PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 18 June 2025

Sec. Mindfulness

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1606604

This article is part of the Research TopicApplications of Mindfulness in Media and Communication StudiesView all 13 articles

How should we approach negative news in the media? A ‘mindful’ and ‘harmonious’ consumption of negative news by users might be an answer

  • 1Department of Psychology, College of Education, Psychology and Social Work, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA, Australia
  • 2Flinders University Institute for Mental Health and Wellbeing, College of Education, Psychology and Social Work, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA, Australia
  • 3Department of Communication, Media Neuroscience Lab, University of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, United States
  • 4Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, United States
  • 5Division of Communication and Media, Ewha Woman’s University, Seoul, Republic of Korea

The prevalence of negative sentiment in news content appears to have increased over time. This trend has raised concerns about the potential challenges associated with consuming negative news, particularly when it is consumed frequently. In response, two general recommendations have emerged: news organizations should strive to balance negative coverage with positive reporting, and users are advised to limit their exposure to negative news. However, these recommendations seem to not be effective in practice in many cases and may also raise moral, intellectual, informational, and societal challenges (e.g., the public’s unawareness of societal issues). This Perspective aims to propound that negative news can be consumed ‘mindfully’ and ‘harmoniously’. We propose that individuals should adopt an agentic mindset toward negative news consumption. We outline what mindful and harmonious consumption of negative news might look like and argue that recommending a fixed duration limit for exposure to negative news for all users may not be feasible, given the interpersonal and intrapersonal variability among individuals. The mindful-harmonious consumption of negative news may serve as an adaptive strategy for contemporary users, who are continuously exposed to negative news yet still require it. Conceptual and empirical investigations into the mindful and harmonious consumption of negative news warrant scholarly and public attention.

Introduction

Beyond the debate over whether the modern world experiences more negative events or whether people today have greater access to negative news than in the past, the contemporary news landscape appears increasingly saturated with negative coverage, and, as always, people are drawn to such content. Indeed, “bad seems to be stronger than good” for humans; for example, negative information may be processed more thoroughly than positive information [see Baumeister et al. (2001)]. Many individuals display negativity bias, a tendency to prioritize and assign greater significance to negative information over positive content (e.g., Soroka et al., 2019). This bias may have evolutionary origins, as attentiveness to negative news can provide information about potential threats in the environment and contribute to self-preservation (Blades, 2021; Shoemaker, 1996). However, this “hardwired brain” for negative news (Shoemaker, 1996; Trussler and Soroka, 2014) has likely contributed to the media’s growing emphasis on negative coverage, particularly in recent years [e.g., see Leetaru (2011)].

Emotionally neutral news is scarce in today’s media landscape. The prevalence of negative sentiment in news content and headlines—often characterized by emotions such as anger, fear, disgust, and sadness—appears to have risen over time (Rozado et al., 2022). Positive news, in contrast, is frequently overlooked or dismissed as less newsworthy (Van der Meer et al., 2019), as negative content tends to attract higher readership, engagement, and feedback (Robertson et al., 2023; “If It Bleeds, It Leads”). Negative news is even shared more often by consumers (e.g., 1.91 times more often than positive news; see Watson et al., 2024). In this context, concerns have arisen regarding the potential challenges associated with exposure to negative news.

Research has found a link between time spent on negative news consumption and adverse psychological outcomes, such as increased psychological negative states (e.g., anxiety, irritation, insecurity; de Hoog and Verboon, 2020). The issue is particularly concerning for individuals who engage in problematic and compulsive negative news consumption—commonly referred to as doomscrolling—a dysfunctional behavior that has been associated with serious mental health challenges and maladaptive attitudes and reactions [e.g., elevated misanthropy, reduced self-protective behaviors, and increased engagement in risky activities; see Shabahang et al. (2023, 2024a) and Sharma et al. (2022)].

In response to the reinforcing loop between the increasing production of negative news by media agencies and the rising consumption of such news by users—each intensifying and sustaining the other [see Shoemaker (1996), for a discussion]—as well as the associated consequences of negative news exposure, two primary recommendations have been widely discussed in recent years, one directed at ‘news media’ and the other at ‘users’. News organizations are encouraged to balance negative coverage with positive reporting (e.g., maintaining a ratio of one positive report for every three negative reports), while users are advised to limit their exposure to negative news [e.g., see Vander Weele and Brooks (2023)]. However, such recommendations seem to not be effective in practice in many cases and may also raise moral, intellectual, informational, and societal challenges.

News media

The commitment of news media to maintaining a balance between negative and positive reporting may be impractical and could contribute to public misperceptions. During certain periods (e.g., the onset of a global pandemic resulting in millions of deaths) and circumstances (e.g., a country engaged in war or experiencing multiple natural or human-made disasters), positive news may be scarce or even non-existent, making the maintenance of a consistent ratio between negative and positive coverage almost impossible. Even when positive news exists in such contexts, the significance of negative reports are likely to overshadow uplifting stories due to the misalignment of these positive reports with the prevailing collective negative states during these periods (e.g., concerns for survival). Furthermore, a rigid commitment to positive reporting could misrepresent reality for the audience, potentially fostering an unrealistic perception of reality rather than accurately reflecting the conditions. In addition, studies suggest that positive news appear to be often more associated with entertainment and emotional upliftment and less aligned with journalism’s core functions, such as holding power accountable and providing the public with the information necessary for an informed electorate e.g., (see McIntyre, 2016). Even when the intent is to produce uplifting content, the “silver-lining approach”—which highlights positive aspects of negative events—is recommended as a more viable alternative to solely positive reporting [e.g., see McIntyre and Gibson (2016)]. Therefore, positive news reporting is expected to struggle to keep pace with the volume and immediacy of negative news coverage, and imposing a fixed quota of positive news may not always be a practical and beneficial approach.

News consumers

Likewise, the recommendation for users to restrict their exposure to negative news presents potential personal and societal challenges, as negative news holds informational value [see Neijzen (2024), for a discussion]. Negative news highlights problems and the need for reactions and solutions. While positive news can linger without urgency, negative news demands immediate attention (Shoemaker, 2006, for a commentary). Humans require the “ability to learn from bad news” [see Moutsiana et al. (2013)], as negative news aids in updating our understanding of the environment and potential threats. At the personal level, a lack of such updates may increase vulnerabilities, such as unfamiliarity with threats, irrational risk-taking, and dysfunctional decision-making (Moutsiana et al., 2013). For example, a longitudinal study by Tandoc and Kim, 2023 found a link between news avoidance and increased belief in COVID-19 misinformation (e.g., “Gargling with salt water can protect you from COVID-19”). Furthermore, negative news has societal implications [signaling effect; see Fu (2023)]. Exposure to negative news may sometimes help individuals to transcend personal concerns and engage more deeply with humanity, global issues, and broader collective challenges [see self-transcendent media experiences; Oliver et al. (2018)]. For instance, positive changes have followed negative news regarding corporate social irresponsibility [e.g., see Fu (2023)], and collective actions have arisen in response to negative news about minorities [e.g., see Saleem et al. (2021)]. In the absence of such reporting, the perceived urgency for change and collective action may be reduced.

Even, we conjecture that negative news, in certain cases, may exert profound impacts on some audiences—effects that might be essential for societal functioning in some cases. Prior research suggests that exposure to negative news may indirectly traumatize consumers [see media-induced PTSS; Abdalla et al. (2021)]. For instance, Robert et al. (2021), in their study following the November 2015 Paris terrorist attacks—which resulted in hundreds of deaths and injuries—found that individuals with higher media exposure to the event reported increased levels of post-traumatic stress symptoms. Considering that traumatic experiences may, in some cases, lead to positive fundamental changes—such as an increased sense of self-efficacy, greater appreciation of life, and enhanced connectedness with humanity [see post-traumatic growth theory; Tedeschi and Calhoun (1996)]—exposure to negative news may similarly foster growth in some extent among some users. We refer to this potential phenomenon as ‘negative news post-traumatic growth’. Research has identified negative emotions, such as threat-based awe, as motivators of helping behavior in many cases (e.g., Septianto et al., 2021), as these emotions may capture attention and heighten feelings of personal responsibility to address the perceived issue [see Carlson and Miller (1987), for arguments]. Strong negatively valenced news may elicit self-transcendent experiences and promote substantial helping behaviors. Imagine, for instance, an individual watches intensive media reports on child trafficking for organ removal in their hometown and subsequently decides to donate funds originally saved for a holiday trip to child protection programs and organizations (sacrificing personal welfare). Indeed, we propose that negative news may deeply engage some audiences with the issue, motivating acts of self-sacrificial helping behavior in few cases. Such actions, which go beyond simple acts of kindness, may be essential in contexts where low-intensity helping behaviors are insufficient to address urgent societal challenges.

Therefore, restricting exposure to negative news by users may also not be an effective recommendation in many cases, both for individual users and society at large, as such avoidance may lead to personal unawareness and the potential for societal recession.

Our suggestion

So, the question now is, what can be done about negative news? This Perspective does not propose a new recommendation for the news media. As previously argued (e.g., McIntyre, 2016; Neijzen, 2024; Shoemaker, 2006), negative news coverage should continue due to its informational significance, without imposing a requirement to produce positive news at a predetermined rate solely to counterbalance the overall negativity of the news landscape. Our recommendation is for news consumers. We propose that a ‘mindful and harmonious approach to consuming negative news’ may be adaptive, as research suggests that goal-oriented, mindful, and harmonious engagement with media—even at high frequencies of use—is associated with positive outcomes.

The ‘quality’ of media engagement (‘how’ users engage) often warrants greater attention than the ‘quantity’ of use (‘how much’ users engage). Media engagement can be non-problematic as long as such engagement does not displace other meaningful activities or interfere with daily life [see digital Goldilocks hypothesis; Przybylski and Weinstein (2017)]. Przybylski and Weinstein (2017) found that users could engage in digital activities for up to about 2 h longer on weekends than on weekdays before experiencing negative outcomes. They suggested that factors beyond mere time spent (screen-time) must be considered when examining the relationship between users and negative outcomes of media use. Indeed, the frequency of media use may not always be an informative variable (Størup and Lieberoth, 2023). An eight-year longitudinal study by Coyne et al. (2020) showed that the relationship between time spent on social media and mental health issues was negligible. Similarly, a longitudinal study by Koban et al. (2023) found that while compulsive social media use was associated with information and communication overload, habitual social media use was not. Furthermore, studies have reported only a weak association between time spent on social media and the presence of problematic usage patterns (e.g., Peng and Liao, 2023), suggesting that frequent social media users do not necessarily meet the criteria for problematic use. Additionally, research indicates that obsessive passion (a rigid and uncontrollable urge to engage) is associated with problematic media use, whereas harmonious passion (a flexible and volitional form of engagement) is not [e.g., see Mylonopoulos and Theoharakis (2021); Przybylski et al. (2009)]. Moreover, studies highlight positive outcomes associated with media use, depending on how users engage with the platform. For example, Sun et al. (2023) found that using social media for problem-focused coping (e.g., “I used social media to take action to improve the lockdown situation.”) was linked to poorer psychological adjustment. In contrast, using social media for socioemotional coping (e.g., “I received comfort and understanding from someone through social media.”) was associated with better psychological adjustment, including reduced anger and tension. Shabahang et al. (2024b) found that mindful social media use—defined as being consciously aware of one’s intentions, emotions, and thoughts while using social media—was associated with lower levels of problematic social media use and psychological distress. Recent study by Shabahang et al. (2025) demonstrated that mindful social media use was linked to weaker conspiracy thinking and lower endorsement of conspiracy beliefs. They suggest that, contrary to the common assumption that higher social media use is associated with greater conspiracism, social media use itself may not be inherently conducive to conspiracy belief formation. Rather, a high-quality use of social media may reduce susceptibility to conspiricism.

Taking these findings and arguments into account, frequent media use does not necessarily equate to problematic use or the experience of distress and dysfunction. Accordingly, we suggest that the question of ‘how news consumers engage with negative news’ is often more important than ‘how much negative news they consume’.

News consumers should not perceive engagement with negative news in binary terms (i.e., avoidance versus engagement); rather, they should view it as existing along a spectrum. By (a) maintaining mindful awareness of their intentions, emotions, and thoughts before, during, and after exposure to negative reports [see Raney et al., 2021; Shabahang et al. (2024b)], and (b) regulating their engagement and disengagement harmoniously (see dualistic model of passion; Vallerand et al., 2003), users can determine their personally optimal frequency of negative news consumption at various moments. For instance, if a user begins consuming negative news while feeling psychologically and emotionally stable—close to their baseline optimal state (mindful awareness before consumption)—and notices that even a few minutes of exposure reduces their well-being (mindful awareness during consumption), they should consider discontinuing exposure at that moment. Reflecting on their psychological state after consumption (mindful awareness after consumption) can help them determine whether to continue engaging with negative news that day or postpone further exposure to another time. On a different day, when feeling emotionally resilient, the same individual may be able to consume negative news for several hours without deviating from their baseline optimal psychological and emotional state (see Figure 1). A mindful-harmonious consumption of negative news task could look like the following:

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mindful-Harmonious versus Mindless-Inharmonious consumption of negative news. Note. The size of each circle represents the level of mindful awareness regarding personal psychological states and receptiveness toward negative news, with larger circles indicating higher levels of mindful awareness. The color within the circles reflects the valence of psychological states, with hues closer to green denoting more positive states and hues closer to red indicating more negative states. Mindless-inharmonious negative news consumers are characterized by a lack of awareness of their psychological states and their preparedness for exposure to negative news content. They are expected to either compulsively engage with or rigidly avoid negative news without consideration of their current psychological condition or readiness for negative news consumption. This pattern of engagement is assumed to result in extreme worldviews—specifically, a markedly negative assumptive worldview among individuals with consistently high engagement, and an excessively positive assumptive worldview among those who consistently disengage or avoid such content. In contrast, mindful-harmonious negative news consumers are expected to maintain an awareness of their psychological states, their preparedness to be exposed to negative news, and their capacity for continued engagement. Based on this self-awareness, they engage with negative news in a harmonious and autonomous manner. For example, they may choose to limit or avoid exposure when they do not feel psychologically prepared, and increase their engagement when they are in a state of readiness for negative news consumption.

“Whenever you, as a news consumer, intend to engage with negative news, we encourage you to pause and take a few deep breaths. Take a moment to observe your current needs, desires, emotions, bodily sensations, and thoughts. Allow yourself time to reflect before proceeding (e.g., before typing the name of a news website into your browser). How is your day going? Have you experienced any difficulties today—whether in your family, school, or workplace? Are there responsibilities you still need to attend to? Is your body feeling tense? Are you feeling worried or emotionally low? Consider whether this is the right moment for you to engage with negative news content. What are your reasons for wanting to do so? Reflect on whether you feel equipped to process negative news critically at this time. Do you believe you can make well-informed conclusions and decisions, or might your current state of fatigue or emotional strain lead to more superficial judgments? [Pause] Reflect on the thoughts, emotions, and needs that may be motivating your decision to engage with negative news. Hold your attention on these reflections. [Pause] As you consume negative news, approach both the content and your own experience with curiosity and openness. Pay careful attention to factors such as the emotional intensity of the news, the credibility of the source, the level of detail provided, the origin of the information, and its potential implications for both yourself and society. At the same time, observe your internal state. Are you feeling disheartened or fatigued? Do you notice physical signs of stress, such as tension, sweating, or restlessness? Are you forming rigid or negative stereotypes in response to what you are seeing? Are you making quick judgments or decisions without allowing sufficient time for critical reflection? Strive to maintain mindful awareness throughout the experience. [Pause] Before quickly moving on to another piece of negative news, allow yourself time to process the information you have just received, as well as the bodily and psychological experiences that accompanied it. Reflect on the new knowledge you have gained and consider how it has influenced your emotions, thoughts, and attitudes. [Pause] Did engaging with this negative news fulfill the need or intention you were hoping to address? Do you sense an increased likelihood of reacting with irritability or aggression if someone were to frustrate you at this moment? Are the conclusions you are drawing from this news shaped more by emotion or by rational reflection? Ask yourself whether you feel emotionally and mentally well enough to continue engaging with negative news. Do you have the energy not only to be exposed to further negative content but also to critically analyze and reflect upon it? [Pause] Take a deep breath, and give yourself a moment to decide whether to continue or to discontinue reading negative news at this time. [Pause] Based on how your current psychological state compares to your personally optimal state, determine when you are ready to resume exposure to negative news. This may be immediately, in a few hours, tomorrow, or even after several days. Regardless of the timing, maintain an active awareness of your internal state throughout each instance of engagement. Consider writing down your observations through personal check-ins. Note the intensity and fluctuations of your negative emotions, thoughts, and behaviors, and assess your current capacity to process negative news. By reflecting on these insights—before, during, and after exposure—you can make more informed decisions about when and how to continue engaging with negative news content (harmonious consumption). It is important to remember that your engagement with negative news is within your control. There is no need to proceed immediately, nor to avoid it for extended periods. The key is finding a balance that aligns with your well-being. You have the autonomy to decide both when to engage and to what extent.”

We suggest that recommending a fixed duration limit for exposure to negative news for all users is not feasible, as individuals may vary both interpersonally (e.g., one person may experience distress and dysfunction after three hours of exposure, while another may not even after five hours) and intrapersonally (e.g., an individual may feel distressed and experience dysfunction after two hours of exposure on one day but not after four hours on another day). Consequently, a mindful and dynamic (harmonious) approach to consuming negative news may be a more universally applicable recommendation. Such an approach—allowing engagement and disengagement to flow based on one’s current psychological state and resources (from brief news checks to excessive consumption)—may help individuals stay informed and form informative and moderate conclusions while maintaining their personally baseline optimal psychological and emotional state.

Overall, this Perspective seeks to propound the idea that negative news may be consumed mindfully and harmoniously, and that such an adaptive consumption pattern may be beneficial for news consumers. Drawing on theoretical frameworks and empirical findings suggesting that conscious and self-regulated media engagement has the potential to reduce user vulnerability—such as evidence linking mindful social media use to lower psychological distress and fewer anxiety-inducing thoughts (e.g., Shabahang et al., 2024b, 2025), and associations between harmonious internet use and positive psychological outcomes (e.g., Naydanova and Beal, 2016)—it can be posited that adopting an adaptive approach to negative news consumption could mitigate vulnerabilities of users about negative news consumption.

We propose that individuals who adopt an agentic mindset toward negative news—perceiving their engagement as within their control and sustaining that sense of agency throughout the consumption process [e.g., see social media use mindsets; Lee and Hancock (2024)]—may exhibit reduced vulnerability to its adverse effects. We anticipate that empowering news consumers by fostering the ability to engage with negative news mindfully and harmoniously may, in some cases, be a more effective approach than encouraging users to avoid negative news or urging news media to reduce negativity. Such strategies may, in some cases, inadvertently diminish collective awareness and motivation for positive change while fostering unrealistic representations and perceptions—both of which are functionally and ethically problematic.

However, it is important to acknowledge that the argument presented in this Perspective is grounded solely in relevant, albeit ‘indirect’, theoretical and empirical frameworks that suggest mindful-harmonious interaction with media and its content may be beneficial. To the best of our knowledge, no ‘direct’ empirical evidence currently exists regarding adaptive engagement with negative news or the potential outcomes such a consumption pattern may yield. However, it may be informative to note that recently collected pilot data (Shabahang, 2025) identified a negative association between mindful social media use and problematic consumption of negative news on social media, with a Pearson correlation of −0.291 (p < 0.01) observed in a sample of 973 Iranian adolescent social media users. This association was identified using the Mindful Use of Social Media Scale (Shabahang et al., 2024a) and the Social Media Doomscrolling Scale (Shabahang et al., 2023). It seems that users who engage more mindfully with social media may be less likely to scroll negative news on social media in a problematic manner. It is conceivable that, to some extent, mindful social media users may apply their mindful approach to their interaction with negative news content as well.

To advance both scientific understanding and public awareness of mindful and dynamic approaches to negative news consumption, future research is encouraged to pursue conceptual and empirical investigations. These may include the development of measurement instruments—drawing on existing tools assessing adaptive media use (e.g., Mindful Use of Social Media Scale; Shabahang et al., 2024a)—to capture mindful awareness during exposure to negative news (an example item for a self-report assessment could be: When browsing negative news, I take a moment to reflect on the news item I have read and its effects on my thoughts and emotions before proceeding to another negative news item), as well as the design and evaluation of mindful news consumption tasks and interventions. The mindful and harmonious consumption of negative news might serve as an adaptive strategy for today’s users, who are continuously exposed to negative news yet still require it. This remains an assumption that warrants scholarly investigation and could attract public attention should supportive evidence emerge.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author contributions

RS: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft. RW: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abdalla, S. M., Cohen, G. H., Tamrakar, S., Koya, S. F., and Galea, S. (2021). Media exposure and the risk of post-traumatic stress disorder following a mass traumatic event: an in-silico experiment. Front. Psych. 12:674263. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.674263

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., and Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 5, 323–370. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blades, R. (2021). Protecting the brain against bad news. Can. Med. Assoc. J. 193, E428–E429. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.1095928

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, M., and Miller, N. (1987). Explanation of the relation between negative mood and helping. Psychol. Bull. 102, 91–108. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.102.1.91

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coyne, S. M., Rogers, A. A., Zurcher, J. D., Stockdale, L., and Booth, M. (2020). Does time spent using social media impact mental health?: an eight year longitudinal study. Comput. Hum. Behav. 104:106160. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.106160

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Hoog, N., and Verboon, P. (2020). Is the news making us unhappy? The influence of daily news exposure on emotional states. Br. J. Psychol. 111, 157–173. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12389

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fu, L. (2023). Why bad news can be good news: the signaling feedback effect of negative media coverage of corporate irresponsibility. Organ. Environ. 36, 98–125. doi: 10.1177/10860266221108704

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koban, K., Stevic, A., and Matthes, J. (2023). A tale of two concepts: differential temporal predictions of habitual and compulsive social media use concerning connection overload and sleep quality. J. Comput.-Mediat. Commun. 28:zmac 040. doi: 10.1093/jcmc/zmac040

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, A. Y., and Hancock, J. T. (2024). Social media mindsets: a new approach to understanding social media use and psychological well-being. J. Comput.-Mediat. Commun. 29:zmad 048. doi: 10.1093/jcmc/zmad048

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leetaru, K. (2011). Culturomics 2.0: forecasting large-scale human behavior using global news media tone in time and space. First Monday 16. doi: 10.5210/fm.v16i9.3663

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntyre, K. (2016). What makes “good” news newsworthy? Commun. Res. Rep. 33, 223–230. doi: 10.1080/08824096.2016.1186619

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntyre, K. E., and Gibson, R. (2016). Positive news makes readers feel good: a “silver-lining” approach to negative news can attract audiences. South Commun. J. 81, 304–315. doi: 10.1080/1041794X.2016.1171892

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moutsiana, C., Garrett, N., Clarke, R. C., Lotto, R. B., Blakemore, S. J., and Sharot, T. (2013). Human development of the ability to learn from bad news. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 110, 16396–16401. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1305631110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mylonopoulos, N., and Theoharakis, V. (2021). Are you keeping your Facebook passions and habit under control? A dual-system perspective on Facebook addiction-like symptoms. Int. J. Electron. Commer. 25, 181–203. doi: 10.1080/10864415.2021.1887697

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Naydanova, E., and Beal, B. D. (2016). Harmonious and obsessive internet passion, competence, and self-worth: a study of high school students in the United States and Russia. Comput. Human Behav. 64, 88–93. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.06.034

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Neijzen, M. (2024). The epistemic value of doombehaviour: beyond the prudential consequences of doomscrolling, doomchecking, and doomsurfing. Synthese 203, 1–25. doi: 10.1007/s11229-024-04602-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliver, M. B., Raney, A. A., Slater, M. D., Appel, M., Hartmann, T., Bartsch, A., et al. (2018). Self-transcendent media experiences: taking meaningful media to a higher level. J. Commun. 68, 380–389. doi: 10.1093/joc/jqx020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, P., and Liao, Y. (2023). Six addiction components of problematic social media use in relation to depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms: a latent profile analysis and network analysis. BMC Psychiatry. 23:321. doi: 10.1186/s12888-023-04837-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Przybylski, A. K., and Weinstein, N. (2017). A large-scale test of the goldilocks hypothesis: quantifying the relations between digital-screen use and the mental well-being of adolescents. Psychol. Sci. 28, 204–215. doi: 10.1177/0956797616678438

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Przybylski, A. K., Weinstein, N., Ryan, R. M., and Rigby, C. S. (2009). Having to versus wanting to play: background and consequences of harmonious versus obsessive engagement in video games. Cycberpsychol. Behav. 12, 485–492. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2009.0083

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raney, A. A., Janicke-Bowles, S. H., Oliver, M. B., and Dale, K. R. (2021). Introduction to positive media psychology. New York: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.

Google Scholar

Robert, M., Stene, L. E., Garfin, D. R., Vandentorren, S., Motreff, Y., du Roscoat, E., et al. (2021). Media exposure and post-traumatic stress symptoms in the wake of the November 2015 Paris terrorist attacks: a population-based study in France. Front. Psych. 12:509457. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.509457

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, C. E., Pröllochs, N., Schwarzenegger, K., Pärnamets, P., Van Bavel, J. J., and Feuerriegel, S. (2023). Negativity drives online news consumption. Nat. Hum. Behav. 7, 812–822. doi: 10.1038/s41562-023-01538-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozado, D., Hughes, R., and Halberstadt, J. (2022). Longitudinal analysis of sentiment and emotion in news media headlines using automated labelling with transformer language models. PLoS One 17:e0276367. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0276367

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saleem, M., Hawkins, I., Wojcieszak, M. E., and Roden, J. (2021). When and how negative news coverage empowers collective action in minorities. Commun. Res. 48, 291–316. doi: 10.1177/0093650219877094

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Septianto, F., Seo, Y., and Paramita, W. (2021). The role of implicit theories in motivating donations in response to threat-based awe. J. Public Policy Mark. 41, 72–88. doi: 10.1177/07439156211042281

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shabahang, R. (2025). Mindful use of social media and doomscrolling on social media. doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/9H3TF

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shabahang, R., Aruguete, M. S., Kim, S., Hosseinkhanzadeh, A. A., and Kakabaraee, K. (2023). “Give your thumb a break” from surfing tragic posts: potential corrosive consequences of social media users’ doomscrolling. Media Psychol. 26, 460–479. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2022.2157287

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shabahang, R., Hwang, H., Thomas, E. F., Aruguete, M. S., McCutcheon, L. E., Orosz, G., et al. (2024a). Doomscrolling evokes existential anxiety and fosters pessimism about human nature? Evidence from Iran and the United States. Computers Human Behavior Reports 15:100438. doi: 10.1016/j.chbr.2024.100438

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shabahang, R., Thomas, E. F., Douglas, K. M., Weber, R., Billieux, J., Reyes, M. E. S., et al. (2025). Mindful use of social media is associated with weaker conspiracy beliefs—evidence from the Philippines, Australia, and Iran. Available online at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=5236000 (Accessed April 29, 2025).

Google Scholar

Shabahang, R., Zsila, Á., Aruguete, M. S., Huynh, H. P., and Orosz, G. (2024b). Embrace the moment using social media: a cross-cultural study of mindful use of social media. Mindfulness 15, 157–173. doi: 10.1007/s12671-023-02271-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharma, B., Lee, S. S., and Johnson, B. K. (2022). The dark at the end of the tunnel: Doomscrolling on social media newsfeeds. Technol. Mind Behav. 3. doi: 10.1037/tmb0000059

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shoemaker, P. J. (1996). Hardwired for news: using biological and cultural evolution to explain the surveillance function. J. Commun. 46, 32–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1996.tb01487.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shoemaker, P. (2006). News and newsworthiness: a commentary. Communications 31, 105–111. doi: 10.1515/COMMUN.2006.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soroka, S., Fournier, P., and Nir, L. (2019). Cross-national evidence of a negativity bias in psychophysiological reactions to news. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 116, 18888–18892. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1908369116

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Størup, J. O., and Lieberoth, A. (2023). What’s the problem with “screen time”? A content analysis of dominant voices and worries in three years of national print media. Convergence 29, 201–224. doi: 10.1177/13548565211065299

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, X., Li, B. J., Zhang, H., and Zhang, G. (2023). Social media use for coping with stress and psychological adjustment: a transactional model of stress and coping perspective. Front. Psychol. 14:1140312. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1140312

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tandoc, E. C. Jr., and Kim, H. K. (2023). Avoiding real news, believing in fake news? Investigating pathways from information overload to misbelief. Journalism 24, 1174–1192. doi: 10.1177/14648849221090744

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tedeschi, R. G., and Calhoun, L. G. (1996). The posttraumatic growth inventory: measuring the positive legacy of trauma. J. Trauma. Stress. 9, 455–471. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490090305

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trussler, M., and Soroka, S. (2014). Consumer demand for cynical and negative news frames. Int. J. Press Politics 19, 360–379. doi: 10.1177/1940161214524832

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallerand, R. J., Blanchard, C., Mageau, G. A., Koestner, R., Ratelle, C., Léonard, M., et al. (2003). Les passions de l’âme: On obsessive and harmonious passion. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 85, 756–767. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.756

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Meer, T. G., Kroon, A. C., Verhoeven, P., and Jonkman, J. (2019). Mediatization and the disproportionate attention to negative news: the case of airplane crashes. J. Stud. 20, 783–803. doi: 10.1080/1461670X.2018.1423632

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vander Weele, T. J., and Brooks, A. C. (2023). A public health approach to negative news media: the 3-to-1 solution. Am. J. Health Promot. 37, 447–449. doi: 10.1177/0890117120914227

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, J., van der Linden, S., Watson, M., and Stillwell, D. (2024). Negative online news articles are shared more to social media. Sci. Rep. 14:21592. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-71263-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: news, news consumption, mindfulness, harmonious passion, doomscrolling

Citation: Shabahang R and Weber R (2025) How should we approach negative news in the media? A ‘mindful’ and ‘harmonious’ consumption of negative news by users might be an answer. Front. Psychol. 16:1606604. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1606604

Received: 05 April 2025; Accepted: 20 May 2025;
Published: 18 June 2025.

Edited by:

Chao Liu, Huaqiao University, China

Reviewed by:

Loukia Taxitari, Neapolis University, Cyprus

Copyright © 2025 Shabahang and Weber. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: René Weber, cmVuZXdAdWNzYi5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.