ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 11 July 2025

Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1565267

Psychometric analysis of the social connectedness instrument

  • The Social Connectedness Lab, Department of Psychology, Liberty University, Lynchburg, VA, United States

Introduction: Social connectedness is decreasing, especially among young adults, which poses a significant public mental and physical health threat globally. However, before attempting to improve social connectedness, measurement must first be evaluated. Many previous instruments used to measure loneliness (perceived social disconnectedness) provide a simple measure of intensity (i.e., how lonely/disconnected someone feels) but lack information about specific factors of loneliness and disconnectedness that are potentially modifiable (e.g., social skills, negative thoughts, technology use).

Methods: The current study aims to address these gaps by evaluating the psychometric properties of the Social Connectedness Instrument (SCI) using a sample of 719 college students (Mage = 19.63, SDage = 1.60, 75% female) attending a mid-Atlantic university. Psychometric evaluation, including exploratory factor analysis, principal component analysis, structural equation modeling, and tests of reliability and validity, was performed on the SCI.

Results: The final model of the SCI consists of two latent constructs, Psychoemotional Disconnectedness (PED) and Psychosocial Disconnectedness (PSD), which displayed satisfactory psychometric properties overall. While PED refers to a perception of feeling disconnected due to emotional contributors (e.g., social anxiety, fear of rejection), PSD refers to a perception of feeling disconnected due to social contributors (e.g., social skills, social motivation). An additional eleven single-item indicators of disconnectedness, which were not included in the final model, may be retained for further insight into someone's disconnectedness if brevity is not an issue.

Discussion: This novel instrument is recommended for use when a greater depth of perceived social disconnectedness and potentially modifiable contributors are needed to inform individualized interventions.

1 Introduction

Social connection/connectedness is a multidimensional construct that is defined as a continuum of the size and diversity of one's social network and roles, the functions that these relationships serve, and their positive or negative qualities (Holt-Lunstad, 2024, 2025). If one of these aspects of social connection are not achieved, a person may experience loneliness, a lack of social support, or a diminished social network. A recent scientific and public health emphasis has been placed on loneliness (perceived social isolation/disconnectedness), which is a particular dimension of social connectedness. While social connectedness is often defined objectively (e.g., social network size, social isolation; Holt-Lunstad, 2024), loneliness is defined as the perceived discrepancy between a person's desired and actual quantity and/or quality of relationships, resulting in a negative emotional state (Perlman and Peplau, 1981). Acute feelings of loneliness are universal and adaptive, leading individuals to reconnect with others (Cacioppo et al., 2010; Ypsilanti and Lazuras, 2022). In contrast to this, chronic feelings of loneliness are harmful and maladaptive, resulting in further perceived isolation, worse social connection, and negative affect (Cacioppo et al., 2010; Ypsilanti and Lazuras, 2022). Due to the global prevalence of these issues (Surkalim et al., 2022), loneliness and a lack of adequate social connection are being considered a public health threat. In the U.S., the Surgeon General declared that there is a “loneliness epidemic,” citing that changes must occur at a systemic level to improve social connection and reduce the adverse effects of loneliness (Murthy, 2023). However, until loneliness and poor social connectedness are better understood and conceptualized, it will be difficult to prevent and mitigate the growing problem.

Studies have reported that the prevalence of loneliness when accounting for age appears to be a U-shaped distribution, which indicates that loneliness is highest among late adolescents/young adults and older adults (Lasgaard et al., 2016; Victor and Yang, 2012). A recent meta-analysis on longitudinal studies reported this relationship between age and loneliness over time (Graham et al., 2024); participants had higher loneliness during late adolescence/young adulthood, followed by lower levels of loneliness during middle adulthood and steep increases in loneliness during older adulthood (Graham et al., 2024). Furthermore, other studies have shown that participants under 25 reported the highest rate (9%) of severe loneliness (Victor and Yang, 2012), and there was a 50% increase in agreeing to often feeling lonely for 12th graders in 2017 compared to 12th graders in 2012 (Twenge et al., 2019). Studies focused on social disconnectedness and social support have reported similar trends. Bruss et al. (2024) found that 24.1% of adults on average reported rarely or never receiving the social and emotional support they needed, with the highest rate (29.7%) reported by young adults (18–34). Among adolescents, 38.5% expressed feeling disconnected from others at school (Wilkins et al., 2023), with social ties appearing to decline with age (Akindele and Adebayo, 2021). Thus, while loneliness and poor social connectedness are important issues to address for all ages, they are an increasingly important issue to address within the late adolescent/young adult population. If chronic feelings of loneliness and poor social connectedness are left unaddressed, individuals are vulnerable to physical and mental health problems across the lifespan (Hawkley and Cacioppo, 2010).

Deficits in social connectedness can have significant adverse biopsychosocial consequences across the lifespan. Previous research has shown relationships between poor social connectedness (high loneliness, high social isolation, poor relationship quality) and various physical health problems, such as increased risk for hypertension, heart disease, stroke, and premature mortality (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2015, 2017; Park et al., 2020; Valtorta et al., 2016; Ueno et al., 2022). These relationships may exist due to an impact on key biological processes. For example, high levels of loneliness are related to higher levels of cortisol, inflammation, dysregulated immunity, a dysregulated hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, and worse health behaviors (Cacioppo et al., 2011; Cole et al., 2007; Lauder et al., 2006). Additionally, high loneliness and low perceived social support have strong relationships with worse mental health and lower emotional wellbeing (Park et al., 2020; Wickramaratne et al., 2022). Loneliness is linked with (cross-sectionally and longitudinally) higher levels of depression, anxiety, social anxiety, and suicidal ideation (Allan et al., 2021; Cacioppo et al., 2010; Helm et al., 2020; Maes et al., 2019; Moeller and Seehuus, 2019; Park et al., 2020; Shaw et al., 2021). Loneliness is also related to poorer social functioning, such as inhibiting a person's positive affect, enjoyment, and responsivity they demonstrate during a social interaction (Arpin and Mohr, 2018; Moeller and Seehuus, 2019; Smith et al., 2022).

The culmination of recent research findings indicate that poor social connectedness is a growing public health threat, negatively impacting overall biopsychosocial quality of life and wellbeing. Thus, we propose that social connectedness should be assessed as an essential marker of an individual's wellbeing by physicians, counselors, psychologists, teachers, employers, religious leaders, and community organizers. However, measuring an individual's level/intensity of social connectedness is only the starting point of what needs to be addressed. Subsequent efforts need to be made to reduce chronic feelings of disconnectedness and promote increased social connection. Previous research on efforts (i.e., interventions) to reduce chronic feelings of loneliness have shown mixed results (small to moderate effects) depending on type of intervention (e.g., social skills, social support, social contact, psychological, school-based) (Eccles and Qualter, 2021; Hickin et al., 2021; Masi et al., 2011; Allen et al., 2021). Due to the complexities of perceived social disconnectedness and varying perceptions on what might be contributing to or causing the experience (Hawkley and Cacioppo, 2010; Hemberg et al., 2022; Turner et al., 2024), researchers suggest providing an intervention plan tailored to an individual's unique experience with the phenomenon (Eccles and Qualter, 2021; Hickin et al., 2021; Masi et al., 2011). For example, if someone is experiencing disconnectedness due to poor interpersonal social skills, a social skills intervention may be the most appropriate for reducing feelings of loneliness and promoting social connection. If someone is experiencing disconnectedness due to maladaptive social cognitive processes (e.g., negative thoughts about themselves or others, social anxiety), then a psychological intervention may be more appropriate. However, to our knowledge, no social connectedness instrument exists to provide insight into someone's intensity of perceived disconnectedness (i.e., loneliness) in addition to potentially modifiable contributing variables (e.g., social skills, social anxiety) to that experience.

Well-established social connectedness instruments include, but are not limited to, the following: the Loneliness and Aloneness Scale for Children and Adolescents (LACA; Marcoen et al., 1987); the Children's Loneliness and Social Dissatisfaction Scale (CLS; Asher et al., 1984); the UCLA Loneliness Scale (UCLA LS; Russell et al., 1978; Russell, 1996), the Differential Loneliness Scale (DLS; Schmidt and Sermat, 1983), the Social and Emotional Loneliness Scale for Adults (SELSA; DiTommaso and Spinner, 1993), the De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale (DJGLS; de Jong-Gierveld and Kamphuis, 1985), the Revised Social Connectedness Scale (SCS-R; Lee et al., 2001), the Watts Connectedness Scale (WCS; Watts et al., 2022), the Hemmingway Measure of Adolescent Connectedness (HMAC; McWhirter and McWhirter, 2011), the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS; Zimet et al., 1988), the Sense of Belonging Instrument (SOBI; Hagerty and Patusky, 1995), and the Basic Psychological Needs Satisfaction Scale (BPNS; Ryan and Deci, 2000). The UCLA LS is the most widely used instrument in the scientific literature (Cole et al., 2021; Maes et al., 2022), and it attempts to provide a unidimensional measure for feelings of loneliness. Other instruments, such as the LACA (Marcoen et al., 1987), SELSA (DiTommaso and Spinner, 1993), DJGLS (de Jong-Gierveld and Kamphuis, 1985), SCS-R (Lee et al., 2001), WCS (Watts et al., 2022), HMAC (McWhirter and McWhirter, 2011), and MSPSS (Zimet et al., 1988) attempt to provide a multidimensional measure for various latent constructs of connectedness (e.g., social loneliness, emotional loneliness, perceived familial support, perceived peer support, etc.) (Too et al., 2022). More recently, researchers have used brief measures to quickly capture the intensity of perceived social disconnectedness (i.e., loneliness), especially in population-based studies (de Jong-Gierveld and Tilburg, 2006; Halvorson and Kuczynski, 2024; Hughes et al., 2004; Kotwal et al., 2022). Overall, these measures provide valuable insight into the intensity of social connectedness and various latent constructs, but there is limited insight provided about potential contributors to one's experience of disconnectedness. Additionally, while all of these measures focus solely on perceived social disconnectedness (i.e., loneliness, perceived belonging, perceived social support), social connection also includes objective aspects (Holt-Lunstad, 2025). Thus, there is need for a novel social connectedness instrument that provides more information about an individual's personalized experience of perceived disconnection and isolation in addition to objective modifiable contributors (e.g., number of close relationships, meaningful conversations, technology use) that could help in future intervention implementation.

The purpose of the current study is to assess the psychometric properties of a novel instrument, the Social Connectedness Instrument (SCI), designed to assess not only intensity of perceived social disconnectedness but also important perceived contributors (both subjective and quantifiable/objective) to that experience. For example, on the UCLA LS (version 3; Russell, 1996) a sample item is, “How often do you feel alone?” which provides insight into intensity of loneliness but limited knowledge from a modification standpoint (i.e., how to deal with the loneliness/disconnectedness). However, the SCI consists of subjective items, like “I am concerned people will reject me” and “Because of the heavy emotions I experience, I withdraw from others,” and objective/quantifiable items, like “How many close relationships do you have?” and “How many hours a day do you normally spend watching television, movies, or streaming content?” which provide insight into potentially modifiable contributors (e.g., fear or rejection, heavy emotions, close relationships, watching/streaming entertainment) to difficulties in connecting with others. While other perceived social disconnectedness instruments still have immense value for capturing feelings of loneliness, social support, and belongingness, especially in population-based research, the SCI may provide more details and insight into an individual's specific experience with feelings of loneliness and disconnection.

2 Aims

Our study aimed to assess the reliability and validity of the SCI in a sample of young adults at a university. Our objectives were as follows:

1. Conduct exploratory factor analysis and structural equation modeling to determine the latent constructs of the instrument.

2. Assess the fit and internal reliability and validity of the proposed model.

3. Examine the external convergent and divergent validity of the proposed model.

3 Materials and methods

3.1 Item development and data collection

The first iteration of items for the SCI was developed using an inductive and deductive strategy (Swan et al., 2023). We conducted focus-groups and qualitative interviews to establish perceptions of social connectedness (loneliness, social support, social isolation) and its causes and contributors amongst the target audience (young adults). Subsequently, we performed a literature review to cross-validate these perceptions. For example, in the interviews, many individuals related feelings of poor social connectedness to fear of rejection, which we then confirmed in the literature. Initially, 23 items were developed for the SCI, and pilot data was collected from 367 university students on these items. After assessing the initial data and receiving feedback on the content validity of these items, the following changes were made: (1) several items were edited for clarity, (2) several items were deleted due to poor content validity, and (3) several items were added to increase content validity.

The final iteration of the SCI contained 26 items (Table 1). Items 1–18 were rated on a Likert-type scale from one (never) to seven (always) and contained items on perceived social disconnectedness and potential contributors to the experience. Items 19–25 were rated on a quantitative scale from zero to five (or more) and contained objectively quantifiable aspects of social connection. Item 26 was rated on a Likert-type scale from one (not at all lonely) to ten (extremely lonely) and provided a single-item measure of acute feelings of loneliness. The data collection for the current study consists of three different waves. All three waves of data collection included the SCI but contained different secondary measures. The first wave consisted of 348 university students measured on the SCI and self-reported health behaviors. The second wave consisted of 91 university students measured on the SCI, social anxiety, and wellbeing (N = 43 completed a second time point after 7 days). The third wave consisted of 292 university students measured on the SCI and mindfulness. All of the data across studies were collected via Qualtrics and subsequently combined in Microsoft Excel to create the total sample for the current study.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The Social Connectedness Instrument.

3.2 Participants

The total sample for this study consisted of 719 university students from a large mid-Atlantic religious university (Mage = 19.63, SDage = 1.60). Inclusion criteria consisted of being ≥ 18 years old and a residential student at the university. The sample was predominantly female (75.3%). Regarding race/ethnicity, data was only collected for 627 out of the total 719 participants (missing from wave two). The race/ethnicity breakdown is as follows: 84.7% White, 4.1% Hispanic or Latinx, 3.8% Black, 3.5% Biracial or Multiracial, 3.3% Asian, 0.5% American Indian or Alaskan Native, and 0% Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander. The overall sample displayed high homogeneity (mostly young adults, religious, female, and White).

3.3 Psychometric analysis

We used Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) and Principal Component Analysis (PCA) with Horn's (1965) Parallel-Analysis to establish the initial factors of the SCI. Both statistical techniques offer different explorative approaches, and the final model was selected based off of factors adhering closer to an existing theoretical framework. Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) was then conducted to further examine the factors of the SCI and to establish the model fit. The final model was evaluated using goodness-of-fit statistics, which included the adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI), comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) (Hu and Bentler, 1998). A Multi-Group Structural Equation Model (MG SEM) was then conducted to explore measurement invariance for the latent constructs across sex. The internal reliability was analyzed using Raykov's Rho (Composite Reliability) (Hair et al., 2019). Next, the average variance extracted (AVE) was calculated for each latent construct to examine the internal convergent validity (Fornell and Larcker, 1981), and heterotrait-monotrait ratio of correlations was calculated to examine the internal discriminant validity (Henseler et al., 2015). Subsequent correlations were calculated to examine the SCI's external convergent and divergent validity.

Data analyses were performed using SPSS v29.0.0.(241) (IBM Corp, 2023), STATA/BE (StataCorp, 2024), and Microsoft Excel.

3.4 Other measures

To examine the external convergent and divergent validity of the SCI, the following measures were used:

1. The UCLA LS three-item version (UCLA LS-3; Hughes et al., 2004) is a brief measure of loneliness based off of the revised UCLA LS (R-UCLA LS; Russell et al., 1980). A sample item is “how often do you feel that you lack companionship?” The three items are rated on a Likert-type scale ranging from one (hardly ever) to three (often), with higher scores indicating higher levels of loneliness. Hughes et al. (2004) reported good internal reliability (α = 0.72) and strong concurrent validity with the R-UCLA LS (r = 0.82) for the UCLA LS-3. In the current study, the UCLA LS-3 also demonstrated good internal reliability (α = 0.83). Data on this measure were collected from 292 participants (from wave three).

2. We included a single-item measure of loneliness right now (LRN) at the end of the SCI (item 26). The item was “How lonely do you feel right now?” The item was rated from one (not at all lonely) to ten (extremely lonely), with higher scores indicating higher levels of acute loneliness. LRN demonstrated strong concurrent validity with the R-UCLA LS (r = 0.77) and the UCLA LS-3 (r = 0.74). Data on this single-item measure were collected from all participants (from waves one, two, and three).

3. The Social Avoidance and Distress Scale (SADS; Watson and Friend, 1969) assesses anxiety and distress in the context of social situations. Participants respond to 28 items by answering if the item is true or false regarding their situation. A sample item is “I have no particular desire to avoid people.” After totaling the responses to items, higher scores indicate higher levels of social anxiety and distress. In the current study, the SADS demonstrated excellent internal reliability (α = 0.93). Data on this measure were collected from 91 participants (from wave two).

4. The Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (WEMWBS; Bass et al., 2016) measures one's subjective wellbeing. The WEMWBS is comprised of 14 items, which are rated on a Likert-type scale from one (none of the time) to five (all of the time). A sample item is “I've been feeling good about myself.” Higher scores on the scale indicate higher levels of wellbeing. In the current study, the WEMWBS demonstrated good internal reliability (α = 0.87). Data on this measure were collected from 91 participants (from wave two).

5. The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS; Brown and Ryan, 2003) measures the present awareness and attention that one has in the current moment. Participants respond to 15 items on a Likert-type scale from one (almost always) to six (almost never). A sample item is “I could be experiencing some emotion and not be conscious of it until some time later.” Higher scores indicate higher levels of trait mindfulness. In the current study, the MAAS demonstrated good internal reliability (α = 0.88). Data on this measure were collected from 279 participants (from wave three).

6. The Healthy Lifestyle Screening for University Students (HLSUS; Dong et al., 2012) measures healthy lifestyle behaviors. The HLSUS consists of 38 items split into eight subscales, which include Exercise Behavior (α = 0.71), Regular Behavior (α = 0.72), Nutrition Behavior (α = 0.68), Health Risk Behavior (α = 0.43; omitted due to poor reliability), Health Responsibility (α = 0.55), Social Support (α = 0.54), Stress Management (α = 0.59), and Life Appreciation (α = 0.75). Items are rated on a Likert-type scale from one (never) to five (always). In the current study, the HLSUS total demonstrated good internal reliability (α = 0.81). Data on this measure were collected from 343 participants (from wave one).

4 Results

4.1 Data screening and assumptions

Analyses were conducted on all items to check data quality, descriptive statistics, and assumptions before further analysis. Of the initial data (N = 719), 652 responses (91.2%) had zero missing values. In total, there were 158 missing values (0.01%) across the 26 items on the SCI. Descriptive statistics were calculated for each item of the SCI (Supplementary Table 1). Items 1–18 (M = 3.09), 19–25 (M = 2.36), and 26 (M = 3.84) showed a slight bias toward lower scores in the current sample. The average inter-item correlation (rs) was 0.21, suggesting sufficient overlap between items while allowing for unique variance to be explained.

Data were then screened on assumptions regarding suitability for factor analysis on items 1–18. First, a Kaiser-Myer-Olkin was performed to provide a measure of sampling adequacy (MSA). The MSA = 0.90, which indicates that items were suitable for factor analysis (Kaiser, 1974). Next, Bartlett's test of sphericity was performed to test if the correlation matrix deviated from an identity matrix (i.e., all variables are unrelated). The test indicated that the correlation matrix was statistically significantly different from an identity matrix, χ2 (153) = 3,688, p < 0.001, which indicates that the variables are suitable for factor analysis (Bartlett, 1937).

4.2 Establishing the factors and model fit

To establish the initial factors of items 1–18 on the SCI, we performed an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and a principal components analysis (PCA). The EFA was conducted using maximum likelihood extraction and an oblique rotation method (PROMAX), which assumes that the underlying factors are correlated. The number of factors retained was based on Horn's Parallel Analysis (Horn, 1965; O'Connor, 2000), which compares the mean eigenvalues and the eigenvalues at the 95th percentile created from 1,000 randomly generated datasets to those from the original dataset, as opposed to Kaiser's criteria (eigenvalues λ ≥ 1; Guttman, 1954; Kaiser, 1960). Overall, the EFA suggested five-factors which explained 42% of the variance, with six items (2, 5, 6, 13, 17, and 18) loading onto factor one, four items (4, 7, 8 and 12) loading onto factor two, two items (15 and 16) loading onto factor three, three items (7, 11, and 14) loading onto factor four, and one item (10) loading onto factor five. The PCA, which was also performed using an oblique rotation method (PROMAX) and with Horn's Parallel Analysis criteria, suggested two factors which explained 40% of the variance, with nine items (3, 4, 7, 8, 9, 10, 12, 15, and 16) loading onto factor one and eight items (1, 2, 5, 6, 11, 13, 17, and 18) loading onto factor two (Supplementary Table 2).

After reviewing the factors reported from each analysis, we decided that the two factors from the PCA had better scientific and theoretical support; therefore, subsequent analyses were performed using the two factors. Specifically, these two factors align closely with the theoretical framework established by Weiss's typology of loneliness, which describes two distinct dimensions of loneliness: emotional and social (Russell et al., 1984; Weiss, 1973). Perceived social disconnectedness, which is broader and more encompassing of aspects of social connection than loneliness, may also reflect these two (emotional and social) underlying components. Items 1 and 9 were removed prior to further testing from their respective factor for containing semantic content inconsistent with the rest of the items in addition to the lowest factor loadings (λ < 0.35).

Next, structural equation modeling (SEM) was used in order to investigate the model fit. The final model consisted of eight items (3, 4, 7, 8, 10, 12, 15, and 16) on latent construct one and seven items (2, 5, 6, 11, 13, 17, and 18) on latent construct two. Overall, the model displayed satisfactory model fit indices [χ2 (82) = 315, p < 0.001; SRMR = 0.052; CFI = 0.985; TLI = 0.980; AGFI = 0.980] except for the RMSEA = 0.063 [95% CI (0.056, 0.071)], which was unsatisfactory (>0.050; Hu and Bentler, 1999). While the slightly elevated RMSEA can suggest model misspecification, given that the other fit indices are satisfactory, the overall model appears to perform well (Chen et al., 2008). The final model, which depicts the standardized factor loadings (ranging from 0.38 to 0.83; M = 0.58) between the indicators and the latent constructs, can be viewed in Figure 1.

Figure 1
Diagram showing two main circles labeled “PED” and “PSD” connected by an arrow with 0.80. PED is linked to SCI 3, 4, 7, 8, 10, 12, 15, 16 with values from 0.38 to 0.70. PSD connects to SCI 2, 5, 6, 11, 13, 17, 18 with values from 0.41 to 0.84.

Figure 1. Final SEM model of the SCI. For clarity, error variances were excluded but are available upon request.

We named the latent constructs based on the semantic content of each item that loaded onto it (Table 1). L1 (eight items) Psychoemotional Disconnectedness (PED) provides a measure of one's perception regarding their emotional experiences in the context of trying to connect, interact, or build relationships with others. All items on this construct are scored normally and higher scores reflect higher perceived social disconnectedness. L2 (seven items) Psychosocial Disconnectedness (PSD) provides a measure of one's perceived social skills, social opportunities, and social experiences. While most items (2, 5, 6, 13, and 17) are positively worded on this latent construct (e.g., I feel included by others), these items are reverse scored; thus, higher scores reflect higher perceived social disconnectedness. PED and PSD have a moderately strong relationship with one another (r = 0.58). Thus, while they are both capturing perceived social disconnectedness, they are measuring unique aspects of it.

4.3 Measurement invariance across sex

To establish the measurement invariance of the final model across sex (male N = 173, female N = 543), we performed a multi-group SEM. The results on measurement invariance found that no indicators showed sex bias (i.e., Wald test results across sex, p < 0.001). When the covariances and means of the latent variables were equal across groups, the multigroup SEM grouped by sex showed χ2 (217) = 638.12. The full model SEM resolved (scaled because of ordinal data) χ2(89) = 637. Therefore, the difference between the results across sex compared to the whole group equals: Δ χ2 (128) = 1.12, ns. Since Δ χ2 is viewed as one of the critical dimensions in evaluating group bias (Hair et al., 2019), results demonstrate that there is no statistically significant bias in the model due to sex differences. Both models (i.e., sex = male, sex = female) displayed high coefficients of determination (CD = 0.951); thus, each model accounted for 95.1% of the variation in the data.

4.4 Internal reliability and construct validity

The internal reliability of the constructs was calculated using Raykov's rho (Raykov and Shrout, 2002; Hair et al., 2022). PED (L1) displayed satisfactory reliability (ρ = 0.812) and PSD (L2) displayed satisfactory reliability (ρ = 0.776). Next, the internal convergent validity (i.e., whether the indicator variables converge to measure the same latent construct) was examined by calculating the average variance extracted (AVE) (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). PED (L1) and PSD (L2) did not meet the target cutoff of ≥0.50 (AVE = 0.358 and AVE = 0.345, respectively). However, given the high composite reliability and theoretical relevance for both of the latent constructs, we suggest that they each adequately converge to measure the underlying latent construct (PED and PSD) (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). For discriminant validity (i.e., whether the latent constructs can be differentiated from one another), the heterotrait-monotrait (HTMT) ratio of correlations was calculated (Henseler et al., 2015). The HTMT ratio of correlations (rHTMT = 0.661) was below the cutoff of 0.85, which suggests that the latent constructs discriminate well from one another (i.e., measure unique aspects of perceived social disconnectedness). Additionally, in a small subset of participants from wave two (N = 43) who completed the SCI twice (seven days in between), both PED (r = 0.82) and PSD (r = 0.74) demonstrated acceptable test-retest reliability.

4.5 External convergent and divergent validity

Next, we examined the convergent and divergent validity of PED and PSD, which is how strongly the constructs relate to other similar (convergent) or dissimilar (divergent) constructs. As expected, both PED and PSD displayed moderate to strong relationships with LRN (rPED = 0.56; rPSD = 0.54) and the UCLA LS-3 (rPED = 0.68; rPSD = 0.70), which are brief measures of loneliness. Regarding other psychological variables, PED displayed moderate to strong relationships with SADS (r = 0.64), WEMWBS (r = −0.50), and MAAS (r = −0.46). PSD also displayed moderate to strong relationships with SADS (r = 0.52), WEMWBS (r = −0.64), and MAAS (r = −0.40). For self-reported health behaviors, the PED and PSD displayed weak to moderate relationships with the HLSUS total (rPED = −0.29; rPSD = −0.48). The PED and PSD displayed weak relationships (r < 0.40) with the HLSUS subscales, except for between PSD and Social Support (r = −0.45) and Life Appreciation (r = −0.48) (see Table 2). Thus, PED and PSD demonstrated high convergent validity with similar measures (e.g., UCLA LS-3, LRN, SADS, WEMWBS, MAAS) and high divergent validity with dissimilar measures (e.g., Exercise Behavior, Regular Behavior, Health Responsibility).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlations between PSD, PED, and convergent/divergent measures.

Items 1, 9, and 14, which did not load onto the final model, and items 19–25, which were not used for the final model, can be used as single-item indicators of disconnectedness if someone scores high on PED, PSD, or alternative measures for social connectedness (e.g., loneliness, social support, etc.). All of these single-item measures, except for item 23, had statistically significant relationships with PED, PSD, UCLA LS-3, and LRN. Additionally, many of these items displayed statistically significant relationships with other psychological measures (SADS, WEMWBS, MAAS) and self-reported health behaviors (HLSUS) (see Table 3).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Spearman correlations between single-item indicators and PSD, PED, and other measures.

5 Discussion

5.1 Summary

While previous social connectedness measurement has focused solely on intensity, there is a need for a novel tool designed to capture not only intensity but also perceived contributors to social disconnectedness. To address this, the current study investigated the psychometric properties of the SCI. After preliminary descriptive and assumption analyses, an EFA and a PCA were performed to establish the initial factor-structure of items 1–18 on the SCI. Subsequent testing was performed using the two factor-structure established by the PCA. Next, SEM was used in order to examine the model fit and standardized factor loadings; the final model displayed satisfactory fit for all of the indices except for RMSEA, which was slightly unsatisfactory. After reviewing the content of each item on the respective latent construct, we named the first latent construct, Psychoemotional Disconnectedness (PED), and the second latent construct, Psychosocial Disconnectedness (PSD). The final model displayed measurement invariance across sex, satisfactory composite reliability, satisfactory discriminate validity, acceptable test-retest reliability, but both PED and PSD displayed low convergent validity (AVE < 0.5). Because PED and PSD showed satisfactory composite reliability, we propose that these constructs have adequate convergent validity (Fornell and Larcker, 1981) and therefore measure the same overall construct. PED and PSD displayed high external convergent and divergent validity, with higher correlations with measures of similar constructs and lower correlations with measures of dissimilar constructs. Additionally, the single-item indicators, which were not a part of PED or PSD, also demonstrated high external convergent and divergent validity.

5.2 Interpreting the theoretical model of the SCI: from perceived social disconnectedness to connectedness

The overall model of the SCI consists of two correlated latent constructs, PED and PSD, with additional single-item indicators of (dis)connectedness that may further detail an individual's social experience. While the two latent constructs are framed negatively (i.e., disconnectedness), the instrument name, the Social Connectedness Instrument, is framed positively (i.e., connectedness). Similar to the mental health continuum, which ranges from languishing (negative) to flourishing (positive) (Keyes, 2002), the theoretical continuum of social connectedness ranges from disconnectedness (negative) to connectedness (positive) (Holt-Lunstad, 2024). The SCI focuses on a continuum of perceived social connectedness, which ranges from low perceived connectedness (i.e., high disconnectedness) to high perceived connectedness (i.e., low disconnectedness). Thus, higher scores on PED and PSD indicate higher perceived disconnectedness (lower connectedness), while lower scores indicate lower perceived disconnectedness (higher connectedness) (see Figure 2).

Figure 2
Diagram showing the levels of perceived social connectedness across psychoemotional and psychosocial dimensions. On the left, high disconnectedness is highlighted with issues like health concerns, mistreatment by friends, and social exclusion. On the right, lower disconnectedness is associated with decreased levels of the same issues. At the bottom, factors such as geographic location and meaningful conversations are linked with increased connectedness. Arrows indicate whether issues are increasing or decreasing.

Figure 2. Final theoretical model of the SCI. The arrow direction indicates high scores (pointing up) or low scores (pointing down).

5.2.1 Psychoemotional disconnectedness and psychosocial disconnectedness

PED and PSD form related but unique constructs of perceived social disconnectedness. While these are similar constructs to emotional and social loneliness, as established by Weiss (1973) and measured using the DJGLS (de Jong-Gierveld and Kamphuis, 1985) and SELSA (DiTommaso and Spinner, 1993), they are distinct in that they measure the broader construct of perceived social disconnectedness and provide valuable insight into modifiable contributors to the experience of feeling disconnected from others (e.g., loneliness, social support, social cohesion). PED is made up of items that assess health concerns (item 3) (Carter et al., 2015; Tuncay et al., 2018), fear of rejection (item 4) (Zlomke et al., 2016), social anxiety (item 7) (Danneel et al., 2019; O'Day et al., 2021), bullying (item 8) (Bayat et al., 2021), mistreatment by friends (item 10) (Jones et al., 2020), lack of identity (item 12) (Kaniušonyte et al., 2019), negative thoughts (item 15) (Besser et al., 2022), heavy emotions (item 16) (Buecker et al., 2021; Gum et al., 2017; Lim et al., 2016) in the context of how lonely or socially disconnected it makes the person feel (see Table 1). The items converge together to provide insight into the overall psychoemotional aspects of perceived social disconnectedness, while still providing insight into potentially modifiable psychoemotional contributors at an item-by-item level. Psychosocial Disconnectedness (PSD) is made up of items that assess life transitions (item 2) (Laursen and Hartl, 2013; Sundqvist et al., 2024), interests/desires/hobbies (item 5) (Mak et al., 2023; Martin et al., 2013), social skills/confidence (item 6) (Floyd and Woo, 2020; Lodder et al., 2016), social motivation (item 11) (Nikitin and Freund, 2017), feeling included (item 13) (Park and Baumeister, 2015), experiencing belonging/purpose through religious beliefs (item 17) (Nezlek, 2021), and perceived isolation (item 18) (Cacioppo et al., 2010) in the context of how socially disengaged and disconnected they make the person feel (items 2, 5, 6, 13, and 17 are reverse-scored; see Table 1). The items converge together to provide insight into the overall psychosocial aspects of perceived social disconnectedness, while still providing insight into potentially modifiable psychosocial contributors at an item-by-item level. PED and PSD demonstrated high external convergent validity, displaying strong relationships with the UCLA LS-3 (Hughes et al., 2004) and item 26 on the SCI. Additionally, both PSD and PED displayed moderate to strong relationships with social anxiety, wellbeing, and mindfulness.

PED and PSD have a moderate positive association with one another, which suggests that as someone displays higher PED, they also tend to display higher PSD. This finding lines up with previous social connectedness instruments which also have associated factors, such as between social and emotional loneliness on the DJGLS (de Jong-Gierveld and Kamphuis, 1985) and SELSA (DiTommaso and Spinner, 1993). However, it is possible for someone to display a high score for PED and a low score for PSD since these are related but unique constructs. Thus, someone can theoretically have feelings of disconnectedness and connectedness simultaneously. For example, someone could experience emotional disruptions (e.g., social anxiety, fear of rejection) that make them feel disconnected from others (high PED) while having sufficient social opportunities (e.g., shared interests/hobbies, belonging through religious beliefs) that make them feel connected to others (low PSD). However, given the relationship between PED and PSD, it is more likely that one will feel disconnected/connected both emotionally and socially.

5.2.2 Other single-item indicators of disconnectedness

Of the 26 items on the SCI, three items did not load onto a factor (1, 9, and 14) and eight items were not included in the factor analysis (items 19–26). Although these items were not included in the final factors of the SCI, we recommend including them still if there are no time-constraints as they provide more information about an individual's experience with social connectedness. Item 1 examines how geographic location may impact a person's ability to connect with others. While objective social isolation and loneliness typically have a weak relationship with one another (Lennartsson et al., 2022), people may perceive that their specific geographical area offers limited opportunities to connect with others, which can contribute to feelings of disconnectedness (Victor and Pikhartova, 2020). Item 9 examines how commitments may impede one's ability to have a social life. As people develop across the lifespan, it is essential to consider how specific commitments (e.g., job, school, family) may reduce the ability or time available for building meaningful social connections (McKenna-Plumley et al., 2023). Question 19 (meaningful conversations), question 20 (close relationships), and question 25 (meaningful religious services attended) provide brief measures focused on more quantifiable components of social connection. Higher levels for these constructs are associated with better overall social connectedness (Block et al., 2022; Hastings, 2016; Okabe-Miyamoto et al., 2024). Conversely, question 24 (social events declined) provides a brief quantifiable measure of how isolated one is choosing to be by turning down invitations for connection, which can be related to higher feelings of perceived isolation (Santini et al., 2020). Item 14 (preference for connecting online), question 21 (social media usage), question 22 (streaming entertainment), and question 23 (playing video games) all provide information about someone's technology use. While technology use is increasingly prevalent and not always an indicator of poor social connectedness, studies have demonstrated a relationship between fewer in-person interactions and disconnectedness (Twenge et al., 2019), decreasing social media usage and improvements in feelings of loneliness (Hunt et al., 2018), higher binge-watching behavior and higher loneliness (Gabbiadini et al., 2021), and online gaming addiction and higher loneliness (Gao et al., 2024). Each of these single-item indicators of disconnectedness had statistically significant relationships with the UCLA LS-3 (Hughes et al., 2004) (except item 23), LRN (item 26), PED (except item 23), and PSD, suggesting that they provide valuable insight into a person's experience with social disconnectedness. However, due to their single-item nature (i.e., no measure of reliability), they must be interpreted with caution and may need longer-form follow-up assessments (e.g., interviews, longer self-report instruments) if individuals score high.

5.2.3 Advantages

Previous research suggests that intervention plans tailored to a person's specific experience may be more effective at increasing social connectedness and reducing feelings of disconnectedness (Eccles and Qualter, 2021; Hickin et al., 2021; Masi et al., 2011). However, the most widely used instrument to measure loneliness, the UCLA LS Version 3 (Russell, 1996), provides a unidimensional measure of loneliness that captures how lonely someone is without providing many details about possible reasons for their experience. The SCI provides a novel theoretical framework in which social connectedness interventions can better be tailored to a person's specific experience of feeling disconnected. Instead of a general intervention, such as a social skills or mindfulness training, being assigned arbitrarily to individuals with poor connectedness, professionals (e.g., practitioners, researchers, educators, etc.) can now measure and intervene on two correlated but distinct latent constructs of perceived social disconnectedness. It is recommended to first target the latent construct (PED or PSD) with a higher score or to combine approaches if scores are equal (e.g., a socially and emotionally focused intervention). For example, if someone scores higher on PSD, one should further examine the scores of each item that form the latent construct and develop a socially focused intervention that will help the person to overcome their specific challenges. Perhaps the individual scored highest on item 5 (interests/desires/hobbies), item 6 (social skills/confidence) and item 13 (feeling excluded). In this case, an intervention for this individual may first try to increase feelings of social connectedness by helping the person to improve their social skills and social confidence in relationships, while offering direction for finding and connecting with other people who share similar interests/hobbies. The additional single-item indicators of (dis)connectedness may also offer direction for personalized interventions that are more quantifiable/behavioral (e.g., reducing technology use, increasing meaningful conversations). However, since this is a novel theoretical framework for improving perceived social connectedness, data needs to be collected to examine the effectiveness of using an individualized approach informed by the SCI as opposed to the typical population-based intervention approach.

5.3 Limitations and future directions

The study's most significant limitation is the homogeneous sample, which included a sample of college students who were mainly religious (Christian), White, and female. Due to the sampling limitation, the SCI has limited external validity across other demographics. This could have impacted the results, such as by influencing which specific items loaded onto each factor. For example, item 17 (purpose through religion) loaded onto PSD, but may not load onto the factor in other non-religious samples. Due to the sampling limitation, the SCI has limited external validity across other demographics. Thus, a future study should investigate the external and cross-cultural validity with a more diverse sample, including people of different ages, socioeconomic statuses, races/ethnicities, countries, and health statuses (clinical populations). Next, only a couple of secondary measures were included to capture external convergent/divergent validity. A subsequent study should examine how the SCI is related to other established measures of social connectedness (loneliness, social support, and social isolation) and mental health (depression, anxiety, stress, self-esteem, etc.). Lastly, there is no data to support that these latent constructs (PED and PSD) and other single-item indicators help to provide a more personalized intervention for reducing loneliness and promoting social connectedness. Therefore, a future longitudinal study should examine if tailoring an intervention based off of the SCI is better at improving social connectedness as compared to assigning someone to any social connectedness intervention at random.

6 Conclusion

In efforts to reduce loneliness and promote social connection, it is important to investigate novel instruments that provide further depth. Overall, the testing on the SCI revealed two latent constructs (PED and PSD) in addition to other single-item indicators of disconnectedness, which were well-supported statistically and theoretically. However, in order to generalize this model to different demographics, further studies need to investigate how the SCI performs amongst different samples. While there are established measures of loneliness and social connectedness, the SCI provides further insight into one's experience with perceived social disconnectedness that may be useful for implementing personalized social connectedness instruments.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Liberty University Institutional Review Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

BK: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. BF: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AW: Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MF: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. The data collection was made possible in part by support from the Office of Sponsored Programs & Research: Internal Awards, Liberty University.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Liberty University, the School of Behavioral Sciences, the Department of Psychology, and the many other faculty and students who contributed to this work.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1565267/full#supplementary-material

References

Akindele, A. O., and Adebayo, A. M. (2021). Social connectedness and health risk behaviours among in-school adolescents in urban and rural areas of Oyo State, Nigeria. J. Prev. Med. Hyg. 62, E689–E703. doi: 10.15167/2421-4248/jpmh2021.62.3.2107

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Allan, N. P., Volarov, M., Koscinski, B., Pizzonia, K. L., Potter, K., Accorso, C., et al. (2021). Lonely, anxious, and uncertain: critical risk factors for suicidal desire during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatry Res. 304, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2021.114144

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, J. P., Narr, R. K., Nagel, A. G., Costello, M. A., and Guskin, K. (2021). The connection project: changing the peer environment to improve outcomes for marginalized adolescents. Dev. Psychopathol. 33, 647–657. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419001731

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arpin, S. N., and Mohr, C. D. (2018). Transient loneliness and the perceived provision and receipt of capitalization support within event-disclosure interactions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 45, 240–253. doi: 10.1177/0146167218783193

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Asher, S. R., Hymel, S., and Renshaw, P. D. (1984). Loneliness in children. Child Dev. 55, 1456–1464. doi: 10.2307/1130015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartlett, M. S. (1937). Properties of sufficiency and statistical tests. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. A Math. Phys. Sci. 160, 268–282. doi: 10.1098/rspa.1937.0109

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bass, M., Dawkin, M., Muncer, S., Vigurs, S., and Bostock, J. (2016). Validation of Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS) in a population of people using secondary care mental health services. J. Mental Health 25, 323–329. doi: 10.3109/09638237.2015.1124401

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayat, N., Fokkema, T., Mujakovic, S., and Ruiter, R. A. C. (2021). Contextual correlates of loneliness in adolescents. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 127:106083. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2021.106083

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Besser, A., Flett, G. L., Nepon, T., and Zeigler-Hill, V. (2022). Personality, cognition, and adaptability to the COVID-19 pandemic: associations with loneliness, distress, and positive and negative mood states. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 20, 971–995. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00421-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Block, V. J., Haller, E., Villanueva, J., Meyer, A., Benoy, C., Walter, M., et al. (2022). Meaningful Relationships in community and clinical samples: their importance for mental health. Front. Psychol. 13:832520. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.832520

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruss, K. V., Seth, P., and Zhao, G. (2024). Loneliness, lack of social and emotional support, and mental health issues—United States, 2022. MMWR Morbid. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 73, 539–545. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm7324a1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buecker, S., Denissen, J. J. A., and Luhmann, M. (2021). A propensity-score matched study of changes in loneliness surrounding major life events. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 121, 669–690. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000373

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Norman, G. J., and Bernston, G. G. (2011). Social Isolation. Ann. New York Acad. Sci. 1, 17–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06028.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., and Thisted, R. A. (2010). Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. Psychol. Aging 25, 453–463. doi: 10.1037/a0017216

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, B., Qualter, P., and Dix, J. (2015). social relationships, loneliness and adolescence: the potential disruption by chronic illness. J. Child Health Care 19, 421–422. doi: 10.1177/1367493515618477

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F., Curran, P. J., Bollen, K. A., Kirby, J., and Paxton, P. (2008). An empirical evaluation of the use of fixed cutoff points in RMSEA test statistic in structural equation models. Sociol. Methods Res. 36, 462–494. doi: 10.1177/0049124108314720

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, A., Bond, C., Qualter, P., and Maes, M. (2021). A systematic review of the development and psychometric properties of loneliness measures for children and adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:3285. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18063285

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, S. W., Hawkley, L. C., Arevalo, J. M., Sung, C. Y., Rose, R. M., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). Social regulation of gene expression in human leukocytes. Genome Biol. 8, 1–13. doi: 10.1186/gb-2007-8-9-r189

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Danneel, S., Nelemans, S., Spithoven, A., Bastin, M., Bijttebier, P., Colpin, H., et al. (2019). Internalizing problems in adolescence: linking loneliness, social anxiety symptoms, and depressive symptoms over time. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 47, 1691–1705. doi: 10.1007/s10802-019-00539-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Jong-Gierveld, J., and Kamphuis, F. (1985). The development of a Rasch-Type Loneliness Scale. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 9, 289–299. doi: 10.1177/014662168500900307

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Jong-Gierveld, J., and Tilburg, T. (2006). A 6-Item Scale for overall, emotional, and social loneliness: confirmatory tests on survey data. Res. Aging 28, 582–598. doi: 10.1177/0164027506289723

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

DiTommaso, E., and Spinner, B. (1993). The development and initial validation of the social and emotional loneliness scale for adults (SELSA). Pers. Individ. Dif. 14, 127–134. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90182-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, W., Xiao-Hui, X., and Xian-Bo, W. (2012). The Healthy Lifestyle Scale for university students: development and psychometric testing. Aust. J. Prim. Health 18, 339–345. doi: 10.1071/PY11107

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, A. M., and Qualter, P. (2021). Alleviating loneliness in young people–a meta-analysis of Interventions. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 26, 17–33. doi: 10.1111/camh.12389

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Floyd, K., and Woo, N. T. (2020). Loneliness and social monitoring: a conceptual replication of Knowles et al. Pers. Relatsh. 27, 209–223. doi: 10.1111/pere.12304

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Market. Res. 18, 39–50. doi: 10.1177/002224378101800104

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabbiadini, A., Baldissarri, C., Valtorta, R. R., Durante, F., and Mari, S. (2021). Loneliness, escapism, and identification with media characters: an exploration of the psychological factors underlying binge-watching tendency. Front. Psychol. 12:785970. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.785970

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, B., Cai, Y., Zhao, C., Qian, Y., Zheng, R., and Liu, C. (2024). Longitudinal associations between loneliness and online game addiction among undergraduates: a moderated mediation model. Acta Psychol. 243:104134. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2024.104134

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, E. K., Beck, E. D., Jackson, K., Yoneda, T., McGhee, C., Pieramici, L., et al. (2024). Do we become more lonely with age? A coordinated data analysis of nine longitudinal studies. Psychol. Sci. 35, 579–596. doi: 10.1177/09567976241242037

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gum, A. M., Shiovitz-Ezra, S., and Ayalon, L. (2017). Longitudinal associations of hopelessness and loneliness in older adults: results from the US Health and Retirement Study. Int. Psychogeriatr. 29, 1451–1459. doi: 10.1017/S1041610217000904

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guttman, L. (1954). Some necessary conditions for common-factor analysis. Psychometrika 19, 149–161. doi: 10.1007/BF02289162

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagerty, B. M., and Patusky, K. (1995). Developing a measure of sense of belonging. Nurs. Res. 44, 9–13. doi: 10.1097/00006199-199501000-00003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., and Anderson, R. E. (2019). Multivariate Data Analysis. 8th Edn. Cengage Learning EMEA.

Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Hult, G. T. M., Ringle, C. M., and Sarstedt, M. (2022). A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM). 3rd Edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.

Google Scholar

Halvorson, M. A., and Kuczynski, A. M. (2024). Measuring loneliness in everyday life. Transl. Iss. Psychol. Sci. 10, 331–345. doi: 10.1037/tps0000438

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hastings, O. P. (2016). Not a lonely crowd? Social connectedness, religious service attendance, and the spiritual but not religious. Soc. Sci. Res. 57, 63–79. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2016.01.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkley, L. C., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Ann. Behav. Med. 40, 218–227. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Helm, P. J., Medrano, M. R., Allen, J. J. B., and Greenberg, J. (2020). Existential isolation, loneliness, depression, and suicide ideation in young adults. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 39, 641–674. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2020.39.8.641

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hemberg, J., Korzhina, Y., Nyström, L., Östman, L., Groundstroem, H., and Nyman-Kurkiala, P. (2022). Loneliness as experienced by adolescents and young adults: an explorative qualitative study. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 27, 362–384. doi: 10.1080/02673843.2022.2109422

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Henseler, J., Ringle, C. M., and Sarstedt, M. (2015). A new criterion for assessing discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. J. Acad. Market. Sci. 43, 115–135. doi: 10.1007/s11747-014-0403-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickin, N., Käll, A., Shafran, R., Sutcliffe, S., Manzotti, G., and Langan, D. (2021). The effectiveness of psychological interventions for loneliness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 88, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102066

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt-Lunstad, J. (2024). Social connection as a critical factor for mental and physical health: evidence, trends, challenges, and future implications. World Psychiatry 23, 312–332. doi: 10.1002/wps.21224

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt-Lunstad, J. (2025). Social connection or loneliness? How we frame the issue may significantly impact public policy. Health Psychol. 44, 560–562. doi: 10.1037/hea0001433

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt-Lunstad, J., Robles, T. F., and Sbarra, D. A. (2017). advancing social connection as a public health priority in the United States. Am. Psychol. 72, 517–530. doi: 10.1037/amp0000103

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., and Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 227–237. doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika 30, 179–185. doi: 10.1007/BF02289447

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: sensitivity to under parameterized model misspecification. Psychol. Methods 3, 424–453. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.3.4.424

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Eq. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). A Short Scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: results from two population-based studies. Res. Aging 26, 655–672. doi: 10.1177/0164027504268574

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunt, M. G., Marx, R., Lipson, C., and Young, J. (2018). No more FOMO: limiting social media decreases loneliness and depression. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 37, 751–768. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2018.37.10.751

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

IBM Corp (2023). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows. Vol. (Version 29.0.2.0). Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

Google Scholar

Jones, J. L., Kahn, J. H., and Sullivan, S. D. (2020). Relational aggression victimization as a predictor of middle-school girls' self-disclosure to peers. Violence Vict. 35, 54–67. doi: 10.1891/0886-6708.VV-D-18-00085

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaiser, H. F. (1960). The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 20, 141–151. doi: 10.1177/001316446002000116

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaiser, H. F. (1974). An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika 39, 31–36. doi: 10.1007/BF02291575

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaniušonyte, G., Truskauskaite-Kunevičiene, I., Žukauskiene, R., and Crocetti, E. (2019). Knowing who you are for not feeling lonely? A longitudinal study on identity and loneliness. Child Dev. 90, 1579–1588. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13294

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keyes, C. L. M. (2002). The mental health continuum: from languishing to flourishing in life. J. Health Soc. Behav. 43, 207–222.

Google Scholar

Kotwal, A. A., Cenzer, I. S., Waite, L. J., Smith, A. K., Perissinotto, C. M., and Hawkley, L. C. (2022). A single question assessment of loneliness in older adults during the COVID-19 pandemic: a nationally-representative study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 70, 1342–1345. doi: 10.1111/jgs.17700

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lasgaard, M., Friis, K., and Shevlin, M. (2016). Where are all the Lonely People?” A population-based study of high-risk groups across the life span. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 51, 1373–1384. doi: 10.1007/s00127-016-1279-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauder, W., Mummery, K., Jones, M., and Caperchione, C. (2006). A comparison of health behaviors in lonely and non-lonely populations. Psychol. Health Med. 11, 233–245. doi: 10.1080/13548500500266607

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laursen, B., and Hartl, A. C. (2013). Understanding loneliness during adolescence: developmental changes that increase the risk of perceived social isolation. J. Adolesc. 36, 1261–1268. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.06.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, R. M., Draper, M., and Lee, S. (2001). Social connectedness, dysfunctional interpersonal behaviors, and psychological distress: testing a mediator model. J. Couns. Psychol. 48:310. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.48.3.310

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lennartsson, C., Rehnberg, J., and Dahlberg, L. (2022). The association between loneliness, social isolation and all-cause mortality in a nationally representative sample of older women and men. Aging Ment. Health 26, 1821–1828. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2021.1976723

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, M. H., Rodebaugh, T. L., Zyphur, M. J., and Gleeson, J. F. M. (2016). Loneliness over time: the crucial role of social anxiety. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 125, 620–630. doi: 10.1037/abn0000162

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lodder, G. M. A., Goossens, L., Scholte, R. H. J., Engels, R. C. M. E., and Verhagen, M. (2016). Adolescent loneliness and social skills: agreement and discrepancies between self-, meta-, and peer-evaluations. J. Youth Adolesc. 45, 2406–2416. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0461-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maes, M., Nelemans, S. A., Danneel, S., Fernández-Castilla, B., Van den Noortgate, W., Goossens, L., et al. (2019). Loneliness and social anxiety across childhood and adolescence: multilevel meta-analyses of cross-sectional and longitudinal associations. Am. Psychol. Assoc. 55, 1548–1565. doi: 10.1037/dev0000719

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maes, M., Qualter, P., Lodder, G. M. A., and Mund, M. (2022). How (not) to measure loneliness: a review of the eight most commonly used scales. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:10816. doi: 10.3390/ijerph191710816

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mak, H. W., Noguchi, T., Bone, J. K., Wels, J., Gao, Q., Kondo, K., et al. (2023). Hobby engagement and mental wellbeing among people aged 65 years and older in 16 countries. Nat. Med. 29, 2233–2240. doi: 10.1038/s41591-023-02506-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcoen, A., Goossens, L., and Caes, P. (1987). Loneliness in pre-through late adolescence: exploring the contributions of a multidimensional approach. J. Youth Adolesc. 16, 561–577. doi: 10.1007/BF02138821

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, A., Jacob, C., and Guéguen, N. (2013). Similarity facilitates relationships on social networks: a field experiment on facebook. Psychol. Rep. 113, 217–220. doi: 10.2466/21.07.PR0.113x15z8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Masi, C. M., Chen, H. - Y, Hawkley, L. C., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2011). A meta-analysis of interventions to reduce loneliness. Person. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 15, 219–266. doi: 10.1177/1088868310377394

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McKenna-Plumley, P. E., Turner, R. N., Yang, K., and Groarke, J. M. (2023). Experiences of loneliness across the lifespan: a systematic review and thematic synthesis of qualitative studies. Int. J. Qual. Stud. Health Well-being 18:2223868. doi: 10.1080/17482631.2023.2223868

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McWhirter, B. T., and McWhirter, E. H. (2011). Connectedness among Chilean adolescents: factor analysis of the hemingway measure of adolescent connectedness. Psykhe 20, 45–62. doi: 10.4067/S0718-22282011000100004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moeller, R. W., and Seehuus, M. (2019). Loneliness as a mediator for college students' social skills and experiences of depression and anxiety. J. Adolesc. 73, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.03.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murthy, V. H. (2023). Our Epidemic of Loneliness and Isolation: The US Surgeon General's Advisory on the Healing Effects of Social Connection and Community. US Department of Health and Human Services.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nezlek, J. B. (2021). Relationships between religiosity and naturally occurring social interaction. J. Relig. Health 60, 3454–3466. doi: 10.1007/s10943-020-01079-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nikitin, J., and Freund, A. M. (2017). Social motives predict loneliness during a developmental transition. Swiss J. Psychol. 76, 145–153. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185/a000201

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Connor, B. P. (2000). SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of components using parallel analysis and Velicer's MAP Test. Behav. Res. Methods Inst. Comp. 32, 396–402. doi: 10.3758/BF03200807

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Day, E. B., Butler, R. M., Morrison, A. S., Goldin, P. R., Gross, J. J., and Heimberg, R. G. (2021). Reductions in social anxiety during treatment predict lower levels of loneliness during follow-up among individuals with social anxiety disorder. J. Anxiety Disord. 78:102362. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2021.102362

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Okabe-Miyamoto, K., Walsh, L. C., Ozer, D. J., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2024). Measuring the experience of social connection within specific social interactions: The Connection during Conversations Scale (CDCS). PLoS ONE 19:e0286408. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0286408

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, C., Majeed, A., Gill, H., Tamura, J., Ho, R. C., Mansur, R. B., et al. (2020). The effect of loneliness on distinct health outcomes: a comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 294:113514. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113514

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, J., and Baumeister, R. F. (2015). Social exclusion causes a shift toward prevention motivation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 56, 153–159. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.09.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perlman, D., and Peplau, L. A. (1981). Toward a social psychology of loneliness. Pers. Relatsh. 3, 31–56.

Google Scholar

Raykov, T., and Shrout, P. E. (2002). Reliability of scales with general structure: point and interval estimation using a structural equation modeling approach. Struct. Eq. Model. 9, 195–212. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, D., Cutrona, C. E., Rose, J., and Yurko, K. (1984). Social and emotional loneliness: an examination of Weiss's typology of loneliness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 46, 1313–1321. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.46.6.1313

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., and Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39, 472–480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.39.3.472

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., and Ferguson, M. L. (1978). Developing a measure of loneliness. J. Pers. Assess. 42, 290–294. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4203_11

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, D. W. (1996). UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. J. Pers. Assess. 66, 20–40. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6601_2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55:68. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Santini, Z. I., Jose, P. E., York Cornwell, E., Koyanagi, A., Nielsen, L., Hinrichsen, C., et al. (2020). Social disconnectedness, perceived isolation, and symptoms of depression and anxiety among older Americans (NSHAP): a longitudinal mediation analysis. Lancet Public Health 5, e62–e70. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(19)30230-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, N., and Sermat, V. (1983). Measuring loneliness in different relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44, 1038–1047. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.5.1038

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, R. J., Cullen, B., Graham, N., Lyall, D. M., Mackay, D., Okolie, C., et al. (2021). Living alone, loneliness and lack of emotional support as predictors of suicide and self-harm: a nine-year follow up of the UK Biobank Cohort. J. Affect. Disord. 279, 316–323. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.10.026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, K. E., Norman, G. J., and Decety, J. (2022). Increases in loneliness during medical school are associated with increases in individuals' likelihood of mislabeling emotions as negative. Emotion 22, 740–750. doi: 10.1037/emo0000773

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

StataCorp (2024). STATA Statics and Data Sciences. Vol. 18.0. College Station, TX: BE-Basic Edition StataCorp, LLC.

Google Scholar

Sundqvist, A. J. E., Nyman-Kurkiala, P., Ness, O., and Hemberg, J. (2024). The influence of educational transitions on loneliness and mental health from emerging Adults' perspectives. Int. J. Qual. Stud. Health Wellbeing 19:2422142. doi: 10.1080/17482631.2024.2422142

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Surkalim, D. L., Luo, M., Eres, R., Gebel, K., van Buskirk, J., Bauman, A., et al. (2022). The prevalence of loneliness across 113 countries: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ 376:067068. doi: 10.1136/bmj-2021-067068

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Swan, K., Speyer, R., Scharitzer, M., Farneti, D., Brown, T., Woisard, V., et al. (2023). Measuring what matters in healthcare: a practical guide to psychometric principles and instrument development. Front. Psychol. 14:1225850. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1225850

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Too, E. K., Chongwo, E., Mabrouk, A., and Abubakar, A. (2022). Adolescent connectedness: a scoping review of available measures and their psychometric properties. Front. Psychol. 13:856621. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.856621

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuncay, F. O., Ferteli, T., and Mollaoglu, M. (2018). Effects of loneliness on illness perception in persons with a chronic disease. J. Clin. Nurs. 27, e1494–e1500. doi: 10.1111/jocn.14273

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, S., Fulop, A., and Woodcock, K. A. (2024). Loneliness: adolescents' perspectives on what causes it, and ways youth services can prevent it. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 157:107442. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2024.107442

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Twenge, J. M., Spitzberg, B. H., and Campbell, W. K. (2019). Less in-person social interaction with peers among U.S. adolescents in the 21st century and links to loneliness. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 36, 1892–1913. doi: 10.1177/0265407519836170

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ueno, T., Nakagomi, A., Tsuji, T., and Kondo, K. (2022). Association between social participation and hypertension control among older people with self-reported hypertension in Japanese communities. Hypertens. Res. 45, 1263–1268. doi: 10.1038/s41440-022-00953-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Valtorta, N. K., Kanaan, M., Gilbody, S., Ronzi, S., and Hanratty, B. (2016). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for coronary heart disease and stroke: systematic review and meta- analysis of longitudinal observational studies. Heart 102, 1009–1016. doi: 10.1136/heartjnl-2015-308790

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Victor, C. R., and Pikhartova, J. (2020). Lonely places or lonely people? Investigating the relationship between loneliness and place of residence. BMC Public Health 20, 1–12. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-08703-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Victor, C. R., and Yang, K. (2012). The prevalence of loneliness among adults: a case study of the United Kingdom. J. Psychol. 146, 85–104. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2011.613875

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., and Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 33, 448–457. doi: 10.1037/h0027806

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watts, R., Kettner, H., Geerts, D., Gandy, S., Kartner, L., Mertens, L., et al. (2022). The Watts Connectedness Scale: A New Scale for measuring a sense of connectedness to self, others, and world. Psychopharmacology 239, 3461–3483. doi: 10.1007/s00213-022-06187-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, R. S. (1973). Loneliness: The Experience of Emotional and Social Isolation. The MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Wickramaratne, P. J., Yangchen, T., Lepow, L., Patra, B. G., Glicksburg, B., Talati, A., et al. (2022). Social connectedness as a determinant of mental health: a scoping review. PLoS ONE 17:e0275004. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0275004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkins, N. J., Krause, K. H., Verlenden, J. V., Szucs, L. E., Ussery, E. N., Allen, C. T., et al. (2023). School connectedness and risk behaviors and experiences among high school students—youth risk behavior survey, United States, 2021. Morbid. Mort. Wkly. Rep. Suppl. 72, 13–21. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.su7201a2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ypsilanti, A., and Lazuras, L. (2022). Loneliness is not a homogeneous experience: an empirical analysis of adaptive and maladaptive forms of loneliness in the UK. Psychiatry Res. 312:114571. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2022.114571

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., and Farley, G. K. (1988). The multidimensional scale of perceived social support. J. Pers. Assess. 52, 30–41. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5201_2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zlomke, K., Jeter, K., and Cook, N. (2016). Recalled childhood teasing in relation to adult rejection and evaluation sensitivity. Pers. Individ. Dif. 89, 129–133. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.10.021

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: loneliness, social connectedness, social disconnectedness, psychometric validation, structural equation modeling, latent constructs, intervention, young adults

Citation: Kelley B, Fraser B, Wells A and Ferdock M (2025) Psychometric analysis of the social connectedness instrument. Front. Psychol. 16:1565267. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1565267

Received: 22 January 2025; Accepted: 20 June 2025;
Published: 11 July 2025.

Edited by:

Alessandro Giuliani, National Institute of Health (ISS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Shakshi Priya Giri, Deen Dayal Upadhyaya Gorakhpur University, India
Edwin Adrianta Surijah, Central Queensland University, Australia

Copyright © 2025 Kelley, Fraser, Wells and Ferdock. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Blake Fraser, YmFmcmFzZXJAbGliZXJ0eS5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.