OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 13 June 2025

Sec. Cognitive Science

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1593269

Unwilling or unable? Interpreting effort task performance in myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome

  • 1Independent scholar, Boston, MA, United States
  • 2Independent scholar, Dublin, Ireland
  • 3Independent scholar, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 4Department of Medicine, University College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 5Department of Psychology, Reed College, Portland, OR, United States

Introduction

In a recent, high-profile study of post-infectious myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome (PI-ME/CFS), Walitt et al. (2024) assessed the performance of patients and healthy volunteers on the Effort-Expenditure for Rewards Task (EEfRT), among a host of other measures. The EEfRT is a widely used behavioral index of reward motivation and effort-based decision-making that requires repeatedly choosing between an easy task and a hard task, each involving rapid, repetitive button-pressing (Treadway et al., 2009). Walitt et al.'s study—the first to investigate effort-based decision-making in PI-ME/CFS—found that patients were less likely to choose the hard task than healthy volunteers. The authors interpreted this difference as evidence of altered “effort preference,” which they defined as “how much effort a person subjectively wants to exert” (p. 9). Walitt et al. concluded that “effort preference, not fatigue, is the defining motor behavior of this illness” (p. 10). Here we interrogate this conclusion. Were PI-ME/CFS patients less likely to choose the hard task because they wanted to exert less effort, consciously or otherwise? Or were they less able to complete the hard task, and thus chose it less often? We argue that the data support the latter interpretation.

Accounting for ability in the EEfRT

For the EEfRT to yield interpretable results, participants' choices between the easy and hard tasks must be decoupled from their ability to complete these tasks. As the developers of the measure cautioned, “an important requirement for the EEfRT is that it measure individual differences in motivation for rewards, rather than individual differences in ability or fatigue” (Treadway et al., 2009, p. 4). Several techniques have been established to satisfy this requirement. In an initial validation study with a healthy student sample, Treadway et al. (2009) ruled out differences in both ability and within-task fatigue through two manipulation checks: confirmation of ceiling-level trial completion rates for all participants (96–100%) and the inclusion of trial number in statistical models. Subsequent studies have prospectively assessed and statistically controlled for motor skills, which have been shown to influence effort-based decision-making (Ohmann et al., 2020, 2022). In studies of schizophrenia, where patients have exhibited lower maximum button press rates than controls, the required number of button presses is often calibrated to individual ability levels (e.g., Cooper et al., 2019; Fervaha et al., 2013; Le et al., 2023; Reddy et al., 2015). This method helps fulfill the prerequisite, affirmed by the EEfRT developers, that “all subjects [are] readily able to complete both the hard and easy tasks throughout the experiment” (Treadway et al., 2009, p. 4; emphasis added).

Walitt et al.'s interpretation: preference for avoiding effort

Despite the importance of ensuring that decision-making in the EEfRT cannot be explained by ability or fatigue, Walitt et al. (2024) only ruled out fatigue.1 In their study, 15 PI-ME/CFS patients and 16 healthy controls chose between the easy and hard tasks an average of 46 times during the 15-minute testing session. The easy task required 30 button presses in 7 seconds with the dominant index finger, while the hard task required 98 button presses in 21 seconds with the non-dominant little finger.2 Whereas the EEfRT is typically regarded as a measure of reward motivation, effort-based decision-making, or effort allocation (Cooper et al., 2019; Fervaha et al., 2013; Le et al., 2023; Ohmann et al., 2020, 2022; Reddy et al., 2015; Treadway et al., 2009), Walitt et al. characterized it as assessing the new construct of “effort preference, the decision to avoid the harder task when decision-making is unsupervised and reward values and probabilities of receiving a reward are standardized” (p. 2). Effort preference was operationalized as the Proportion of Hard Task Choices (PHTC). Compared to the PI-ME/CFS patients, the controls were 1.65 times more likely to choose the hard task (PHTC) and, upon choosing it, were 27.23 times more likely to successfully complete it. Walitt et al. interpreted the difference in PHTC as indicating that people with PI-ME/CFS prefer to avoid effort, ignoring the substantially larger—and more fundamental—difference in the ability of patients and controls to complete the hard task successfully.

Our interpretation: limited ability to execute the task

Our own analysis of Walitt et al.'s (2024) data, accessed via mapMECFS (Mathur et al., 2021), confirmed the large difference in ability between the groups (analysis code available at https://osf.io/vqzca/). PI-ME/CFS patients were only able to complete an average of 65% of the hard tasks they chose (SD = 37%), compared to 96% (SD = 8%) for controls, two-sided Mann-Whitney U: p = 0.01, r = 0.45 (non-parametric analyses were used to account for extreme negative skew in completion rates). This constitutes a failure of Treadway et al.'s (2009) manipulation check for differences in ability. Moreover, as shown in Figure 1, seven of the 15 PI-ME/CFS patients had lower hard task completion rates than any control, successfully completing only 30% of the hard tasks they chose (SD = 23%). These results show that the hard task was simply too hard for many PI-ME/CFS patients. By contrast, both groups completed the easy task at near-ceiling rates (PI-ME/CFS: M = 98%, SD = 4%; controls: M = 99%, SD = 2%), two-sided Mann-Whitney U: p = 0.63.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Positive correlation between the proportion of hard tasks completed successfully and the proportion of hard task choices (PHTC/“effort preference”). This correlation suggests that effort preference was confounded with participants' ability to complete the hard task (healthy volunteers: blue, n = 16; PI-ME/CFS patients: red, n = 15). The hard task completion rates (x-axis) indicate that this task was too hard for many PI-ME/CFS patients: seven of them completed it at lower rates (M = 0.3) than any healthy volunteer (lowest rate = 0.7).

The stark contrast in performance on the two tasks suggests that, for the PI-ME/CFS patients, choosing the hard task over the easy one (PHTC) may have been more a matter of ability than preference. Consistent with this possibility, we found that PHTC was positively correlated with the proportion of hard tasks completed successfully, rs(29) = 0.38, p = 0.03 (see Figure 1). These results suggest that PHTC was confounded with ability in Walitt et al.'s (2024) study: participants who had more difficulty completing the hard task chose it less often. Because difficulty with the hard task disproportionately affected the PI-ME/CFS group, the large difference in hard task completion rates could explain the comparatively small difference in PHTC between groups. Therefore, we interpret Walitt et al.'s data as showing that people with PI-ME/CFS were less able to execute the EEfRT's hard task, rather than unwilling to expend effort.

To some, it might seem implausible that those with PI-ME/CFS would be unable to complete a rapid repetitive button-pressing task despite expending effort. Yet there are at least two reasons why this finding is unsurprising. First, the hard task on the EEfRT was designed to be challenging but achievable for healthy people (Treadway et al., 2009). ME/CFS patients, however, report greater physical limitations than healthy people and those with other disabling illnesses on the Short-Form 36 (SF-36) measure of health-related quality of life, including its physical function and role-physical subscales (Komaroff et al., 1996; Nacul et al., 2011). In an analysis of Walitt et al.'s (2024) SF-36 data, we found that the PI-ME/CFS group had particularly poor physical capacity, with physical function scores (M = 28.7, SD = 21.1, n = 15) far below those of the control group (M = 97.5, SD = 4.1, n = 16; scale: 0–100), two-sided Mann-Whitney U: p < 0.001, r = 0.87. Second, these general physical limitations may be accompanied by more specific psychomotor deficits. Although Walitt et al. found no significant group differences on one measure of psychomotor ability (Grooved Pegboard Test; Kløve, 1963), their analyses were underpowered to detect even large differences (e.g., d = 0.8: 60% power). Indeed, studies using several other measures point to motor speed impairments in ME/CFS (Majer et al., 2008; Michiels et al., 1996; Schrijvers et al., 2009), echoing patients' cognitive processing speed deficits (Lange et al., 2024; Sebaiti et al., 2022). In light of their significant physical limitations and the broader body of evidence for psychomotor deficits, it is little wonder that Walitt et al.'s PI-ME/CFS patients struggled to press a button 98 times in 21 s with the non-dominant little finger.

To distinguish willingness to expend effort from ability, several investigators have calibrated the higher-effort task on the EEfRT to individual ability levels (Cooper et al., 2019; Fervaha et al., 2013; Le et al., 2023; Reddy et al., 2015). For example, in a study of people with schizophrenia, Reddy et al. (2015) used a calibration phase to determine each participant's maximum button pressing rate, and then set that participant's hard task target to 85% of their maximum. Others have suggested that the calibration phase could itself be affected by reduced willingness to expend effort (Le Bouc et al., 2023), thus artificially lowering the target for the hard task and obscuring group differences. In an attempt to mitigate this concern in a study using a hand grip task, Le Bouc et al. (2023) set hard task targets based on participants' estimated potential arm strength rather than their performed arm strength. However, this approach is insufficient because it targets only some of the skills required for the high-effort task. Calibration should be task-specific, targeting all skills required for the task. Otherwise, group differences in any number of psychomotor skills could explain the results. For this reason, there is no way to retrospectively compensate for the lack of calibration in Walitt et al.'s (2024) study. Adequate performance on other measures does not imply that participants are readily able to complete the hard task on the EEfRT.

Conclusion

In sum, Walitt et al.'s (2024) data provide no evidence of altered effort preference in PI-ME/CFS patients, who lacked the physical ability to consistently execute the task assessing it. Conclusions about effort preference are unwarranted when group differences in ability could account for disparities in task performance. To decouple what patients are willing to do from what they are able to do, future research in ME/CFS should calibrate measures of effort-based decision-making to the ability of individual patients. The amount of effort a person wants to exert on a task is irrelevant if they are unable to exert it.

Author contributions

AK-Q: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Formal analysis, Visualization. KK: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. OJ: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. JE: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. KH: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Supervision.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. The publication fee was supported by a start-up award from Reed College to KH.

Acknowledgments

Several ME/CFS patients from the Science4ME community, who wish to remain anonymous, contributed to the conceptualization of this project. The authors, some of whom are ME/CFS patients themselves, are grateful for their valuable insights.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^Neither within-task fatigue nor reward sensitivity differed between groups.

2. ^Performance feedback was provided both during and after each trial. Upon completing the measure, participants won a monetary reward for two of their successful trials. Reward values and probabilities of receiving the reward changed from trial to trial, with higher-value rewards for the hard task.

References

Cooper, J. A., Barch, D. M., Reddy, L. F., Horan, W. P., Green, M. F., and Treadway, M. T. (2019). Effortful goal-directed behavior in schizophrenia: computational subtypes and associations with cognition. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 128, 710–722. doi: 10.1037/abn0000443

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fervaha, G., Graff-Guerrero, A., Zakzanis, K. K., Foussias, G., Agid, O., and Remington, G. (2013). Incentive motivation deficits in schizophrenia reflect effort computation impairments during cost-benefit decision-making. J. Psychiatr. Res. 47, 1590–1596. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.08.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kløve, H. (1963). Clinical neuropsychology. Med. Clin. North Am. 47, 1647–1658. doi: 10.1016/S0025-7125(16)33515-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Komaroff, A. L., Fagioli, L. R., Doolittle, T. H., Gandek, B., Gleit, M. A., Guerriero, R. T., et al. (1996). Health status in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome and in general population and disease comparison groups. Am. J. Med. 101, 281–290. doi: 10.1016/S0002-9343(96)00174-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange, G., Lin, J. S., Chen, Y., Fall, E. A., Peterson, D. L., Bateman, L., et al. (2024). Cognitive assessment in myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome (ME/CFS): a cognitive substudy of the multi-site clinical assessment of ME/CFS (MCAM). Front. Neurosci. 18:1460157. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1460157

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Bouc, R., Borderies, N., Carle, G., Robriquet, C., Vinckier, F., Daunizeau, J., et al. (2023). Effort avoidance as a core mechanism of apathy in frontotemporal dementia. Brain 146, 712–726. doi: 10.1093/brain/awac427

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Le, T. P., Green, M. F., Wynn, J. K., Iglesias, J. E., Franco, R. L., Kopelowicz, A., et al. (2023). Effort-based decision-making as a determinant of supported employment outcomes in psychotic disorders. Schizophr. Res. 262, 149–155. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2023.11.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Majer, M., Welberg, L. A. M., Capuron, L., Miller, A. H., Pagnoni, G., and Reeves, W. C. (2008). Neuropsychological performance in persons with chronic fatigue syndrome: results from a population-based study. Psychosom. Med. 70, 829–836. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31817b9793

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathur, R., Carnes, M. U., Harding, A., Moore, A., Thomas, I., Giarrocco, A., et al. (2021). mapMECFS: a portal to enhance data discovery across biological disciplines and collaborative sites. J. Transl. Med. 19:461. doi: 10.1186/s12967-021-03127-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Michiels, V., Cluydts, R., Fischler, B., Hoffmann, G., Le Bon, O., and De Meirleir, K. (1996). Cognitive functioning in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 18, 666–677. doi: 10.1080/01688639608408290

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nacul, L. C., Lacerda, E. M., Campion, P., Pheby, D., Drachler, M. D. L., Leite, J. C., et al. (2011). The functional status and well being of people with myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome and their carers. BMC Public Health 11:402. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-402

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohmann, H. A., Kuper, N., and Wacker, J. (2020). A low dosage of the dopamine D2-receptor antagonist sulpiride affects effort allocation for reward regardless of trait extraversion. Personal. Neurosci. 3:e7. doi: 10.1017/pen.2020.7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohmann, H. A., Kuper, N., and Wacker, J. (2022). Examining the reliability and validity of two versions of the effort-expenditure for rewards task (EEfRT). PLoS ONE 17:e0262902. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0262902

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reddy, L. F., Horan, W. P., Barch, D. M., Buchanan, R. W., Dunayevich, E., Gold, J. M., et al. (2015). Effort-based decision-making paradigms for clinical trials in schizophrenia: part 1—psychometric characteristics of 5 paradigms. Schizophr. Bull. 41, 1045–1054. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv089

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schrijvers, D., Van Den Eede, F., Maas, Y., Cosyns, P., Hulstijn, W., and Sabbe, B. G. C. (2009). Psychomotor functioning in chronic fatigue syndrome and major depressive disorder: a comparative study. J. Affect. Disord. 115, 46–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.08.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebaiti, M. A., Hainselin, M., Gounden, Y., Sirbu, C. A., Sekulic, S., Lorusso, L., et al. (2022). Systematic review and meta-analysis of cognitive impairment in myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome (ME/CFS). Sci. Rep. 12:2157. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-04764-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Treadway, M. T., Buckholtz, J. W., Schwartzman, A. N., Lambert, W. E., and Zald, D. H. (2009). Worth the ‘EEfRT'? The effort expenditure for rewards task as an objective measure of motivation and anhedonia. PLoS ONE 4:e6598. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006598

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walitt, B., Singh, K., LaMunion, S. R., Hallett, M., Jacobson, S., Chen, K., et al. (2024). Deep phenotyping of post-infectious myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome. Nat. Commun. 15:907. doi: 10.1038/s41467-024-45107-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: myalgic encephalomyelitis, chronic fatigue syndrome, ME/CFS, effort, effort preference, effort-based decision-making, calibration

Citation: Kirvin-Quamme A, Kirke KD, Junge O, Edwards JCW and Holmes KJ (2025) Unwilling or unable? Interpreting effort task performance in myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome. Front. Psychol. 16:1593269. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1593269

Received: 17 March 2025; Accepted: 13 May 2025;
Published: 13 June 2025.

Edited by:

Ion Juvina, Wright State University, United States

Reviewed by:

Jarean Carson, Wright State University, United States

Copyright © 2025 Kirvin-Quamme, Kirke, Junge, Edwards and Holmes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Andrew Kirvin-Quamme, YWtpcnZpbnF1YW1tZUBnbWFpbC5jb20=

ORCID: Andrew Kirvin-Quamme orcid.org/0000-0002-1836-6575
Karen D. Kirke orcid.org/0000-0003-3968-3593
Oscar Junge orcid.org/0009-0003-5367-9504
Jonathan C. W. Edwards orcid.org/0000-0002-9128-7101
Kevin J. Holmes orcid.org/0000-0003-1373-0881

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.