIs were imputed using the random point method (Vandormael et al., 2018). To identify diagnostic code defined head injuries, we used the Centers for Disease Control and Preventions (CDC) surveillance definition for TBI (Langlois et al., 2002; Hedegaard et al., 2016) (Supplementary Table 2).

In secondary analyses, we additionally considered the number of head injuries (no head injury; one head injury; two or more head injuries) and the severity of the head injury (among those injuries identified by ICD-9/10 codes; severity defined in accordance with the Department of Defense definition (Cené et al., 2012) as no head injury; mild head injury; moderate or severe/penetrating head injury).

Anger proneness

Anger proneness was defined using the Spielberger Trait Anger Scale (Spielberger et al., 1983), which was administered to participants at ARIC Visits 2 (1990–1992) and 4 (1996–1998). This 10-question questionnaire uses a 4-point Likert Scale (1: almost never, 2: sometimes, 3: often, 4: almost always) to assess anger-temperament and anger-reaction, resulting in overall anger proneness scores ranging from 10 (low) to 40 (high) (Supplementary Table 3). In accordance with prior research (Golden et al., 2006), we considered anger proneness as a continuous variable and as a categorical variable (low anger proneness: score 10–14, moderate anger proneness: score 15–21, and high anger proneness: score 22–40).

Other variables

The following covariates, which were assessed at ARIC Visit 2 (unless otherwise specified), were included in statistical models: age (derived from ARIC Visit 2 date and birth date), self-reported biological sex (female; male), self-reported racial identity/center (White Maryland; White Minnesota; White North Carolina; Black North Carolina; Black Mississippi), education (self-reported at ARIC Visit 1, less than high school; high school, GED, or vocational school; some college, college, graduate, or professional school), marital status (married; divorced or separated; never married; widowed), military veteran status (yes; no), diabetes (yes; no, defined as self-reported physician diagnosis, use of diabetes medications, fasting glucose ≥126 mg/dL, or non-fasting glucose ≥200 mg/dL), hypertension (yes; no, defined as self-reported physician diagnosis, use of hypertension medication, systolic blood pressure ≥140 mmHg, or diastolic blood pressure ≥90 mmHg), cigarette smoking (current; former; never), and alcohol consumption (current; former; never).

In sensitivity analyses, we also considered depression, defined by high vital exhaustion on the Maastricht Vital Exhaustion Questionnaire, as a potential effect modifier. The Maastricht Vital Exhaustion Questionnaire consists of 21 questions scored on a 0–2 scale (0: no, 1: do not know, 2: yes). The Maastricht Vital Exhaustion Questionnaire has previously been shown to have a 0.62 correlation with the Beck Depression Inventory (Garg et al., 2021). In accordance with prior studies (Cené et al., 2012), we defined depression (high vital exhaustion) as a score ≥14.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted using R Version 4.2.2 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria) using two-tailed tests with statistical significance defined a priori as p < 0.05. Participant characteristics are reported by TBI and anger proneness status as means and standard deviations for continuous variables and as number and proportions for categorical variables.

We performed three analyses to evaluate bidirectional associations of TBI and anger proneness (Figure 1). First, we conducted a cross-sectional association of prior TBI with anger proneness based on data from Visit 2 (1990–1992). For these analyses, we used adjusted Tobit regression models to evaluate the association between prior TBI and anger proneness to reduce the impact of floor effects observed on the Spielberger Trait Anger Scale (Supplementary Figure 1). Second, we examined the association of interval TBI with change in anger proneness scores between Visit 2 and Visit 4. Adjusted linear regression models were used to evaluate the association between interval TBI and change in anger proneness score over time (defined as Visit 4 score minus Visit 2 score) given the normally distributed change in anger proneness data (Supplementary Figure 2). As a sensitivity analysis, we modeled change in anger proneness using a linear mixed effects model that included a random intercept and a random slope and employed an unstructured covariance matrix. Finally, we examined the association of baseline anger proneness with incident TBI measured from Visit 2 onward. These prospective analyses utilized adjusted Cox proportional hazard regression models to examine the relationship between baseline anger proneness and incident TBI. Visual inspection of the Schoenfeld residuals and complementary log–log plots confirmed that the proportional hazards assumption was met (Supplementary Figure 3). Time since ARIC Visit 2 was used as the time scale with follow-up extending to the date of first TBI, study withdrawal/loss to follow-up, death, or administrative censoring on 12/31/2020. As secondary analyses, we performed adjusted Fine and Gray proportional hazards models to account for the competing risk of death (Fine and Gray, 1999). All statistical models were adjusted for age, self-reported biological sex, self-reported racial identity/center, education, military veteran status, marital status, diabetes, hypertension, cigarette smoking, and alcohol consumption, except for secondary analyses investigating head injury severity which are presented unadjusted due to small numbers.

In secondary analyses, given prior findings on differences in TBI and anger proneness by age (Downing et al., 2024), biological sex (Gupte et al., 2019), racial identity (Maldonado et al., 2023), and depression status (Hudak et al., 2012), all analyses had these subgroups formally evaluated for multiplicative interaction. If evidence for interaction by self-reported racial identify was present, we performed a sensitivity analysis among participants from the North Carolina center to distinguish racial identity from center. Additionally, sensitivity analyses were conducted stratified by TBI definition source (self-reported TBI vs. ICD-code defined TBI).

Results

Cross-sectional associations between prior TBI and anger proneness

Of the 13,694 participants included in the cross-sectional analyses, 1,978 participants experienced at least one prior TBI. Overall, participants were a mean age of 57.0 years, 55.1% self-reported as biologically female, and 24.2% self-reported Black racial identity (Table 1). Compared to individuals without TBI, individuals with prior TBI were less likely to self-report as biologically female (41.9 vs. 57.3%) and self-report their racial identify as Black (14.8 vs. 25.7%) and were more likely to have greater than high school education (42.6 vs. 36.0%), be military veterans (35.0 vs. 21.1%), and be current consumers of alcohol (62.3 vs. 55.8%).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Baseline participant characteristics stratified by prevalent and incident TBI status (cross-sectional analysis and change analysis populations), ARIC visit 2 (1990–1992).

When using Tobit models to account for floor effects (Supplementary Figure 1), individuals with prior TBI had anger proneness scores that were 0.35 points higher (95% CI = 0.17, 0.54) than individuals without prior TBI in fully adjusted models (Table 2). Prior TBI was associated with higher anger proneness score among individuals of self-reported White race (β = 0.40; 95% CI = 0.21, 0.60) but not among individuals of self-reported Black race (β = 0.03; 95% CI = −0.48, 0.55), p-interaction = 0.03. In order to distinguish self-reported racial identify from center, we performed a sensitivity analysis among North Carolina center participants and found similar results to our main analysis (White: β = 0.57; 95% CI = 0.22, 0.92 and Black: β = 0.03; 95% CI = 0.03; −0.48, 0.55). There was no evidence of interaction by age, self-reported biological sex, or depression status. In stratified sensitivity analyses, associations of self-reported TBI and ICD-code defined TBIs with anger proneness were consistent with the primary analysis (Supplementary Table 4).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Associations of prevalent TBI with anger proneness score (cross-sectional ARIC visit 2, 1990–1992) and of incident TBI with change in anger trait (ARIC visit 2, 1990–1992 to ARIC visit 4, 1996–1998).

In secondary analyses, similar associations were seen by TBI frequency (1 TBI, n = 1,576: β = 0.35, 95% CI = 0.14, 0.55 and 2 + TBIs n = 402: β = 0.37, 95% CI = −0.01, 0.76). There were 63 individuals with mild TBI and 5 with moderate or severe/penetrating TBI among the subset of TBIs identified using ICD-9/10 codes, which resulted in limited precision in associations of TBI severity with anger proneness (mild TBI: unadjusted β = −0.27, 95% CI = −1.25, 0.70; moderate or severe/penetrating TBI: unadjusted β = −1.83, 95% CI = −5.31, 1.65).

Association between TBI and change in anger proneness over time

Among the 9,022 participants included in the change analysis, 390 individuals experienced at least one interval TBI occurring between ARIC Visit 2 (1990–1992) and Visit 4 (1996–1998). Overall, participants were a mean age of 56.9 years at baseline, 58.1% self-reported as biologically female, and 22.6% self-reported Black racial identity. Individuals with vs. without interval TBI were more likely to self-report as biologically female (64.4 vs. 57.8%) but were otherwise similar (Table 1).

Overall, interval TBI was not significantly associated with change in anger proneness score over time (β = 0.16, 95% CI = −0.16, 0.48) (Table 2). There was no evidence of interaction by age, self-reported biological sex, self-reported racial identity, or depression. Similarly, we did not see any evidence for associations of interval TBI number with change in anger proneness score (1 TBI, n = 144: β = −0.07, 95% CI = −0.59, 0.44; 2 + TBIs, n = 246: β = 0.30, 95% CI = −0.10, 0.70). By interval TBI severity (among the interval TBIs identified by ICD-9/10 codes), interval mild TBI was not significantly associated with change in anger proneness score (n = 126, unadjusted β = 0.30, 95% CI = −0.26, 0.85), while moderate or severe/penetrating TBI was associated with lower anger proneness score over time (n = 27, unadjusted β = −1.82, 95% CI = −3.01, −0.63). In sensitivity analyses, associations of interval self-reported TBI and of interval ICD-code defined TBI with change in anger proneness over time were consistent with the main change analyses (Supplementary Table 4). Similarly, in sensitivity analyses using a linear mixed effects model, results were consistent with our primary analysis (β = 0.14, 95% CI = −0.18, 0.45).

Prospective association between anger proneness and incident TBI

A total of 1,961 of the 11,713 participants in the prospective analysis had an incident TBI over a median of 14.7 years (25th percentile-75th percentile = 6.9–20.5) of follow-up. Overall, the mean age of included individuals was 57.1 years at baseline, 57.3% self-reported as biologically female, and 25.7% self-reported Black racial identity (Table 3). Compared to individuals with low anger proneness, individuals with high anger proneness were more likely to have less than high school education (32.1 vs. 23.6%), be current smokers (30.2 vs. 18.1%), consumers of alcohol (61.1 vs. 50.4%), and have depression (56.4 vs. 19.3%).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Baseline characteristics of participants without prevalent TBI stratified by anger trait level (prospective analysis population), ARIC visit 2 (1990–1992).

The cumulative TBI-free survival time was comparable across baseline anger proneness levels (Figure 3). In adjusted Cox proportional hazards models increasing baseline anger proneness was not significantly associated with increased risk of incident TBI (moderate anger proneness: HR = 1.05, 95% CI = 0.95, 1.15; high anger proneness: HR = 1.15, 95% CI = 0.97, 1.37; Table 4). In models accounting for the competing risk of death, point estimates were attenuated. There was no evidence of interaction by age, self-reported biological sex, self-reported racial identify, or depression. In sensitivity analyses, associations of baseline anger proneness with incident TBI were comparable to the main analysis regardless of TBI ascertainment source (self-reported vs. ICD-code defined) (Supplementary Table 5).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Probability of TBI-free survival.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Adjusted hazard ratios (HRs) for the prospective associations of anger proneness with incident TBI, ARIC study visit 2, 1990–1992 through December 31, 2020.

Discussion

Cross-sectionally we found minimal evidence that TBI was associated with increased anger proneness overall and among select subgroups with estimated differences between individuals with vs. without TBI falling below the minimum scoring increment. Changes in anger proneness were also small in magnitude. Prospective analyses suggested that anger proneness is not a significant risk factor for TBI. Taken together, the results of this study suggest that anger proneness is similar between individuals with and without TBI and is not significantly associated with incident TBI among community-dwelling middle-aged and older adults.

Our results contrast with some prior literature, which reported increases in anger proneness after TBI in select younger populations (mean age <40 years; largely clinic populations) (Arciniegas and Wortzel, 2014; Barry and Ettenhofer, 2012; Dikmen et al., 1986). Some have attributed findings of an increase in anger proneness to psychiatric sequalae and maladaptive coping mechanisms, such as substance use (Arciniegas and Wortzel, 2014; Castaño Monsalve et al., 2012), following TBI, rather than the TBIs themselves. However, some other studies have reported anger proneness as stable or decreasing over time post-injury (Arciniegas and Wortzel, 2014). Given the self-reported nature of the Spielberger Trait Anger Scale, one explanation of this is the presence of alexithymia, a condition defined by difficulty recognizing and describing a person’s own emotional state. This condition is significantly more common following TBI (Fynn et al., 2021), and it could potentially lead individuals to chronically underreport anger and/or fail to connect descriptions of anger with their own experiences. Another possible explanation is social desirability biases, whereby some individuals may be more likely to underreport anger proneness, which is generally perceived to be a negative trait.

There is a paucity of research focused on anger as a risk-factor for incident TBIs, and studies that have been performed were in select populations. One study examining anger and TBI bidirectionally found individuals with high aggression-hostility had a greater rate of incident TBIs (Matei et al., 2022). However, this study cohort consisted of 4,881 young Swiss males (mean age 25 years) and required TBI to be associated with loss of consciousness (Matei et al., 2022). Another smaller study examined TBIs among 100 men and women with diagnosed personality disorders (mean age 39 years) and found that individuals with antisocial personality disorder, which is commonly associated with aggression as a symptom, featured a notably higher rate of TBI (Hibbard et al., 2000). The differences between our study and these prior studies are likely driven by the older age of our cohort and the inclusion of individuals of both sexes, as well as a broader definition of TBI (incorporating injuries not associated with loss of consciousness).

We attempted to address the potential role of mood disorders by investigating for potential effect modification by depression status in our statistical models, although we did not find evidence for interaction. Though TBIs are heterogeneous, previous research has found that lesions in the temporal (Hibbard et al., 2000; Grafman et al., 1996) and frontal lobes (Tateno et al., 2003; Hibbard et al., 2000; Grafman et al., 1996) following TBI are associated with increased anger following TBI. Indeed, this may be in part due to mood disorders, such as depression, that may develop after TBI; selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) were found to reduce post-TBI aggression (Tateno et al., 2003). Other research has suggested that repeated TBIs are linked with changes in the limbic system, which is understood to play a key role in emotion regulation (Lepage et al., 2019).

Our study is not without limitations. First, the anger proneness data was collected at only two time points and relied on self-reported data, which may be differential with respect to TBI status. Further, the sparsity of time points meant we were unable to evaluate the possibility of dynamic changes in anger proneness over time. In addition, TBI was defined by self-report and ICD codes, however prior validation studies have shown ICD code definitions of TBI to have 55–72% sensitivity and 80–85% specificity (Carlson et al., 2013; Warwick et al., 2020). Further, while the self-reported questions about TBI did change over time, they focused on TBIs requiring medical care and/or associated with loss of consciousness and therefore may not capture milder injuries that did not require medical care. Indeed, prior literature suggests that milder TBI events may be misclassified/underdiagnosed (Cota et al., 2019; Powell et al., 2008), which may have attenuated observed associations towards the null. We also did not have detailed information on clinical characteristics of the injury (e.g., post-traumatic amnesia, altered mental status), injury mechanism, or acute head imaging findings. Further, our analyses incorporating injury severity were underpowered, limiting conclusions that can be drawn. Although the ARIC Study populations are representative of the communities the participants are recruited from, our results may not generalize to younger, non-Black/non-White populations, or to other geographic regions. Our change analyses included the subset of participants with data at two visits, and these results may be impacted by selection/survivorship bias. Additionally, vital exhaustion is an imperfect proxy measure of depression; future studies with more direct measures of depression are warranted.

Conclusion

Our results did not find strong evidence for an association between TBI and anger proneness in this community-based population of middle aged and older adults. Further research regarding relationships between anger proneness and TBI may not be warranted in older populations.

Data availability statement

The ARIC study data is publicly available. This data can be found at: NHLBI Bio LINCC (https://biolincc.nhlbi.nih.gov/home/).

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the University of Pennsylvania Institutional Review Board approved this study as exempt (#844030). All participants or their legally authorized representative provided written consent at each study visit. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

CL: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. HE: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. AW: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. TM: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. KW: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. RG: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. AS: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study is carried out as a collaborative study supported by National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute contracts (75N92022D00001, 75N92022D00002, 75N92022D00003, 75N92022D00004, 75N92022D00005). The ARIC Neurocognitive Study is supported by U01HL096812, U01HL096814, U01HL096899, U01HL096902, and U01HL096917 from the NIH (NHLBI, NINDS, NIA and NIDCD). AS was supported by Department of Defense grants W81XWH-21-1-0590 and HT9425-23-1-0981 and by NINDS grant K23NS123340, RG was supported by the NINDS Intramural Research Program, KW was supported by the NIA Intramural Research Program.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the staff and participants of the ARIC study for their important contributions.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1546443/full#supplementary-material

References

Arciniegas, D. B., and Wortzel, H. S. (2014). Emotional and behavioral dyscontrol after traumatic brain injury. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 37, 31–53. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2013.12.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baguley, I. J., Cooper, J., and Felmingham, K. (2006). Aggressive behavior following traumatic brain injury: how common is common? J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 21:1. doi: 10.1097/00001199-200601000-00005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barry, D. M., and Ettenhofer, M. L. (2012). A comparison of long-term postconcussive symptoms between university students with and without a history of mild traumatic brain injury or orthopedic injury. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 18, 451–460. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711001895

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, K. F., Barnes, J. E., Hagel, E. M., Taylor, B. C., Cifu, D. X., and Sayer, N. A. (2013). Sensitivity and specificity of traumatic brain injury diagnosis codes in United States Department of Veterans Affairs administrative data. Brain Inj. 27, 640–650. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2013.771795

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Castaño Monsalve, B., Bernabeu Guitart, M., López, R., Vilasar, A. B., and Quemada, J. I. (2012). Psychopathological evaluation of traumatic brain injury patients with the neuropsychiatric inventory. Rev. Psiquiatr. Salud Ment. 5, 160–166. doi: 10.1016/j.rpsmen.2012.02.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cené, C. W., Loehr, L., Lin, F.-C., Hammond, W. P., Foraker, R. E., Rose, K., et al. (2012). Social isolation, vital exhaustion, and incident heart failure: findings from the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Eur. J. Heart Fail. 14, 748–753. doi: 10.1093/eurjhf/hfs064

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2021). Surveillance report of traumatic brain injury-related hospitalizations and deaths by age group, sex, and mechanism of injury-United States, 2016 and 2017. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

Google Scholar

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2022). Surveillance report of traumatic brain injury-related deaths by age group, sex, and mechanism of injury-United States, 2018 and 2019. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

Google Scholar

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. (2023). Get the facts about TBI | concussion | traumatic brain injury |CDC injury center. Available online at: https://www.cdc.gov/traumaticbraininjury/get_the_facts.html (accessed May 24, 2023).

Google Scholar

Challakere Ramaswamy, V. M., Butler, T., Ton, B., Wilhelm, K., Mitchell, P. B., Knight, L., et al. (2023). Self-reported traumatic brain injury in a sample of impulsive violent offenders: neuropsychiatric correlates and possible “dose effects”. Front. Psychol. 14:d. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1243655

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chida, Y., and Steptoe, A. (2009). The association of anger and hostility with future coronary heart disease: a meta-analytic review of prospective evidence. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 53, 936–946. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2008.11.044

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cota, M. R., Moses, A. D., Jikaria, N. R., Bittner, K. C., Diaz-Arrastia, R. R., Latour, L. L., et al. (2019). Discordance between documented criteria and documented diagnosis of traumatic brain injury in the emergency department. J. Neurotrauma 36, 1335–1342. doi: 10.1089/neu.2018.5772

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deffenbacher, J. L., Filetti, L. B., Richards, T. L., Lynch, R. S., and Oetting, E. R. (2003). Characteristics of two groups of angry drivers. J. Couns. Psychol. 50, 123–132. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.50.2.123

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dikmen, S., McLean, A., and Temkin, N. (1986). Neuropsychological and psychosocial consequences of minor head injury. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 49:1227. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.49.11.1227

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Downing, M. G., Carty, M., Olver, J., Ponsford, M., Acher, R., Mckenzie, D., et al. (2024). The impact of age on outcome 2 years after traumatic brain injury: case control study. Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 67:101834. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2024.101834

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fine, J. P., and Gray, R. J. (1999). A proportional hazards model for the subdistribution of a competing risk. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 94, 496–509. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1999.10474144

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fynn, D. M., Gignac, G. E., Becerra, R., Pestell, C. F., and Weinborn, M. (2021). The prevalence and characteristics of alexithymia in adults following brain injury: a Meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 31, 722–738. doi: 10.1007/s11065-021-09484-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Garg, P. K., Claxton, J. S., Soliman, E. Z., Chen, L. Y., Lewis, T. T., Mosley, T., et al. (2021). Associations of anger, vital exhaustion, anti-depressant use, and poor social ties with incident atrial fibrillation: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Eur. J. Prev. Cardiol. 28, 633–640. doi: 10.1177/2047487319897163

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Golden, S. H., Williams, J. E., Ford, D. E., Yeh, H. C., Sanford, C. P., Nieto, F. J., et al. (2006). Anger temperament is modestly associated with the risk of type 2 diabetes mellitus: the atheroslcerosis risk in communities study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 325–332. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.08.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grafman, J., Schwab, K., Warden, D., Pridgen, A., Brown, H. R., and Salazar, A. M. (1996). Frontal lobe injuries, violence, and aggression. Neurology 46, 1231–1238. doi: 10.1212/WNL.46.5.1231

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gupte, R., Brooks, W., Vukas, R., Pierce, J., and Harris, J. (2019). Sex differences in traumatic brain injury: what we know and what we should know. J. Neurotrauma 36, 3063–3091. doi: 10.1089/neu.2018.6171

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haarbauer-Krupa, J., Pugh, M. J., Prager, E. M., Harmon, N., Wolfe, J., and Yaffe, K. (2021). Epidemiology of chronic effects of traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma 38, 3235–3247. doi: 10.1089/neu.2021.0062

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedegaard, H., Johnson, R. L., Warner, M., Chen, L.-H., and Annest, J. L. (2016). Proposed framework for presenting injury data using the international classification of diseases, tenth revision, clinical modification (ICD-10-CM) diagnosis codes. Natl. Health Stat. Rep. 89, 1–20.

Google Scholar

Hibbard, M. R., Bogdany, J., Uysal, S., Kepler, K., Silver, J. M., Gordon, W. A., et al. (2000). Axis II psychopathology in individuals with traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 14, 45–61. doi: 10.1080/0269905001209161

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Howlett, J. R., Nelson, L. D., and Stein, M. B. (2022). Mental health consequences of traumatic brain injury. Biol. Psychiatry 91, 413–420. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.09.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hudak, A. M., Hynan, L. S., Harper, C. R., and Diaz-Arrastia, R. (2012). Association of depressive symptoms with functional outcome after traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 27, 87–98. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0b013e3182114efd

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S. H., Manes, F., Kosier, T., Baruah, S., and Robinson, R. G. (1999). Irritability following traumatic brain injury. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 187, 327–335. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199906000-00001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Langlois, J. A., Kegler, S. R., Butler, J. A., Gotsch, K. E., Johnson, R. L., et al. (2002). Traumatic brain injury-related hospital discharges. Results from a 14-state surveillance system, 1997. MMWR Surveill. Summ. 52, 1–20.

Google Scholar

Lepage, C., Muehlmann, M., Tripodis, Y., Hufschmidt, J., Stamm, J., Green, K., et al. (2019). Limbic system structure volumes and associated neurocognitive functioning in former NFL players. Brain Imaging Behav. 13, 725–734. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-9895-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maldonado, J., Huang, J. H., Childs, E. W., and Tharakan, B. (2023). Racial/ethnic differences in traumatic brain injury: pathophysiology, outcomes, and future directions. J. Neurotrauma 40, 502–513. doi: 10.1089/neu.2021.0455

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matei, V. P., Rosca, A. E., Pavel, A. N., Paun, R. M., Gmel, G., Daeppen, J. B., et al. (2022). Risk factors and consequences of traumatic brain injury in a Swiss male population cohort. BMJ Open 12:e055986. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2021-055986

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, J. M., Bennett, J. M., de la Piedad Garcia, X., and Willis, M. L. (2022). Emotion recognition and traumatic brain injury: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 32, 520–536. doi: 10.1007/s11065-021-09510-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Powell, J. M., Ferraro, J. V., Dikmen, S. S., Temkin, N. R., and Bell, K. R. (2008). Accuracy of mild traumatic brain injury diagnosis. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 89, 1550–1555. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.12.035

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rao, V., Rosenberg, P., Bertrand, M., Salehinia, S., Spiro, J., Vaishnavi, S., et al. (2009). Aggression after traumatic brain injury: prevalence and correlates. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 21, 420–429. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2009.21.4.420

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richard, Y., Tazi, N., Frydecka, D., Hamid, M. S., and Moustafa, A. A. (2022). A systematic review of neural, cognitive, and clinical studies of anger and aggression. Curr. Psychol. 42:3143. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-03143-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, A. L. C., Wang, D., Gottesman, R. F., and Selvin, E. (2021). Prevalence of disability associated with head injury with loss of consciousness in adults in the United States: a population-based study. Neurology 97, e124–e135. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000012148

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, A. L. C., Wang, D., Ling, G., Gottesman, R. F., and Selvin, E. (2018). Prevalence of self-reported head injury in the United States. N. Engl. J. Med. 379, 1176–1178. doi: 10.1056/NEJMc1808550

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Butcher, J. N., and Russel, S. (1983). The state-trait anger scale. Advances in personality assessment. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 161–189.

Google Scholar

Stith, S. M., Smith, D. B., Penn, C. E., Ward, D. B., and Tritt, D. (2004). Intimate partner physical abuse perpetration and victimization risk factors: a meta-analytic review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 10, 65–98. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2003.09.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tateno, A., Jorge, R. E., and Robinson, R. G. (2003). Clinical correlates of aggressive behavior after traumatic brain injury. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 15, 155–160. doi: 10.1176/jnp.15.2.155

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandormael, A., Dobra, A., Bärnighausen, T., de Oliveira, T., and Tanser, F. (2018). Incidence rate estimation, periodic testing and the limitations of the mid-point imputation approach. Int. J. Epidemiol. 47, 236–245. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyx134

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Veenstra, L., Bushman, B. J., and Koole, S. L. (2018). The facts on the furious: a brief review of the psychology of trait anger. Aggress. Violent Behav. 19, 98–103. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Warwick, J., Slavova, S., Bush, J., and Costich, J. (2020). Validation of ICD-10-CM surveillance codes for traumatic brain injury inpatient hospitalizations. Brain Inj. 34, 1763–1770. doi: 10.1080/02699052.2020.1849801

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkowski, B. M., and Robinson, M. D. (2010). The anatomy of anger: an integrative cognitive model of trait anger and reactive aggression. J. Pers. 78, 9–38. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00607.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. E., Nieto, F. J., Sanford, C. P., Couper, D. J., and Tyroler, H. A. (2002). The association between trait anger and incident stroke risk. Stroke 33, 13–20. doi: 10.1161/hs0102.101625

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: traumatic brain injury, anger proneness, trait anger, cohort, epidemiology

Citation: Law CA, Elser H, Walter AE, Mosley TH, Walker K, Gottesman RF and Schneider ALC (2025) Traumatic brain injury and anger proneness: results from the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Front. Psychol. 16:1546443. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1546443

Received: 16 December 2024; Accepted: 10 June 2025;
Published: 25 June 2025.

Edited by:

Jay M. Uomoto, Henry M. Jackson Foundation for the Advancement of Military Medicine (HJF), United States

Reviewed by:

Daniela Di Basilio, Lancaster University, United Kingdom
Charles Levy, Henry M. Jackson Foundation for the Advancement of Military Medicine (HJF), United States

Copyright © 2025 Law, Elser, Walter, Mosley, Walker, Gottesman and Schneider. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Connor A. Law, Y29ubm9yLmxhd0BwZW5ubWVkaWNpbmUudXBlbm4uZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.