- Department of Basic, Developmental, and Educational Psychology, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
Introduction: Out-of-body experiences (OBEs) are primarily characterized by the sensation of the self being located outside one's physical body. The complexity of this phenomenon has led researchers to propose various theories to explain it, including physiological, psychological, and non-local consciousness theories. The objective of this study is to directly explore the interpretations of individuals who have experienced this phenomenon firsthand.
Method: The study employed a qualitative descriptive design with a phenomenological interpretive analysis approach, using in-depth semi-structured interviews. The sample comprised 10 participants without mental disorders or neurological and/or vestibular pathologies. The factors studied were predisposing, precipitating, phenomenological, consequential, and interpretive.
Results: All participants agreed that their experience was not only real but described it as more vivid and authentic than everyday reality. Four participants had no explanation for their experience, while one interpreted it in physiological terms. The remaining five explained their experiences using terms like “other planes or dimensions” and “universal consciousness,” aligning with some authors who use concepts such as “non-local” or “expanded consciousness” to address OBEs.
Discussion: The findings suggest that, given that most participants refer to explanations that go beyond what is commonly understood as consciousness, theories of non-local consciousness could be enriched by incorporating these experiential perspectives.
1 Introduction
An out-of-body experience (OBE) is defined as a phenomenon in which the center of consciousness appears to temporarily occupy a position spatially remote from one's body (Irwin, 1985). Humans have reported OBEs since ancient times, with descriptions of this complex phenomenon found in most cultures (Metzinger, 2005). Despite the enigmatic nature of OBEs, their occurrence is notable, with an estimated prevalence of 10% to 20% of the population (Alvarado, 2000).
OBEs manifest in diverse ways, with each individual describing unique sensations and circumstances surrounding the experience. The triggering factors are diverse and, in many cases, completely opposite to one another. These experiences can arise in moments of deep tranquility, such as during meditation or relaxation (Alvarado, 1988, 1997; Blanke, 2004; Blanke et al., 2004; Gow et al., 2004; Sellers, 2017; Twemlow, 1989; Wilde and Murray, 2009; Zingrone et al., 2010), or, conversely, in situations of extreme stress, when the individual is in physical danger or undergoing psychological trauma (Alvarado, 1988, 1997; Bateman et al., 2017; Roisin, 2009).
Once the experience is triggered, the sensation of being outside the body can be accompanied by a wide range of emotions. Some individuals describe an absolute sense of peace (Alvarado, 1988; Aujayeb, 2013; Blanke et al., 2004; Gow et al., 2004; Facco et al., 2019; Sellers, 2017; Tressoldi et al., 2015; Weiler and Acunzo, 2024; Wilde and Murray, 2009), while in other cases, the experience can be marked by fear (Blanke et al., 2004; Gow et al., 2004), especially due to the thought: “I won't be able to return to my body” (Sellers, 2017).
One aspect that appears to be common among most individuals who have experienced an OBE is the strong conviction that what they are experiencing is real. They do not perceive it as a dream or a hallucination (Alvarado, 1997; Blanke et al., 2004; Campillo-Ferrer et al., 2024; Gallo et al., 2023; Rabeyron and Caussié, 2016; Sellers, 2017; Thakkar et al., 2011; Weiler and Acunzo, 2024; Wilde and Murray, 2009).
In general, OBEs are brief experiences in which the individual feels they are leaving their body, observing themselves from the ceiling, and then returning. However, in some cases, the experience can be much more complex. Some individuals describe traveling to different locations (Alvarado and Zingrone, 1998; Baud, 2017; Sellers, 2017; Tressoldi et al., 2015; Weiler and Acunzo, 2024; Wilde and Murray, 2009) and even encountering other beings (Sellers, 2017; Twemlow, 1989).
Given the complexity and subjective nature of OBEs, analyzing and explaining them within the scientific paradigm is an extremely complex task. It is not surprising, therefore, that the first publications on this phenomenon appeared in parapsychology journals (Alvarado, 1982; De Foe et al., 2013; Irwin, 2000). Over time, the biological and behavioral sciences have begun to investigate these experiences, giving rise to different approaches: psychological, physiological, and non-local consciousness perspectives.
From a psychological perspective, OBEs have been interpreted as a form of mental dissociation, sometimes linked to strategies for coping with traumatic events (Alvarado, 1997, 2016). In contrast, physiological theories suggest that OBEs are distorted perceptions resulting from alterations in the integration of somatosensory and vestibular information (Blanke, 2004; Blanke and Arzy, 2005; Blanke and Mohr, 2005; Blanke et al., 2002, 2004, 2005; Wu et al., 2023). Beyond these approaches, theories have emerged that consider OBEs within the concept of non-local consciousness, moving away from the idea that consciousness is strictly a product of the brain (Brumblay, 2003; Carruthers, 2018; Pace and Drumm, 1992; Persinger, 2010).
Both psychological and physiological approaches have primarily focused on interpreting OBEs as mere consequences of other processes, often overlooking the perspective of those who experience them. The methodologies used in these studies—mainly experimental, quasi-experimental, and observational (analytical or descriptive)—tend to prioritize external interpretations of the phenomenon from researchers' perspectives while neglecting the subjective interpretations of individuals who undergo these experiences.
Following Husserl's (1931) phenomenological framework, we argue that these experiences should be described as they present themselves to consciousness, without resorting to theoretical presuppositions or external causal explanations. This approach emphasizes the value of subjective accounts from those who have experienced OBEs. By analyzing their personal interpretations, we can explore whether OBEs hold intrinsic meaning rather than being mere byproducts of other underlying processes.
Addressing this gap is crucial to achieving a deeper understanding of the phenomenon. This is essential for two main reasons: first, to comprehend the nature of human consciousness, and second, to help normalize these experiences for individuals who undergo them.
In contrast to most studies conducted thus far, this research aims to directly explore the interpretations of individuals who have experienced OBEs firsthand. Moreover, unlike previous studies, this work seeks to provide a comprehensive and systematic understanding of the phenomenon by structuring an analysis through previously defined specific factors. Thus, the goal of this study is to examine predisposing, precipitating, phenomenological, consequential, and interpretive factors, which have been conceptualized as follows:
• Predisposing factors: personality characteristics or psychopathologies that facilitate the experience.
• Precipitating factors: internal or external circumstances that cause the experience.
• Phenomenological factors: the nature of the experience (sensations, emotions, and cognitions).
• Consequential factors: emotional or cognitive repercussions of the experience.
• Interpretive factors: explanations about the reason for the experience.
2 Method
2.1 Subjects
Sampling was not random due to the specific nature of the study population but was intentional. The inclusion criteria included experiencing at least one OBE and the absence of mental disorders, as well as neuronal and/or vestibular pathologies. Additionally, all participants were functionally independent in their daily lives, either working or studying, and demonstrated clear verbal expression and communication skills during the interviews. Cardiac arrests or other circumstances that cause clinical death or bring the person closer to the end of life can lead to OBEs. In these cases, the name used is: “Near Death Experience” (NDE), which is usually analyzed as a different phenomenon due to its own idiosyncrasy (Klemenc-Ketis, 2013; Van Lommel et al., 2001). Therefore, only subjects who have experienced OBEs in non-near-death situations have been included in the present study.
To mitigate possible biases related to the beliefs of the participants, the sample was not recruited through associations or organizations interested in this phenomenon, but through social networks. Sample size was determined by information saturation and depth of understanding achieved. The final sample consisted of 10 participants, aged between 21 and 56 years, of which 6 were men and 4 women.
2.2 Design
The study employed a qualitative descriptive design with a phenomenological interpretive analysis approach (Smith et al., 2009), following the standards proposed by O'Brien et al. (2014). In-depth interviews were conducted to explore individuals' experiences of OBEs and the meaning these hold for them.
2.3 Instruments
Information was obtained through semi-structured recorded interviews lasting approximately 1 h. The interview was structured in three phases. In the first phase, structured questions were asked to obtain sociodemographic and quantitative data, such as the number of OBEs and age of onset. The second phase was conducted in a completely open manner, allowing participants to describe in detail their most significant OBE. The third phase consisted of collecting information that had not emerged in phase two about the pre-established categories (predisposing, precipitating, phenomenological, consequential, and interpretive factors).
2.4 Procedure
Data collection through interviews was conducted in February 2024. The ethics committee of the Autonomous University of Barcelona (CEEAH protocol 6746) approved this study. All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with IRB-approved procedures. All authors of this article were present during the interviews, though only one was responsible for asking the pre-established questions. The presence of the other authors enabled deeper exploration through additional questions. This dynamic facilitated the capture of useful non-verbal information for subsequent data interpretation.
After completing and transcribing the interviews, the information was extracted and organized. For this extraction, sheets were created with two sections: one for sociodemographic and quantitative data, and another for information on pre-established categories (predisposing, precipitating, phenomenological, consequential, and interpretive factors).
The second and third authors completed the sheets individually. Subsequently, the data were triangulated by comparing the information on the sheets. In cases of discrepancies, these two researchers, together with the first author, discussed the differences until reaching a consensus. Finally, the entire research team analyzed and selected quotes that best represented participants' expressions of the factors. These quotes were included in the results (Tables 1A, B).
3 Results
The results are found in Tables 1A, B. As can be seen, the age of onset of two participants was in childhood, three in adolescence and five in youth. Of the 10 participantss interviewed, seven have experienced OBEs once or twice in their lives while 3 on numerous occasions.
3.1 Most meaningful experience
The results indicate that the experiences are very idiosyncratic, although some common elements were identified. In cases 1, 4, and 7, the participants experienced the out-of-body sensation while lying in bed. Participants 1 and 4 saw their bodies from the ceiling, while participant 7 saw theirs from another part of the room. Participant 8 had the experience while sitting on a street bench and viewed their body from the sky, approximately at the height of a second floor. Participant 6 found himself suddenly in a long, dark hallway. Participant 9 saw his body from behind while walking. Participants 5 and 10 reported traveling to very distant places, such as other cities or countries.
3.2 Phenomenology of experience
In cases 2, 3, and 6, the participants felt fear. However, the rest of the participants described their experiences with the following words: calm and strangeness (1), pleasant and comfortable (4), impressive (5), curiosity and freedom (7), euphoria (9), and peace (10). In one case (8), despite living the experience fully, fear arose when thinking that he would not be able to return to the body. All participants perceived the experience as real. The words of participant 3 exemplify this: “I experience it as I am now touching this table, that is, as a reality.”
3.3 Predisposing factors
Participant 1 stated that at the time she experienced the OBE, she was taking antidepressants that caused dissociative states, which she believed predisposed her to the experience. Participant 2 mentioned that a few hours before feeling the onset of his OBE, he had performed meditative therapy for the first time and attributed the OBE to this, as it had produced a kind of internal change: “a strange sensation as if something had moved in me.”
For participant 3, the predisposition was her own motivation. Her aunt, who experienced constant OBEs, encouraged her to have them as well. Two of the participants (5 and 10) had experienced OBEs since childhood, which made it difficult for them to identify predisposing factors. However, participant 5 suggested there might be some hereditary influence from her maternal grandparents. Similarly, participant 10 spoke of influence from her maternal side: grandmother, great-grandmother, great-great-grandmother, all of whom were healers. This, according to her, led to her growing up in an environment open to the unknown and spiritual. Participant 7 stated that lucid dreaming led him to seek ways to self-induce OBEs. The other interviewees (4, 6, 8 and 9) did not have any explanation as to what could have predisposed them.
3.4 Precipitating factors
Three of the participants (3, 6, and 7) induced their OBEs through meditation, while another participant (2) experienced it because of meditation but did not actively seek to provoke the experience; rather, it occurred unexpectedly. For four participants, the OBEs happened in tranquil settings: while lying in bed (4, 5, and 10) and while sitting on a bench at dusk (8). For participant 1, the OBE was triggered by an orgasm. And for participant 9, it resulted from consuming Ketamine.
3.5 Consequential factors
The interviewees were questioned regarding three specific aspects of the experience: (1) whether it left them with a positive impression, prompting a desire to repeat it; (2) if it altered their perspective on death or their worldview; and (3) whether they chose to explain it with others. The question about whether they would repeat the experience only made sense for the seven participants who had experienced it once or twice given that the other three experienced it repeatedly. Among these seven, participant 2 said that he would not repeat it out of fear. In fact, when this individual began to sense the departure from his body, he managed to stop it; it could be argued that he never fully underwent the OBE due to fear. The remaining six participants expressed a willingness to repeat the experience, although some with conditions. Participant 8 would do so if he could exert control over it and participant 9 if it was induced through meditation and not ketamine. Participant 6 stated that, despite the fear it provoked, would repeat it out of curiosity. Finally, three interviewees (1, 4, 7) would repeat it without conditions because they lived it as a very pleasant experience.
When questioned about whether their perspective on death or their worldview had changed, only two participants responded affirmatively. Participant 7 conveyed that the experience led him to believe in a notion of transformation rather than cessation, suggesting that we do not perish; rather, we transform. Participant 10, who had experienced numerous OBEs, reflected that traversing “another plane” had granted her insight into the insignificance of many concerns prevalent in our everyday lives. Those who stated that the experience had not changed them (1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6 and 9) argued that they already had a spiritual vision, and the experience confirmed it. Participant 8, however, articulated that he maintained his disbelief in anything beyond death both before and after the experience because he considered that the experience was not proof of anything spiritual.
Regarding whether the participants had explained their experience, participant 8 had not told it to anyone for fear of the judgment of others. Participants 1, 3 and 7 had only confided in a single individual or their closest circle for similar reasons. Participant 5 revealed that during childhood, she kept it to herself due to concerns about her parents' reaction. Conversely, the remaining participants (2, 4, 6, 9, and 10) shared their experiences without reservation, unconcerned about others' perceptions.
3.6 Interpretative factors
None of the participants claimed to possess a definitive explanation for their experiences, except for the individual who attributed it solely to ketamine (9), suggesting a purely physiological origin. Four of them stated that they had no explanation: “I think it's something that just happens” (4); “it was just a little trip” (6); “maybe I fell asleep or was half asleep, I don't know” (8); and “I had to experience it to know that it existed and that's it” (2). The remainder consistently expressed uncertainty, offering simple hypotheses or possibilities. Explanations varied, including notions of contact with a universal consciousness (1), heightened awareness of the spiritual aspect of the self (3 and 7), and moving in other planes and dimensions (5 and 10).
4 Discussion
The present study is based on in-depth interviews with individuals who have experienced OBEs. As a qualitative study, it does not allow for generalizable conclusions. Instead, the objective is to understand the experiences and personal explanations of the phenomenon from the participants' perspectives. Unlike most literature that aims for researchers to reach specific conclusions, this study prioritizes the participants' own hypotheses.
The results of this study confirm that OBEs are very idiosyncratic experiences, both in their phenomenology and in the precipitating and consequential factors (Alvarado, 1982, 1988; Aspell and Blanke, 2009).
Regarding the age of onset, it was observed that the first OBE typically occurred during childhood or youth. However, it is difficult to determine whether OBEs also commonly begin in adulthood, as the sample was predominantly composed of individuals under 30 years old (7 out of 10). Among the two participants over 50 years old, both reported having experienced their first OBE during childhood. According to Alvarado (1988), studies that attempt to correlate the occurrence of OBEs with age generally do not find a significant correlation. However, Blackmore (1986), through a questionnaire administered to 97 individuals, observed significant differences in age between those who reported experiencing OBEs and those who did not. Those who had experienced OBEs tended to be younger.
Most participants (7 out of 10) reported experiencing only one or two OBEs throughout their lives, with just three indicating frequent occurrences. This finding aligns with previous literature (Blanke, 2004; Parra, 2008).
4.1 Predisposing factors
Throughout the research on this topic, several researchers have postulated various predisposing factors, including: beliefs (Gow et al., 2004; Parra, 2010, 2018); the absorption capacity (Gow et al., 2004; Irwin, 2000; Parra, 2010, 2018); psychopathologies (Gow et al., 2004; Irwin, 2000; Lopez and Elzière, 2018; Mudgal et al., 2021; Murray and Fox, 2005; Parra, 2010; Roisin, 2009; Terhune, 2006) or neurological pathologies (Blanke, 2004; Blanke and Arzy, 2005; Yu et al., 2018). However, unlike these investigations, the present study aimed to explore predisposing factors based on the participants' own intuitions rather than those posited by researchers. Additionally, to understand their opinions on the researchers' hypotheses regarding this point. In this regard, none of the participants indicated that their personality traits might have predisposed them to experience OBEs. Only the motivation to experience the phenomenon was identified as a predisposing factor, as mentioned by two participants. One participant was taking antidepressants at the time of the experience; however, she attributed the OBE not to depression but to the effects of the medication.
Regarding beliefs, certain authors maintain that beliefs of a spiritual nature can facilitate OBEs to a greater extent than those of a scientific nature (Gow et al., 2004; Parra, 2010, 2018). In line with this hypothesis, one of the participants who had experienced OBEs since childhood mentioned growing up in an environment conducive to paranormal and spiritual phenomena, suggesting that this could have predisposed her. In contrast, the rest of the participants did not indicate that their beliefs had any influence.
The data from this study suggest that, for most participants, their beliefs did not influence their first OBE. When asked to explain their experiences, half of them did not provide any spiritual explanation, instead expressing uncertainty or offering physiological explanations. Other studies have also not found a significant relationship between beliefs and OBEs (Aujayeb, 2013; Bova, 2011; Terhune, 2006; Wilde and Murray, 2009).
Although our data suggests that beliefs do not predispose individuals to their first experience, they do appear to influence its repetition. This assertion is based on the observation that in our sample, the three individuals who experienced numerous OBEs believed in realities beyond our own. While we cannot definitively determine the direction of this influence, it is possible that their mindset has contributed to the increased incidence of OBEs, or perhaps experiencing multiple OBEs has reinforced their beliefs.
If we categorize OBEs based on their complexity, we can distinguish between simple and complex experiences. Simple OBEs involve the participant leaving their body but not moving far from it, remaining in a place where they can observe their physical body. In contrast, complex OBEs involve longer journeys and a variety of experiences. In our research, most participants have experienced simple OBEs, and the vast majority have had only one such experience. Conversely, participants who have experienced more complex OBEs have done so on numerous occasions. This leads us to hypothesize that the complexity of OBEs can increase with repeated experiences.
The experience of lucid dreams has also been linked to OBEs (Blackmore, 1986; Raduga et al., 2020). In line with this association, one participant in our sample habitually experienced lucid dreams. Moreover, he stated that it was precisely the experience of lucidity during sleep that led him to seek out OBEs, suggesting that, for him, the two phenomena were somehow related.
4.2 Precipitating factors
Before experiencing OBEs, most participants were in calm situations, supporting findings documented in the literature (Blanke, 2004; Blanke et al., 2004; Gow et al., 2004; Sellers, 2017; Wilde and Murray, 2009; Zingrone et al., 2010). However, we cannot conclude that only these types of circumstances act as precipitating factors, as other studies have also observed that stressful situations can trigger them (Aujayeb, 2013; Bateman et al., 2017; Brandt et al., 2009; Roisin, 2009).
Self-induction through various meditative methods has also been observed as a trigger for OBEs in three of the participants, confirming the findings of other studies (Alvarado, 1982; Blanke and Mohr, 2005; Bova, 2011; Sellers, 2017; Smith and Messier, 2014). Additionally, consistent with Wilkins et al. (2011), this research has demonstrated that Ketamine can act as a precipitating factor.
In one of the participants, orgasm was identified as a precipitating factor. However, we have not found information about this in other investigations. We consider that the physiological alteration induced by the orgasm could have led to the experience. This participant mentioned that the feelings of dissociation caused by the antidepressants she was taking could have predisposed her. Therefore, we hypothesize that the medication may have predisposed her, and that the orgasm triggered the experience.
4.3 Phenomenological factors
Numerous investigations have highlighted two prevalent sensations in OBEs: the feeling of peace (Aujayeb, 2013; Blanke et al., 2004; Gow et al., 2004; Facco et al., 2019; Sellers, 2018; Tressoldi et al., 2015; Wilde and Murray, 2009) and the sense of reality (Blanke et al., 2004; Rabeyron and Caussié, 2016; Sellers, 2017; Thakkar et al., 2011; Wilde and Murray, 2009). In line with these findings, all the participants in our sample stated that they felt the experience as real, clearly distinguishing it from a hallucination or a dream; and most described it as full of peace.
Certain research has demonstrated that the experiences participants have during OBEs align with reality. For instance, Ring and Cooper (1999) noted that some individuals blind from birth reported visual perceptions during their OBEs, which were later validated as accurate. Parra (2008) suggests that the verifiability of many OBEs is attributed to some form of extrasensory perception of environments that are physically inaccessible. In our sample, two participants affirmed that reality confirmed what they had experienced during their OBEs. Participant 5 described an out-of-body experience where she visited the hospital to see her aunt in the Intensive Care Unit (ICU). The following day, upon visiting the hospital in reality, she was deeply surprised to find that the hallway, the door, and the ICU where her aunt was located were exactly as she had seen during the OBE. Participant 10 reported that during her OBE, she visited a village in Scotland. As she flew in, she observed a bridge and a specific landscape, and upon “landing” in the village, she noticed the village's name. Later, she confirmed on a map that both the river and the village existed. From the subjective perspective of these participants, there seems to be a clear correspondence between their experiences during the OBE and reality. Nevertheless, it is crucial to note that these cases do not allow us to draw objective conclusions in this regard.
The presence of fear has also been observed in some cases, especially related to the feeling of lack of control and concern about being able to return to one's own body. These sensations have been documented in other studies (Sellers, 2017).
4.4 Consequential factors
Some studies show that people who experience OBEs decrease their fear of death (Shaw et al., 2023; Irwin, 1988; Osis, 1979). In our sample, only one participant explained that the experience made her feel that death is just a transformation. It was found that people who did not believe in anything else after death still did not believe it after the experience. In fact, Irwin (1985) and Palmer (1979) maintain that OBEs do not typically produce religious conversions among atheists. Conversely, the more spiritual participants maintained that the OBEs only reaffirmed their previous beliefs.
We have noted that people who experience OBEs occasionally do not suffer major changes in their lifestyle or interpretation of life. This finding contrasts with participants from other studies, such as that of Shaw et al. (2023), where beneficial changes have been observed after OBEs. We believe that the impact of the experience may depend on its content and the individual's own characteristics. However, it was observed that the two participants who experienced the phenomenon since their childhood had a perspective on life where the problems were relativized to the maximum, confirming what was found in other studies (Baud, 2017; Bova, 2011; Rabeyron and Caussié, 2016; Wilde and Murray, 2009, 2010) and in which there was no fear of death (Shaw et al., 2023). It seems that the beneficial effects occur mainly in people who have had the experience on numerous occasions. In fact, in the study by Shaw et al. (2023), where profound changes have been observed as a result of OBEs, most participants in the sample had experienced them repeatedly. In these cases, we could say that OBEs can be considered transformative experiences, as defined by many researchers: as phenomena able to engender long-lasting, irreversible, pervasive consequences on individuals' beliefs, perceptions, identity, and values (Chirico et al., 2022).
According to the literature, most people who experience the phenomenon would like to repeat it (Gabbard and Twemlow, 1984; Irwin, 1988; Osis, 1979); we have found the same tendency in the participants of our sample.
In this research, some people opted not to share their OBE with anyone or only confided in those close to them, fearing they would not be understood or labeled as disordered. From this arises the importance of conducting outreach efforts to familiarize the general public with this phenomenon and foster its normalization, as other authors also suggest (Bova, 2011; De Foe, 2012). This participant not only requires dissemination among the general public but is also imperative for inclusion in the training curriculum of health professionals. It is crucial that they receive adequate training to effectively assist individuals who have undergone this phenomenon. The absence of understanding regarding this topic could result in incorrect diagnoses and difficulties in its management by healthcare professionals, as highlighted by De Foe (2012).
4.5 Interpretative factors
How do the participants themselves interpret the OBE phenomenon? Some simply confess that they have no explanation. This lack of explanation is also found in the scientific field. Despite countless investigations in this regard, there are no solid conclusions, but rather a multitude of hypotheses from vastly different paradigms.
One participant attempted to explain the phenomenon in physiological terms, as a state resulting from ketamine. Many authors try to explain OBEs in physiological terms. From a biological framework, they are conceived as distorted perceptions resulting from a failure in somatosensory and vestibular integration (Blanke, 2004; Blanke and Arzy, 2005; Blanke and Mohr, 2005; Blanke et al., 2002, 2004, 2005; Bos et al., 2016). The fact that some people with neurological disorders, such as epilepsy or headaches, present OBEs corroborates this view (Blanke, 2004; Blanke and Arzy, 2005; Blanke and Mohr, 2005; Blanke et al., 2002, 2004; De Ridder et al., 2007; Fang et al., 2014).
Dissociation has been proposed as a psychological explanation for this phenomenon, as many individuals appear to undergo the experience to evade a traumatic event (Alvarado, 2016; Bateman et al., 2017; De Ridder et al., 2007; Gow et al., 2004). This dissociation, as pointed out by Parra (2008), does not signify any psychopathology, contrary to historical assumptions (Alvarado, 1992; Facco et al., 2019); rather, it is viewed as a beneficial coping strategy. However, in this study, none of the participants reported their experience as dissociative. Moreover, none of them were in a stressful situation necessitating dissociation to alleviate distress.
In conventional scientific terms, consciousness is a result of evolution, the biological adaptation of the nervous system. Consciousness is viewed as an individual phenomenon within this framework. In contrast to this traditional understanding of consciousness, half of the participants in our study used terms such as “dimensions”, “planes”, “universal consciousness”, among others. Their expressions were aligned with emerging theories on expanded or non-local consciousness. According to these theories, the mind or consciousness is not completely confined to the brain or body but may have properties that transcend space and time (Hameroff and Penrose, 2014; Tononi and Koch, 2015). The concept of nonlocality finds support in quantum physics, which explores how subatomic particles separated by vast distances can be entangled in a manner beyond explanation by conventional physics. Some authors approach OBEs from this novel perspective (Brumblay, 2003; Pace and Drumm, 1992; Persinger, 2010).
4.6 Final considerations: relevance, limitations and future research avenues
As highlighted by Shaw et al. (2023), most research in this domain tends to be correlational, seeking connections between personality traits or pathologies and OBEs, or concentrating on individuals with neurological disorders. Researchers typically attempt to elucidate this phenomenon through psychological and/or physiological hypotheses. In contrast, our study did not start with an explanatory hypothesis; rather, it aimed to explore the interpretations of the participants themselves. This approach enabled us to compare the perspectives of researchers observing the phenomenon from an external standpoint with the interpretations of participants experiencing it firsthand.
None of the participants in our sample considered their experience to be a hallucination or a perceptual error, as some theories suggest. Even the participant who experienced OBEs under the effects of ketamine stated that what he experienced was real. Likewise, none of them related the experience to any trait of their personality. When some of the participants discussed the potential involvement of their brains in OBEs, they did not interpret it as a disorder or a failure in information integration, as some physiological theories suggest. Instead, they perceived it as an amplification or augmentation of their perception or experiential abilities.
Qualitative studies should persist in capturing the interpretations of the participants themselves and the phenomenology of their experiences. Nonetheless, such studies have their limitations. One of these is information saturation, used as a parameter to determine sample size. Given the complexity of such phenomena, and the necessity for obtaining highly detailed information, achieving saturation is exceedingly complicated; it would require such a large sample size that conducting in-depth analysis would become difficult.
One strategy to mitigate this limitation is to delimit, to the greatest extent possible, the objective of the study. In our case, we have analyzed multiple factors (predisposing, precipitating, phenomenological, consequential, and interpretive). However, in future qualitative studies, it would be more appropriate to focus on a single factor for deeper analysis. Following the example of Shaw et al. (2023), who focused solely on the transformative aspects of OBEs, could be beneficial.
Another way to address this limitation is to ensure a more homogeneous sample. Specifically, we propose to homogenize the number of previous OBEs. In our study, most participants had experienced the phenomenon only once in their lives, while two individuals had experienced it since childhood. This difference was notable in how they interpreted the experience and its impact on their lives. Therefore, we recommend investigating these two groups separately. In line with this proposal, we find Neppe's (2011) perspective. He suggests that the diversity of theoretical perspectives on OBEs is due to each model likely examining a specific type of OBE. Therefore, he argues that the correct approach would be to describe OBEs from a detailed phenomenological perspective to classify them appropriately. This would allow for a better understanding of these experiences.
The future of research in this field can follow two main paths: applied and basic research. In terms of applied research, we believe the initial goals should focus on two key aspects: normalizing this phenomenon and fostering personal growth and self-awareness in those who experience it.
Normalization is essential, as our study confirms that most individuals hesitate to share their experiences out of fear of being perceived as mentally disturbed. Additionally, our findings support the idea that OBEs can, in some cases, lead to profound personal transformations. Therefore, enhancing and integrating these experiences could also have therapeutic potential.
Moreover, beyond these objectives, there is an opportunity to gain valuable insights from individuals who, through their OBEs, have developed a more relative perspective on life and have overcome the fear of death. These insights could be instrumental in addressing psychological conditions associated with existential distress.
And within basic research, the study of OBEs would allow us to explore the concept of consciousness more broadly. By viewing the OBE phenomenon as something natural and not something strange to be explained, we could draw on the experiences and interpretations of the people who experience it to arrive at unexplored points of view about consciousness. The current scientific paradigm appears too narrow to adequately address the complexity of this phenomenon. Adopting an open-minded approach could aid in integrating various perspectives, from physiological and psychological to non-local consciousness, which need not be mutually exclusive. This could lead to a more comprehensive understanding of consciousness.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The Ethics Committee of the Autonomous University of Barcelona (CEEAH protocol 6746) approved this study. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
JM: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. IN: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. AD: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Alvarado, C.S. (2016). Out-of-body experiences during physical activity: report of four new cases. J. Soc. Psychical Res. 80, 1–12.
Alvarado, C. S. (1982). ESP during out-of-body experiences: a review of experimental studies. J. Parapsychol. 46, 209–230.
Alvarado, C. S. (1988). Psychological aspects of out-of-body experiences: a review of spontaneous case studies. Puerto Rican J. Psychol. 5, 31–43.
Alvarado, C. S. (1992). The psychological approach to out-of-body experiences: a review of early and modern developments. J. Psychol. 126, 237–250. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1992.10543358
Alvarado, C. S. (1997). Mapping the characteristics of out-of-body experiences. J. Am. Soc. Phys. Res. 91, 27–32.
Alvarado, C. S. (2000). “Out-of-body experiences,” in Varieties of Anomalous Experience: Examining the Scientific Evidence, eds. E. Cardeña, S. J. Lynn, and S. Krippner (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 183–218. doi: 10.1037/10371-006
Alvarado, C. S., and Zingrone, N. L. (1998). A study of the features of out-of-body experiences in relation to Sylvan Muldoon's claims. J. Parapsychol. 62:104.
Aspell, J. E., and Blanke, O. (2009). “Understanding the out-of-body experience from a neuroscientific perspective,” in Psychological scientific perspective on out-of-body and near-death experiences, ed. C. D. Murray (New York: Nova Science Publishers, Inc), 73–88.
Aujayeb, A. (2013). Out-of-body experience in the Karakorum. Wilderness Environ. Med. 24, 295–297. doi: 10.1016/j.wem.2013.01.012
Bateman, L., Jones, C., and Jomeen, J. (2017). A narrative synthesis of women's out-of-body experiences during childbirth. J. Midwifery Women's Health 62, 442–451. doi: 10.1111/jmwh.12655
Baud, S. (2017). Expériences ≪ hors du corps ≫: un voyage ≪ en esprit ≫ à la rencontre d'un autre de soi. Intellectica 67, 347–366. doi: 10.3406/intel.2017.1849
Blackmore, S. J. (1986). Spontaneous and deliberate out-of-body experiences: a questionnaire survey. J. Soc. Psychical Res. 53, 218–224.
Blanke, O. (2004). Out-of-body experiences and their neural basis. Br. Med. J. 329, 1414–1415. doi: 10.1136/bmj.329.7480.1414
Blanke, O., and Arzy, S. (2005). The out-of-body experience: disturbed self-processing at the temporo-parietal junction. Neuroscientist: A Rev. J. Bringing Neurobiol. Neurol. Psychiatry 11, 16–24. doi: 10.1177/1073858404270885
Blanke, O., Landis, T., Spinelli, L., and Seeck, M. (2004). Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin. Brain 127, 243–258. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh040
Blanke, O., and Mohr, C. (2005). Out-of-body experience, heautoscopy, and autoscopic hallucination of neurological origin. Brain Res. Rev. 50, 184–199. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.05.008
Blanke, O., Mohr, C., Michel, C. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Brugger, P., Seeck, M., Landis, T., and Thut, G. (2005). Linking out-of-body experience and self processing to mental own-body imagery at the temporoparietal junction. J. Neurosci. 25, 550–557. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2612-04.2005
Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T., and Seeck, M. (2002). Neuropsychology: stimulating illusory own-body perceptions. Nature 419, 269–270. doi: 10.1038/419269a
Bos, E. M., Spoor, J. K. H., Smits, M., Schouten, J. W., and Vincent, A. J. P. E. (2016). Out-of-body experience during awake craniotomy. World Neurosurg. 92, 586.e9–586.e13. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2016.05.002
Bova, M. (2011). Recollections of Jimmy's out-of-body experiences. NeuroQuantology 9, 526–529. doi: 10.14704/nq.2011.9.3.461
Brandt, C., Kramme, C., Storm, H., and Pohlmann-Eden, B. (2009). Out-of-body experience and auditory and visual hallucinations in a patient with cardiogenic syncope: Crucial role of cardiac event recorder in establishing the diagnosis. Epilepsy Behav. 15, 254–255. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.02.047
Brumblay R. (2003). Hyperdimensional perspectives in out-of-body and near-death experiences. J. Near-Death Stud. 21, 201–221. doi: 10.1023/A:1024054029979
Campillo-Ferrer, T., Alcaraz-Sánchez, A., Demsar, E., Wu, H.-P., Dresler, M., Windt, J., and Blanke, O. (2024). Out-of-body experiences in relation to lucid dreaming and sleep paralysis: a theoretical review and conceptual model. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 163:105770. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105770
Carruthers, G. (2018). Confabulation or experience? Implications of out-of-body experiences for theories of consciousness. Theory Psychol. 28, 122–140. doi: 10.1177/0959354317745590
Chirico, A., Pizzolante, M., Kitson, A., Gianotti, E., Riecke, B. E., and Gaggioli, A. (2022). Defining transformative experiences: a conceptual analysis. Front. Psychol. 13:790300. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.790300
De Foe, A. (2012). How should therapists respond to client accounts of out-of-body experience? Int. J. Trans. Stud. 31, 75–82. doi: 10.24972/ijts.2012.31.1.75
De Foe, A., Van Doorn, G., and Symmons, M. (2013). Floating sensations prior to sleep and out-of-body experiences. J. Parapsychol. 77, 271–280.
De Ridder, D., Van Laere, K., Dupont, P., Menovsky, T., and Van de Heyning, P. (2007). Visualizing out-of-body experience in the brain. N. Engl. J. Med. 357, 1829–1833. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa070010
Facco, E., Casiglia, E., Al Khafaji, B. E., Finatti, F., Duma, G. M. L., Mento, G., et al. (2019). Neurophenomenology of out-of-body experiences induced by hypnotic suggestions. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 67, 1–30. doi: 10.1080/00207144.2019.1553762
Fang, T., Yan, R., and Fang, F. (2014). Spontaneous out-of-body experience in a child with refractory right temporoparietal epilepsy: case report. J. Neurosurg. 14, 396–399. doi: 10.3171/2014.6.PEDS13485
Gabbard, G.O., and Twemlow, S.W. (1984). With the Eyes of the Mind: An Empirical Analysis of Out-Of-Body States. New York: Praeger.
Gallo, F. T., Spiousas, I., Herrero, N. L., Godoy, D., Tommasel, A., Gasca-Rolin, M., Ramele, R., Gleiser, P. M., and Forcato, C. (2023). Structural differences between non-lucid dreams, lucid dreams and out-of-body experience reports assessed by graph analysis. Sci. Rep. 13:19579. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-46817-2
Gow, K., Lang, T., and Chant, D. (2004). Fantasy proneness, paranormal beliefs and personality features in out-of-body experiences. Contemp. Hypn. 21, 107–125. doi: 10.1002/ch.296
Hameroff, S., and Penrose, R. (2014). Consciousness in the universe: a review of the 'Orch OR' theory. Phys. Life Rev. 11, 39–78. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2013.08.002
Husserl, E. (1931). Ideas: General Introduction to Pure Phenomenology. Translated by W. R. Boyce Gibson. London: George Allen and Unwin.
Irwin, H. (2000). The disembodied self: an empirical study of dissociation and the out-of-body experience. J. Parapsychol. 64, 261–277.
Irwin, H. J. (1985). Flight of Mind: A Psychological Study of the Out-Of-Body Experience. Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow Press.
Irwin, H. J. (1988). Out-of-body experiences and attitudes to life and death. J. Am. Soc. Psychical Res. 82, 237–251.
Klemenc-Ketis, Z. (2013). Life changes in patients after out-of-hospital cardiac arrest: the effect of near-death experiences. Int. J. Behav. Med. 20, 7–12. doi: 10.1007/s12529-011-9209-y
Lopez, C., and Elzière, M. (2018). Out-of-body experience in vestibular disorders: a prospective study of 210 patients with dizziness. Cortex 104, 193–206. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.05.026
Metzinger, T. (2005). Out-of-body experiences as the origin of the concept of a “soul”. Mind Matter, 3, 57–84.
Mudgal, V., Dhakad, R., Mathur, R., Sardesai, U., and Pal, V. (2021). Astral projection: a strange out-of-body experience in dissociative disorder. Cureus 13:e17037. doi: 10.7759/cureus.17037
Murray, C. D., and Fox, J. (2005). Dissociational body experiences: differences between respondents with and without prior out-of-body-experiences. Br. J. Psychol. 96, 441–456. doi: 10.1348/000712605X49169
Neppe, V. M. (2011). Models of the out-of-body experience: a new multi-etiological phenomenological approach. NeuroQuantology 9, 72–83. doi: 10.14704/nq.2011.9.1.391
O'Brien, B. J., Harris, I. B., Beckman, T. J., Reed, D. A., and Cook, D. A. (2014). Standards for reporting qualitative research: a synthesis of recommendations. Acad. Med. 89, 1245–1251. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0000000000000388
Osis, K. (1979). “Insiders' views of the OBE: a questionnaire survey,” in Research in Parapsychology, ed. W. G. Roll (Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow Press), 50–52.
Pace, J. C., and Drumm, D. L. (1992). The phantom leaf effect and its implications for near-death and out-of-body experiences. J. Near-Death Stud. 10, 233–240. doi: 10.1007/BF01074166
Palmer, J. (1979). A community mail survey of psychic experiences. J. Am. Soc. Psychical Res. 73, 221–251.
Parra, A. (2008). Out-of-body experiences and hallucinatory experiences: relationship with cognitive and perceptual variables. Liberabit 14, 5–14.
Parra, A. (2010). Out-of-body experiences and hallucinatory experiences: a psychological approach. Imagination Cogn. Pers. 29, 211–223. doi: 10.2190/IC.29.3.d
Parra, A. (2018). Out of body experiences: an evaluation of the construct of transliminality and “thin” boundaries as cognitive-perceptual anomaly. Psicol. Conocimiento Soc. 8, 100–116.
Persinger, M. A. (2010). The harribance effect as pervasive out-of-body experiences: NEUROQUANTAL evidence with more precise measurements. NeuroQuantology, 8, 444–465. doi: 10.14704/nq.2010.8.4.301
Rabeyron, T., and Caussié, S. (2016). Clinique des sorties hors du corps: trauma, réflexivité et symbolisation, L'Evolution Psychiatrique 81, 755–775. doi: 10.1016/j.evopsy.2015.10.007
Raduga, M., Kuyava, O., and Sevcenko, N. (2020). Is there a relationship between REM sleep dissociated phenomena, like lucid dreaming, sleep paralysis, out-of-body experiences, and false awakening? Med. Hypotheses, 144:110169. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2020.110169
Ring, K., and Cooper, S. (1999). Mindsight: Near-Death and Out-of-Body Experiences in the Blind. Palo Alto, CA: William James Center for Consciousness Studies.
Roisin, J. (2009). La sortie du corps et autres expériences extrêmes en situation de traumatisme. Rev. Francophone Stress Trauma 9, 71–79.
Sellers, J. (2017). Out-of-body experience: Review and a case study. J. Conscious. Explor. Res. 8, 686–708.
Sellers, J. (2018). A brief review of studies of out-of-body experiences in both the healthy and pathological populations. J. Cogn. Sci. 19, 471–491. doi: 10.17791/jcs.2018.19.4.471
Shaw, J., Gandy, S., and Stumbrys, T. (2023). Transformative effects of spontaneous out of body experiences in healthy individuals: An interpretative phenomenological analysis. Psychol. Conscious.: Theory Res. Pract. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/cns0000324
Smith, A. M., and Messier, C. (2014). Voluntary out-of-body experience: an fMRI study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00070
Smith, J. A., Flowers, P., and Larkin, M. (2009). Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis: Theory, Method and Research. London: SAGE.
Terhune, D. B. (2006). Dissociative alterations in body image among individuals reporting out-of-body experiences: a conceptual replication. Percept. Motor Skills 103, 76–80. doi: 10.2466/pms.103.1.76-80
Thakkar, K. N., Nichols, H. S., McIntosh, L. G., and Park, S. (2011). Disturbances in body ownership in schizophrenia: evidence from the rubber hand illusion and case study of a spontaneous out-of-body experience. PLoS ONE 6:e27089. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027089
Tononi, G., and Koch, C. (2015). Consciousness: here, there and everywhere? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 370, 1–18. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0167
Tressoldi, P. E., Pederzoli, L., Caini, P., Ferrini, A., Melloni, S., Prati, E., et al. (2015). Hypnotically induced out-of-body experience: How many bodies are there? Unexpected discoveries about the subtle body and psychic body. Sage Open 5. doi: 10.1177/2158244015615919
Twemlow, S. W. (1989). Clinical approaches to the out-of-body experience. J. Near-Death Stud. 8, 29–43. doi: 10.1007/BF01076137
Van Lommel, P., Wees, R., Meyers, V., and Elfferich, I. (2001). Near-death experience in survivors of cardiac arrest: aprospective study in the Netherlands. Lancet 358, 2039–2045. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(01)07100-8
Weiler, M., and Acunzo, D. J. (2024). What out-of-body experiences may tell us about the mind beyond the brain. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 110. doi: 10.1080/09540261.2024.2436598
Wilde, D., and Murray, C. D. (2009). An interpretive phenomenological analysis of out-of-body experiences in two cases of novice meditators. Aust. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 37, 90–118.
Wilde, D., and Murray, C. D. (2010). Interpreting the anomalous: finding meaning in out-of-body and near-death experiences. Qual. Res. Psychol. 7, 57–72. doi: 10.1080/14780880903304550
Wilkins, L. K., Girard, T. A., and Cheyne, J. A. (2011). Ketamine as a primary predictor of out-of-body experiences associated with multiple substance use. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 943–950. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.01.005
Wu, H. P., Nakul, E., Betka, S., Lance, F., Herbelin, B., and Blanke, O. (2023). Out-of-body illusion induced by visual-vestibular stimulation. iScience 27:108547. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2023.108547
Yu, K., Liu, C., Yu, T., Wang, X., Xu, C., Ni, D., et al. (2018). Out-of-body experience in the anterior insular cortex during the intracranial electrodes' stimulation in an epileptic child. J. Clin. Neurosci. 54, 122–125. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2018.04.050
Keywords: out-of-body experiences, expanded consciousness, non-local consciousness, phenomenological analysis, experiential interpretation, qualitative research
Citation: Moix J, Nieto I and De la Rua AY (2025) Out-of-body experiences: interpretations through the eyes of those who live them. Front. Psychol. 16:1566679. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1566679
Received: 25 January 2025; Accepted: 25 March 2025;
Published: 10 April 2025.
Edited by:
Andreas Kalckert, University of Skövde, SwedenReviewed by:
Patrizio E. Tressoldi, University of Padua, ItalyXimena González-Grandon, Ibero American University, Mexico
Copyright © 2025 Moix, Nieto and De la Rua. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Jenny Moix, amVubnkubW9peEB1YWIuY2F0