Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 20 August 2025

Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1603001

Exploring the mediating role of virtual environment loneliness in the link between interpersonal relationship styles and social anxiety

  • 1Department of Psychology, Faculty of Economics, Administrative and Social Sciences, Hasan Kalyoncu University, Gaziantep, Türkiye
  • 2Alcohol and Drug Treatment and Education Center, Department of Psychiatry, Ankara Training and Research Hospital, University of Health Sciences, Ankara, Türkiye

Objectives: As technology has transformed social interactions, the study investigates whether virtual communication adequately fulfills individuals’ social connection needs. This study explores the relationship between social anxiety, virtual environment loneliness, and interpersonal relationship styles among a diverse sample of 400 participants.

Methods: The study included 400 participants (N = 400), the majority of whom were female (N = 254, 63.5%) and had at least a bachelor’s degree (N = 261; 65%). Participants completed the Environment Loneliness Scale, the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale, and Interpersonal Relationships Scale. Multi-mediation analysis was run to assess the potential mediating role of virtual loneliness.

Results: Results showed that an inhibitory relationship style was positively correlated with social anxiety (r = 0.30, p < 0.01), while a nurturing relationship style was negatively correlated (r = −0.21, p < 0.01). Mediation analyses revealed that a subdimension of virtual environment loneliness, called virtual sharing, significantly mediated the relationship between inhibitory relationship style and social anxiety. However, no significant indirect effect was observed between nurturing relationship style and social anxiety.

Conclusion: In conclusion, the current study provides insight regarding the relationship between interpersonal relationship styles and social anxiety by emphasizing the role of virtual sharing in this association. Successfully managing social interactions is essential for improving individuals’ psychological well-being. Future studies should further investigate these relationships to optimize interventions for individuals struggling with social anxiety.

1 Introduction

Social anxiety is a prevalent psychological condition characterized by an intense fear of social interactions and situations in which individuals anticipate being scrutinized or negatively evaluated by others, restricting their chances of forming meaningful social ties (Morrison and Heimberg, 2013). This condition manifests in various forms, including the avoidance of social gatherings and communication with others, excessive self-consciousness, and physiological responses such as sweating or trembling (Steinert et al., 2013). It adversely impacts interpersonal relationships, disrupts academic, occupational life and daily functioning (Fehm et al., 2008), even may result in other psychiatric disorders (Copeland et al., 2011).

Overall, social anxiety and related disorders are highly prevalent in the society, and mild to extreme levels of social anxiety make people experience difficulty in social situations, retrieve them from having meaningful social interactions, experience adverse physical and psychological symptoms (Alomari et al., 2022). Given its widespread occurrence and detrimental effects on human functioning, examining the underlying mechanisms that contribute to social anxiety remains a crucial interest in psychological research. Within this scope, the current study suggested examining social anxiety from the lenses of interpersonal relationship styles and experience of loneliness in virtual environment.

1.1 Interpersonal relationship styles

While there is no definitive pathway to developing social anxiety disorder, challenges in interpersonal relationships can be a significant factor contributing to its onset (Derin et al., 2022). People have a fundamental psychological need to be related to others, which is directly linked to their psychological health (Self-Determination Theory; SDT; Deci and Ryan, 2013). That is why they need to build and maintain meaningful social relations to sustain their well-being and health (Fotiadis et al., 2019; Arslan, 2024). However, the nature of social interactions and how they are expressed can vary significantly from person to person. Perception of the self and others, the way of communication with or expression of the self to others are referred as interpersonal relationships style (Daffern et al., 2010). There are various ways of classifying interpersonal relationships styles. However, to ensure cultural sensitivity in Turkish context, classification framework proposed by Şahin and colleagues was chosen for the present study (Şahin and Durak, 1994). According to their model also (see Hasta and Güler, 2013), interpersonal relationship styles can be either nurturing/positive (characterized by openness and respect) or inhibitory/negative (dismissive, closed, and egocentric way of social interactions).

While nurturing relationships promote mutual understanding and cooperation, inhibitory styles create challenges in maintaining healthy interpersonal interactions (Grenwald and Yıldırımoğlu, 1999), which may lead to undesired outcomes. To illustrate, unlike nurturing relationship style, holding an inhibitory relationship style found to be predictive of loneliness (Şahin and Durak, 1994). Moreover, the nurturing relationship style has been linked to positive psychosocial outcomes, such as increased subjective well-being (Doğan and Sapmaz, 2012) and greater marital satisfaction (Kasapoğlu et al., 2017). In contrast, the inhibitory relationship style has been predominantly associated with negative factors, including attachment anxiety (Kaya et al., 2023) and anxiety disorders (Şahin et al., 2011a), often leading to adverse outcomes such as hopelessness and suicidal tendencies (Arsel and Batigün, 2011). Although there is an increasing interest in psychological correlates of relationship styles, there is a lack of research on interpersonal relationship styles and social anxiety relationship (Manning et al., 2017).

1.2 Virtual environment loneliness

Social interactions do not always occur in face-to-face physical environments. With the advancement of technology, the way individuals communicate and socialize has rapidly changed. Face-to-face communication has largely been replaced by messaging, voice or video calls, gaming, and social media interactions (Smith and Anderson, 2018). Particularly, social media usage presents itself as a significant phenomenon affecting daily life, with direct implications for our lives (Amichai-Hamburger and Ben-Artzi, 2003). While interactions and communications in virtual environments offer opportunities similar to those in real life, they also present limitless possibilities. However, it is important to understand whether online social interactions adequately meet individuals’ needs for social connection (Towner et al., 2022). The literature reveals conflicting results regarding what online interactions offer. Yao and Zhong (2014) discovered that as social interactions decreased, online interactions increased. Furthermore, they noted that as online addiction intensified, individuals became increasingly isolated (Yao and Zhong, 2014). Additionally, some social interactions may involve cyberbullying and encourage problematic behaviors such as anorexia or self-harm (Margherita and Gargiulo, 2018; Kowalski et al., 2019; Cosma et al., 2020), while some online support groups can be beneficial for individuals with various conditions, such as depression (Breuer and Barker, 2015). A study conducted with university students found that the perceived usefulness of online support groups was positively related to social support when participants reported moderate to high levels of social anxiety (Ruppel and McKinley, 2015).

Overall, the role of virtual interactions in interpersonal relationships and their mental health outcomes appear to be debatable. Individuals who prefer spending excessive time in virtual environments and struggle with physical presence in social settings may gradually detach from concepts such as time and responsibility. However, this situation, which drives individuals to social isolation, can also lead to various problems in the virtual world. Virtual loneliness is a concept that has emerged in recent times with the exponential increase in internet usage, which may lead to psychological and physical distress (Korkmaz et al., 2014). Nevertheless, in the virtual world, a person may experience loneliness without encountering psychological distress or problems in interpersonal relationships (Moretta and Buodo, 2020). Paradoxically, due to the opportunities it provides, the virtual world may be preferred even though individuals experience loneliness (Morahan-Martin and Schumacher, 2003). As is well known, social anxiety disorder pertains to an intense fear of being in one or more social situations where the person might feel embarrassed or humiliated because of potential judgment from others (Association, A. P, 2013). Individuals with social anxiety are more prone to encountering problems in their social relationships, experiencing feelings of loneliness, and facing difficulties with school activities or tasks (Russell and Topham, 2012; Vilaplana-Pérez et al., 2021). Problems in social relationships and feelings of loneliness lead individuals to restrict their social interactions, creating a vicious cycle that results in increased social stress and anxiety in the real world (Huan et al., 2014). Research indicates that individuals with social anxiety tend to use low-risk communication platforms (Ebeling-Witte et al., 2007), which can lead to problematic internet use (Campbell et al., 2006; Caplan, 2006; Liu and Kuo, 2007). In an example study, social anxiety fully mediated the relationship between loneliness and problematic internet use like excessive use of it over spending time in real life (Huan et al., 2014).

1.3 Present study-“the paradox of connection”

As previously discussed, humans have a fundamental need to establish and sustain meaningful and healthy social relationships. However, not all social interactions effectively fulfill this need. Different interpersonal relationship styles play a crucial role in individuals’ psychosocial well-being. Despite this, the existing literature has not sufficiently explored their relationship with social anxiety.

As previously mentioned, the nurturing relationship style supports mutual understanding and cooperation in interpersonal relationships and is associated with psychological well-being (Grenwald and Yıldırımoğlu, 1999). Researchers have found that individuals who grow up in a family environment characterized by nurturing and warmth during childhood tend to form more secure attachments in their close relationships, particularly in later life. This has been linked to the development of healthy emotion regulation mechanisms (Waldinger and Schulz, 2016). Individuals who adopt a nurturing relationship style may demonstrate more functional interpersonal behaviors in both in-person and online communication, and are likely to experience lower levels of social anxiety. In contrast, those with an inhibitory relationship style are expected to show lower functionality in these interactions.

As researchers have noted, while social interactions in the real world are declining, online communication is steadily increasing (Yao and Zhong, 2014). The rise of virtual relationships brings with it a variety of challenges. Although virtual communication has certain advantages, it can also give rise to difficulties that resemble or differ from those experienced in real-life relationships—such as exposure to cyberbullying, loneliness, and problematic internet use. For individuals who already experience social anxiety and interpersonal difficulties, the virtual world may offer new opportunities for communication but may also lead to adverse outcomes. In a recent systematic review, it was found that individuals with social anxiety tend to struggle with face-to-face interactions, while engaging more easily in online communication, seeking greater support through social media, and exhibiting more problematic patterns of internet use (O’Day and Heimberg, 2021). As individuals attempt to build relationships in virtual environments, they may paradoxically experience greater loneliness. Compared to in-person connections, virtual relationships are often weaker and more artificial, which may render them an illusion rather than a substitute for genuine social bonds.

In contemporary society, individuals increasingly seek meaningful social connections, yet often turn to digital platforms where interactions are typically more superficial and transient. Individuals with an inhibitory relationship style—characterized by emotional restraint and insecurity in attachment—may be particularly prone to initiating a pattern of social withdrawal, favoring digital communication over face-to-face interaction. For those experiencing social anxiety, this tendency is often reinforced, as the online environment presents a seemingly safer and more controllable alternative to in-person engagement. However, this shift to digital interaction, while initially reducing social discomfort, may paradoxically exacerbate feelings of loneliness over time, as virtual relationships often fail to fulfill deeper interpersonal and emotional needs. This dynamic reflects what we refer to in the present study as the paradox of connection—a phenomenon in which the pursuit of connection through digital means may ultimately intensify disconnection and perceived loneliness.

To this end, in this study, our goal was to examine the relationship between social anxiety and a previously unexplored concept—virtual environment loneliness—along with interpersonal relationship styles within a community sample. More specifically, we aim to investigate whether different dimensions of virtual environment loneliness, including virtual socialization, virtual sharing, and virtual loneliness, mediate the relationship between interpersonal relationship styles and social anxiety. Our hypotheses can be listed as follows:

H1: Nurturing relationship style is negatively associated with social anxiety.

H2: Inhibitory relationship style is positively associated with social anxiety.

H3: Virtual loneliness mediates the relationship between inhibitory relationship style and social anxiety.

2 Methods

2.1 Sample

The final sample of the study consisted of 400 adults (254 female, 146 male), ranging in age from 18 to 65 years residing in various regions of Turkey, including Kahramanmaraş, Osmaniye, İstanbul, Gaziantep, and Ankara. The survey sample consisted of 400 participants distributed across seven age groups. The largest proportion of respondents (48.3%) were aged 23–27, followed by 28–32 (19.5%) and 18–22 (14.0%). The least represented group was 43 and above (2.8%). Participants were recruited through social media platforms using a convenience sampling method. Inclusion criteria required participants to be between the ages of 18 and 65, fluent in Turkish, and capable of completing an online questionnaire independently. Individuals with self-reported diagnoses of severe psychiatric disorders (e.g., schizophrenia, bipolar disorder) were excluded to ensure data validity. Additionally, responses with extreme outliers or incomplete or inconsistent data were excluded from the final analysis.

The majority of participants were female (254; 63.5%) and held at least a bachelor’s degree (261; 65%). Regarding marital status, 65% of the participants were married, 32.8% were single, and 2% were divorced. This diverse sample allowed for a broader understanding of interpersonal relationship styles and social anxiety across a general adult population.

The procedures and purposes of the study were approved by the ethics committee of Hasan Kalyoncu University, with the decision date and number being 29.08.2024, E-97105791-050.04-64069.

2.2 Measures

Sociodemographic data form: A data form was administered to investigate participants’ age, gender, educational background, and marital status.

Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale: Developed by Liebowitz in 1987, this scale is designed to measure the level of anxiety and avoidance experienced in performance-related situations and social environments (Liebowitz, 1987). The scale comprises 24 items rated on a 4-point Likert scale and includes two subdimensions: anxiety and avoidance. Sample items include “Talking with people you do not know very well” and “Expressing a disagreement or disapproval to people you do not know very well.” Each item is rated separately for the level of anxiety and the level of avoidance it elicits. The total score ranges from 0 to 144, with higher scores indicating greater social anxiety and avoidance. The adaptation of the scale into Turkish was conducted by Soykan et al. (2003). The Cronbach alpha score was 0.97 in this study.

Virtual Environment Loneliness Scale: The scale developed by Korkmaz and colleagues to assess the level of loneliness experienced by individuals in virtual environments, as well as their sharing and socialization, is a 5-point Likert type and consists of a total of 20 items (Korkmaz et al., 2014). It consists of three subdimensions: virtual socialization, virtual loneliness, and virtual sharing. In the Virtual Socialization subdimension, along with items such as “There are people in the virtual environment whom I feel close to,” there are also reverse-coded items like “I have no friends in the virtual environment.” In the Virtual Sharing subdimension, all items are positively coded, such as “I trust my virtual friends more than my real-life friends.” In the Virtual Loneliness subdimension, there are five reverse-coded items, including statements like “Friendships in virtual environments seem fake to me.” The Cronbach alpha score in this study was 0.80 for virtual socialization relationship style, 0.87 for virtual sharing, and 0.71 for virtual loneliness.

Interpersonal Relationships Scale: This scale was developed by Şahin and Durak (1994) to measure interpersonal relationship styles. The IRS consists of 31 items and uses a 4-point Likert scale. It has two subdimensions: nurturing and inhibiting. In the Nurturing Relationship subdimension, there are positive statements related to relationships, such as “I clearly show that I value the opinions and attitudes of others.” In contrast, the Inhibiting Relationship subdimension includes negative statements like “I insist on my own opinions and do not seek compromise.” High scores on the nurturing subdimension reflect a more positive relationship style, while high scores on the inhibiting subdimension indicate a more negative relationship style. The Cronbach alpha score in this study was 0.96 for nurturing relationship style and 0.95 for inhibitory relationship style.

2.3 Data analysis plan

Before carrying out the main analyses, several preliminary analyses were conducted. These included examining the scale characteristics, analyzing the intercorrelations among the study variables, and assessing the normality assumptions by evaluating skewness and kurtosis scores, where absolute values less than |2| were considered acceptable (Kline, 2023). Then, a series of mediation analyses were run. One of them included a parallel multi-mediation analysis so as to check whether virtual socialization, virtual sharing, and virtual loneliness mediate the association between inhibitory relationship style and social anxiety. After that, a second multi-mediation analysis replicated the same model except exchanging the exogenous variable of inhibitory relationship style with nurturing relationship style. All these two mediation analyses were performed by using the PROCESS macro (Model 4) for SPSS version 3.4 (Hayes, 2017). Additionally, internal consistency reliability analyses were conducted for all scales used in the study. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficients obtained were as follows: 0.97 for the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale, 0.80–0.87 for the subdimensions of the Virtual Environment Loneliness Scale, and 0.95–0.96 for the subdimensions of the Interpersonal Relationships Scale. These values indicate high internal consistency and are consistent with prior research, supporting the reliability of the measures used.

For assessing effect sizes, Cohen’s (1988) conventional thresholds were used, categorizing effect sizes as small (0.01–0.059), moderate (0.06–0.139), or large (≥0.14). In the mediation analyses, several key statistical indicators were reported to clarify the nature and strength of the observed effects. The total effect (c path) refers to the overall relationship between the independent variable (e.g., relationship style) and the dependent variable (e.g., social anxiety) without accounting for any mediators. The direct effect (c’ path) represents the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable after controlling for the mediating variables. In contrast, the indirect effect (ab path) captures the portion of the relationship that is transmitted through one or more mediators (e.g., virtual socialization, virtual loneliness). To assess the explained variance in the dependent variable, R2 (squared multiple correlation) values were interpreted, with higher values indicating better model fit and greater predictive power. Furthermore, 95% confidence intervals (CI) for indirect effects were estimated using the bootstrap method with 5,000 resamples. When the confidence interval does not include zero, the mediation effect is considered statistically significant. This approach allows for robust estimation of mediation effects without relying on the assumption of normality (Hayes, 2017). All analyses were performed using SPSS (version 25).

3 Results

3.1 Preliminary analyses

Prior to testing the study’s main hypotheses, we analyzed the descriptive statistics, observed scale characteristics, normality assumptions, and correlations among the study variables. Initially, the normality assumptions of the variables were evaluated using skewness and kurtosis values. Within this scope, traditional threshold range between −2 and +2 was considered for deciding the assumption of normality. As displayed under Table 1, the skewness and kurtosis values for the current study variables ranged between 0.023 and 1.839, showing that the variables are normally distributed. All the descriptive analyses findings are listed under Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics results.

Preliminary analyses continued with the Pearson correlation analyses in respect to the main study variables. Regarding the results, first, inhibitory relationship style and nurturing relationship style were negatively correlated (r = −0.36, p < 0.001). While inhibitory relationship was positively related to social anxiety (r = 0.30, p < 0.001), nurturing one had a negative relationship with it (r = −0.21, p < 0.001). Considering the virtual environment loneliness subdimensions, social anxiety had only significant relationship with virtual sharing (r = −0.28, p < 0.001), but not the remaining subdimensions (p > 0.05).

Furthermore, inhibitory relationship style was significantly associated with virtual sharing (r = 0.20, p < 0.001), as well. On the other hand, nurturing relationship style was not significantly correlated with virtual sharing (p > 0.05), but it had significant associations with both virtual socialization (r = 0.27, p < 0.001) and virtual loneliness (r = −0.22, p < 0.001). Finally, some significant relationships were also observed among these virtual environment loneliness subdimensions. To illustrate, virtual socialization was positively related to both virtual sharing (r = 0.32, p < 0.001) and virtual loneliness (r = 0.14, p < 0.001). However, no significant relationship was captured between virtual sharing and virtual loneliness (p > 0.05). All these correlational findings were displayed under Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlation analysis findings (N = 400).

3.2 Mediation analyses

In order to examine the potential mediating roles of virtual socialization, virtual sharing, and virtual loneliness in the relationship between the two relationship styles (inhibitory vs. nurturing) and social anxiety, two parallel multi-mediation models were tested for each relationship styles. First mediation analysis results regarding the inhibitory relationship style revealed that it significantly predicted virtual sharing (β = 0.20, p < 0.001) and social anxiety (β = 0.24, p < 0.001), but not virtual socialization and virtual loneliness (p > 0.05). Moreover, along with inhibitory relationship style, both virtual sharing (β = 0.27, p < 0.001) and virtual socialization (β = −0.14, p < 0.01) emerged as significant predictors of social anxiety. Moving to the indirect effects, solely virtual sharing mediated the relationship between inhibitory relationship style and social anxiety (β = 0.05, 95% CI = 0.023, 0.089). That is to say that having inhibitory relationship style is related to increased virtual sharing, which in return is correlated with higher social anxiety. All these results are provided under Table 3 and Figure 1.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Standardized coefficients for the first mediation model (Inhibitory relationship style).

Figure 1
Diagram showing the inhibitory relationship style, social anxiety, virtual socialization, virtual sharing, and virtual loneliness. Arrows denote correlations: inhibitory relationship style significantly predicted virtual sharing (β = 0.20) and social anxiety (β = 0.24), but not virtual socialization or virtual loneliness. In addition, virtual sharing (β = 0.27) positively and virtual socialization (β = −0.14) negatively predicted social anxiety. The indirect effect was significant only for virtual sharing (β = 0.05).

Figure 1. Standardized regression effects of the first mediation analysis.

Same mediation analysis was replicated by replacing inhibitory relationship style with nurturing relationship style. This second round mediation analysis results showed that nurturing relationship style positively predicted virtual socialization (β = 0.27, p < 0.001), but it negatively predicted virtual loneliness (β = −0.22, p < 0.001) and social anxiety (β = −0.21, p < 0.001), but not virtual sharing (p > 0.05). In addition, virtual sharing was the only significant predictor of social anxiety among the virtual environment loneliness indicators (β = 0.31, p < 0.001). In terms of the indirect effects, none of hypotheses were supported. All these findings are displayed under Table 4 and Figure 2.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Standardized coefficients for the second mediation model (Nurturing relationship style).

Figure 2
Diagram illustrating the relationship between nurturing relationship style, virtual socialization, virtual sharing, virtual loneliness, and social anxiety. Arrows denote correlations: nurturing relationship style positively predicted virtual socialization (β = 0.27), and negatively predicted virtual loneliness (β = −0.22) and social anxiety (β = −0.21), but not virtual sharing. Among the virtual environment loneliness indicators, only virtual sharing (β = 0.31) significantly predicted social anxiety. No significant indirect effects were found.

Figure 2. Standardized regression effects of the second mediation analysis.

4 Discussion

The aim of this study was to examine the relationships between social anxiety, virtual environment loneliness, and interpersonal relationship styles. The sample encompassed a wide age range, from adolescents to older adults, with the majority of participants being female (63.5%) and having a high level of education (65%). This context is significant for understanding the effects on social interactions. Women are known to be more sensitive in social relationships and more active in seeking social support (Taylor et al., 2000). Additionally, individuals with higher education levels typically have better access to resources and information, enabling them to manage stress more effectively. This context allows for a broader perspective on the evaluation of the study’s results.

The findings indicated a positive correlation between the inhibitory relationship style and social anxiety, while the nurturing relationship style showed a negative correlation with it. This supports the idea related to close connection between social anxiety and inhibitory relationship style. That is to say that having negative interpersonal relationship style is related to adverse mental health outcomes, as also highlighted by previous research (Şahin et al., 2011b; Doğan and Sapmaz, 2012).

From a theoretical standpoint, the association between inhibitory relationship style and social anxiety can be interpreted through the lens of Attachment Theory (Bowlby, 1969) and the Cognitive-Behavioral Model of social anxiety (Bowlby, 1969; Clark and Wells, 1995). According to Bowlby, early interactions with caregivers shape internal working models of the self and others, influencing later interpersonal behaviors and emotional regulation. Individuals with an inhibitory relationship style often exhibit patterns consistent with insecure attachment, particularly avoidant attachment, characterized by discomfort with closeness and emotional intimacy. These early attachment insecurities contribute to the development of negative self-beliefs, heightened sensitivity to social evaluation, and hypervigilance to rejection—all central components in Clark and Well (1995) cognitive-behavioral model of social anxiety.

Inhibitory individuals tend to expect rejection or criticism, leading them to avoid emotional disclosure and suppress expressions of vulnerability. This avoidance limits opportunities to build trust and receive positive social feedback, thus perpetuating feelings of isolation and anxiety in social contexts. The interplay between these cognitive and attachment-related factors creates a feedback loop that sustains social anxiety symptoms, as individuals may continue to withdraw to protect themselves from anticipated social threat.

Inhibitory style appears to damage health acquisition of relatedness need and social support, and may lead to adverse outcomes like social anxiety. This style may encourage avoidance behaviors and emotional withdrawal, limiting opportunities for positive social feedback and secure attachments. Consequently, a vicious cycle can develop between this dysfunctional relationship style and mental health problems. Then, even it might form a vicious circle between this dysfunctional relationship style and mental health problems. For instance, Russell and Topham (2012) noted that individuals with social anxiety face difficulties in social relationships, which can increase emotional isolation (Russell and Topham, 2012). Inhibitory individuals may rely more heavily on virtual interactions as a compensatory strategy, but these interactions often lack the richness and emotional attunement needed to satisfy fundamental social needs, thus potentially exacerbating anxiety symptoms. Conversely, the negative correlation between the nurturing relationship style and social anxiety relates to individuals’ ability to seek social support and cope with stress (Cohen and Wills, 1985).

Our mediation analyses highlighted the significant mediating role of specific virtual environment loneliness dimension, namely virtual sharing, in the relationship between inhibitory relationship style and social anxiety. The results showed that increase in the inhibitory relationship style was associated with greater virtual sharing, which, in turn, heightened social anxiety. This finding aligns with the work of Huan et al. (2014), suggesting that social anxiety can limit individuals’ interactions in virtual environments, potentially leading to problematic internet use (Huan et al., 2014). Thus, individuals with an inhibitory relationship style may engage in more sharing in virtual spaces as a form of “digital escape,” which can exacerbate their social anxiety. This finding supports existing literature emphasizing the close link between interactions in virtual settings and social anxiety (Caplan, 2006). We elaborate that this result might reflect a maladaptive compensatory strategy. Individuals with an inhibitory style may avoid in-person social risks and instead rely on virtual spaces, which lack the nonverbal feedback and emotional attunement necessary for building secure social bonds. This overreliance on digital interaction may paradoxically intensify social anxiety, as it perpetuates avoidance and fails to provide corrective interpersonal experiences.

Although the nurturing style was associated with lower social anxiety and increased virtual socialization, it did not exert a significant mediation effect via virtual sharing. One possible explanation is that individuals with a nurturing style derive emotional security primarily from direct social interactions rather than virtual sharing, which may be perceived as superficial or less fulfilling. Therefore, their social anxiety may be more directly alleviated through high-quality offline relationships, reducing the relevance of virtual sharing as a mediating pathway.

On the other hand, the nurturing relationship style was found to increase virtual socialization while decreasing virtual loneliness, demonstrating its positive impact on forming social connections. The increase in virtual socialization helps alleviate feelings of loneliness, thereby reducing levels of social anxiety. Morahan-Martin and Schumacher (2003) point out that virtual interactions offer opportunities for developing healthy social connections, suggesting that individuals with a nurturing relationship style are more likely to establish meaningful interactions in virtual environments (Morahan-Martin and Schumacher, 2003).

The mediating role of virtual sharing in the relationships between inhibitory relationship style and social anxiety underscores the critical importance of managing virtual interactions and developing healthy social connections for individuals with social anxiety. Future research could further explore the role of virtual environment loneliness in these dynamics, contributing to the development of more effective coping strategies for social anxiety.

In conclusion, this study provides important insights into the complex interactions between social anxiety, virtual loneliness, and interpersonal relationship styles, while emphasizing the mediating effect of virtual sharing in these relationships. Effectively managing social interactions is crucial for enhancing individuals’ psychological well-being.

In light of our findings, intervention efforts targeting individuals with inhibitory relationship styles might benefit from approaches such as social skills training, assertiveness training, and attachment-based group therapies (Spence, 2003; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2010; Speed et al., 2018). These interventions can help reduce avoidance behaviors, foster secure emotional expression, and promote healthier patterns of social engagement. Emotion regulation training, particularly in recognizing and expressing interpersonal needs, may also buffer against the development or worsening of social anxiety symptoms (Berking et al., 2008).

4.1 Limitations

Despite the contributions of the current findings, the present study is not without limitations. First, the cross-sectional design prevents any causal inferences regarding the relationships between interpersonal relationship styles, virtual environment loneliness, and social anxiety. Future research would benefit from longitudinal or experimental designs to establish the directionality of these associations. Second, the study relies on self-report measures, which may be subject to social desirability bias or inaccuracies in participants’ self-perceptions. Incorporating behavioral assessments or multi-informant reports could enhance the validity of findings. Third, although the sample includes participants from various age groups and regions, it is not fully representative, as approximately two-thirds of the participants were women and the age distribution was broad. We did not statistically control for gender or age in the current mediation models, which may have influenced the findings. Future studies should consider including these variables as covariates to better understand their potential effects. Finally, while the sample provides valuable insights, its generalizability may still be limited. Future studies should aim for more diverse and representative samples in terms of age, gender, cultural background, and digital engagement levels.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the ethics committee of Hasan Kalyoncu University, with the decision date and number being 29.08.2024, E-97105791-050.04-64069. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MK: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. KS: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1603001/full#supplementary-material

References

Alomari, N. A., Bedaiwi, S. K., Ghasib, A. M., Kabbarah, A. J., Alnefaie, S. A., Hariri, N., et al. (2022). Social anxiety disorder: associated conditions and therapeutic approaches. Cureus 14:e32687. doi: 10.7759/cureus.32687

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Amichai-Hamburger, Y., and Ben-Artzi, E. (2003). Loneliness and internet use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 19, 71–80. doi: 10.1016/S0747-5632(02)00014-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arsel, C. O., and Batigün, A. D. (2011). Intihar ve Cinsiyet: Cinsiyet Rolleri, Iletisim Becerleri, Sosyal Destek ve Umutsuzluk Açisindan Bir Degerlendirme/suicide and gender: an investigation in terms of gender roles, interpersonal relationship style, social support, and hopelessness. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi 26, 1–10.

Google Scholar

Arslan, G. (2024). Unlocking the power of self-compassion and psychological flex-ibility: enhancing emotional health, subjective wellbeing, and quality of life in college students. Stud. Psychol. (Bratisl.). 66, 50–65. doi: 10.31577/sp.2024.01.890

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Association, A. P (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Google Scholar

Berking, M., Wupperman, P., Reichardt, A., Pejic, T., Dippel, A., and Znoj, H. (2008). Emotion-regulation skills as a treatment target in psychotherapy. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 1230–1237. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.08.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. London: Random House.

Google Scholar

Breuer, L., and Barker, C. (2015). Online support groups for depression: benefits and barriers. SAGE Open 5:2158244015574936. doi: 10.1177/2158244015574936

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, A. J., Cumming, S. R., and Hughes, I. (2006). Internet use by the socially fearful: addiction or therapy? CyberPsychol. Behav. 9, 69–81. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2006.9.69

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caplan, S. E. (2006). Relations among loneliness, social anxiety, and problematic internet use. CyberPsychol. Behav. 10, 234–242. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2006.9963

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, D. M., and Wells, A. (1995). “A cognitive model of social phobia” in Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment and treatment. eds. R. G. Heimberg, M. R. Liebowitz, and D. S. Hope (New York: The Guilford Press), 69–93.

Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Cohen, S., and Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol. Bull. 98, 310–357.

Google Scholar

Copeland, W., Shanahan, L., Costello, E. J., and Angold, A. (2011). Cumulative prevalence of psychiatric disorders by young adulthood: a prospective cohort analysis from the Great Smoky Mountains study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 50, 252–261. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.12.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cosma, A., Walsh, S. D., Chester, K. L., Callaghan, M., Molcho, M., Craig, W., et al. (2020). Bullying victimization: time trends and the overlap between traditional and cyberbullying across countries in Europe and North America. Int. J. Public Health 65, 75–85. doi: 10.1007/s00038-019-01320-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Daffern, M., Thomas, S., Ferguson, M., Podubinski, T., Hollander, Y., Kulkhani, J., et al. (2010). The impact of psychiatric symptoms, interpersonal style, and coercion on aggression and self-harm during psychiatric hospitalization. Psychiatry 73, 365–381. doi: 10.1521/psyc.2010.73.4.365

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2013). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York, NY: Springer Science & Business Media.

Google Scholar

Derin, S., Selman, S. B., Alyanak, B., and Soylu, N. (2022). The role of adverse childhood experiences and attachment styles in social anxiety disorder in adolescents. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 27, 644–657. doi: 10.1177/13591045221078085

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doğan, T., and Sapmaz, F. (2012). Kişiler arasi ilişki tarzlari ve öznel iyi oluş. Türk Eğitim Bilimleri Dergisi 10, 585–602.

Google Scholar

Ebeling-Witte, S., Frank, M. L., and Lester, D. (2007). Shyness, internet use, and personality. CyberPsychol. Behav. 10, 713–716. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.9964

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fehm, L., Beesdo, K., Jacobi, F., and Fiedler, A. (2008). Social anxiety disorder above and below the diagnostic threshold: prevalence, comorbidity and impairment in the general population. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 43, 257–265. doi: 10.1007/s00127-007-0299-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fotiadis, A., Abdulrahman, K., and Spyridou, A. (2019). The mediating roles of psychological autonomy, competence and relatedness on work-life balance and well-being. Front. Psychol. 10:1267. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01267

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grenwald, J., and Yıldırımoğlu, Z. (1999). Bağımlılık mı? bağlılık mı?: zehirli ilşkilerden besleyici ilişkilere. İstanbul: Kuraldışı Yayıncılık.

Google Scholar

Hasta, D., and Güler, M. E. (2013). Saldırganlık: kişilerarası ilişki tarzları ve empati açısından bir inceleme. Ankara Üniv. Sos. Bilim. Derg. 4, 64–104. doi: 10.1501/sbeder_0000000051

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Huan, V. S., Ang, R. P., and Chye, S. (2014). Loneliness and shyness in adolescent problematic internet users: the role of social anxiety. Child Youth Care Forum. 43, 539–551. doi: 10.1007/s10566-014-9252-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasapoğlu, F., Kutlu, M., and Durmuş, E. (2017). Evli bireylerde kişilerarası ilişki tarzı ile evlilik doyumu arasındaki ilişki: evlilikte özyeterlik algısının aracı rolü. Nesne Psikoloji Dergisi (NPD) 5, 469–487. doi: 10.7816/nesne-05-11-07

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaya, F., Odacı, H., and Yazıcı Çelebi, G. (2023). Attachment anxiety and interpersonal relationship styles: the mediating role of interpersonal cognitive distortions. Int. J. Cogn. Ther. 16, 320–339. doi: 10.1007/s41811-023-00166-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2023). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (5th ed.). New York, NY: Guilford publications.

Google Scholar

Korkmaz, Ö., Usta, E., and Kurt, İ. (2014). Sanal Ortam Yalnızlık Ölçeği (SOYÖ) geçerlik ve güvenirlik çalışması. Hacettepe Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi 29, 144–159.

Google Scholar

Kowalski, R. M., Limber, S. P., and McCord, A. (2019). A developmental approach to cyberbullying: prevalence and protective factors. Aggress. Violent Behav. 45, 20–32. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2018.02.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liebowitz, M. R. (1987). Social phobia. Mod. Probl. Pharmacopsychiatry 22, 141–173.

Google Scholar

Liu, C.-Y., and Kuo, F.-Y. (2007). A study of internet addiction through the lens of the interpersonal theory. CyberPsychol. Behav. 10, 799–804. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.9951

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, R. P., Dickson, J. M., Palmier-Claus, J., Cunliffe, A., and Taylor, P. J. (2017). A systematic review of adult attachment and social anxiety. J. Affect. Disord. 211, 44–59. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.12.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Margherita, G., and Gargiulo, A. (2018). A comparison between pro-anorexia and non-suicidal self-injury blogs: from symptom-based identity to sharing of emotions. Psychodyn. Pract. 24, 346–363. doi: 10.1080/14753634.2018.1535326

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikulincer, M., and Shaver, P. R. (2010). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Morahan-Martin, J., and Schumacher, P. (2003). Loneliness and social uses of the internet. Comput. Hum. Behav. 19, 659–671. doi: 10.1016/S0747-5632(03)00040-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moretta, T., and Buodo, G. (2020). Problematic internet use and loneliness: how complex is the relationship? A short literature review. Curr. Addict. Rep. 7, 125–136. doi: 10.1007/s40429-020-00305-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, A. S., and Heimberg, R. G. (2013). Social anxiety and social anxiety disorder. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 9, 249–274. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185631

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Day, E. B., and Heimberg, R. G. (2021). Social media use, social anxiety, and loneliness: a systematic review. Comput. Hum. Behav. Rep. 3:100070. doi: 10.1016/j.chbr.2021.100070

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruppel, E. K., and McKinley, C. J. (2015). Social support and social anxiety in use and perceptions of online mental health resources: exploring social compensation and enhancement. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 18, 462–467. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0652

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, G., and Topham, P. (2012). The impact of social anxiety on student learning and well-being in higher education. J. Ment. Health 21, 375–385. doi: 10.3109/09638237.2012.694505

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Şahin, N. H., Batıgün, A. D., and Koç, V. (2011a). Kişilerarası Tarz, Kendilik Algısı, Öfke ve Depresyon1, 2. Turk Psikiyatri Derg. 21, 1–10. doi: 10.5080/u6264

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Şahin, N. H., Batıgün, A. D., and Uzun, C. (2011b). Anksiyete bozukluğu: Kişilerarası tarz, kendilik algısı ve öfke açısından bir değerlendirme. Anadolu Psikiyiyatri Derg. 12, 107–113.

Google Scholar

Şahin, N. H. B., and Durak, A. (1994). Kişilerarası İlişkiler Ölçeği: Psikometrik özellikleri. İzmir, Turkey: Ulusal Psikoloji Kongresi.

Google Scholar

Smith, A., and Anderson, M. (2018). Social media use 2018: demographics and statistics. Pew Res. Center Internet Technol. Retrieved from https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2018/03/01/social-media-use-in-2018/ (Accessed August 3, 2025).

Google Scholar

Soykan, C., Özgüven, H. D., and Gençöz, T. (2003). Liebowitz social anxiety scale: the Turkish version. Psychol. Rep. 93, 1059–1069. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2003.93.3f.1059

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Speed, B. C., Goldstein, B. L., and Goldfried, M. R. (2018). Assertiveness training: a forgotten evidence-based treatment. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 25:20. doi: 10.1111/cpsp.12216

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, S. H. (2003). Social skills training with children and young people: theory, evidence and practice. Child Adolesc. Mental Health 8, 84–96. doi: 10.1111/1475-3588.00051

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinert, C., Hofmann, M., Leichsenring, F., and Kruse, J. (2013). What do we know today about the prospective long-term course of social anxiety disorder? A systematic literature review. J. Anxiety Disord. 27, 692–702. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.08.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung, R. A., and Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychol. Rev. 107, 411–429. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.107.3.411

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Towner, E., Tomova, L., Ladensack, D., Chu, K., and Callaghan, B. (2022). Virtual social interaction and loneliness among emerging adults amid the COVID-19 pandemic. Curr. Res. Ecol. Soc. Psychol. 3:100058. doi: 10.1016/j.cresp.2022.100058

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vilaplana-Pérez, A., Pérez-Vigil, A., Sidorchuk, A., Brander, G., Isomura, K., Hesselmark, E., et al. (2021). Much more than just shyness: the impact of social anxiety disorder on educational performance across the lifespan. Psychol. Med. 51, 861–869. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719003908

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Waldinger, R. J., and Schulz, M. S. (2016). The long reach of nurturing family environments: links with midlife emotion-regulatory styles and late-life security in intimate relationships. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1443–1450. doi: 10.1177/0956797616661556

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, M. Z., and Zhong, Z.-j. (2014). Loneliness, social contacts and internet addiction: a cross-lagged panel study. Comput. Hum. Behav. 30, 164–170. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.08.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: inhibitory relationship, nurturing relationship, social anxiety, social interaction, virtual loneliness

Citation: Kurtoğlu MB and Sezer Katar K (2025) Exploring the mediating role of virtual environment loneliness in the link between interpersonal relationship styles and social anxiety. Front. Psychol. 16:1603001. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1603001

Received: 04 April 2025; Accepted: 08 August 2025;
Published: 20 August 2025.

Edited by:

Irene Valori, Technical University Dresden, Germany

Reviewed by:

Dong Yang, Krirk University, Thailand
Wahyu Rahardjo, Gunadarma University, Indonesia

Copyright © 2025 Kurtoğlu and Sezer Katar. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mustafa Batuhan Kurtoğlu, bXVzdGFmYS5rdXJ0b2dsdUBoa3UuZWR1LnRy

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.