Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 19 August 2025

Sec. Neuropsychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1607317

Modulatory effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on sensory gating in Fibromyalgia Syndrome

Juan L. Terrasa&#x;Juan L. Terrasa1Christine Winterholler&#x;Christine Winterholler1Pedro Montoya
Pedro Montoya1*Antonio JuanAntonio Juan2Casandra I. Montoro,
Casandra I. Montoro1,3*
  • 1Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience and Clinical Psychology, Research Institute of Health Sciences (IUNICS) and Balearic Islands Health Research Institute (IdISBa), University of the Balearic Islands (UIB), Palma, Spain
  • 2Rheumatology, Hospital Universitari Son Llàtzer, Palma de Mallorca, Spain
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Jaén, Jaén, Spain

Introduction: Several studies have demonstrated a reduced habituation to redundant somatosensory stimulation (sensory gating) in Fibromyalgia Syndrome. Furthermore, anodal transcranial direct current stimulation has been shown to modulate somatosensory processing. The aim of this study was to examine the modulatory effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation applied over the left primary somatosensory cortex on sensory gating in Fibromyalgia Syndrome.

Methods: Thirty-nine female Fibromyalgia Syndrome patients (43–71 years, mean 55.56 ± 7.85) participated in the study and were randomly assigned to the active transcranial direct current stimulation (n = 17) or non-electrical stimulation (sham; n = 22). Before and after transcranial direct current stimulation, somatosensory evoked potentials were recorded during a paired-pulse paradigm, consisting of two identical somatosensory stimuli (S1 and S2) applied in the right forefinger in rapid succession.

Results: Whereas P50 and N100 components were unaltered, a significant modulatory effect on the difference S1 – S2-which is commonly considered an index of sensory gating-in the Late Positive Complex component was found. This modulation manifested as an increased difference in the right hemisphere (contralateral to the stimulation) and a decreased difference in the left hemisphere (ipsilateral).

Discussion: Although this lateralizing pattern remains to be clarified, present results suggest brain excitability and somatosensory processing modulation by using anodal transcranial direct current stimulation in Fibromyalgia Syndrome patients.

1 Introduction

Fibromyalgia Syndrome (FMS) is a condition characterized by chronic widespread musculoskeletal pain with a global prevalence between 0.2 and 6.6% (Marques et al., 2017; Sarzi-Puttini et al., 2020). The syndrome is complex, involving symptoms like chronic pain, fatigue, cognitive dysfunction, sleep disturbances, hypersensitivity to pain (hyperalgesia/allodynia), and psychiatric conditions such as anxiety and depression (Chinn et al., 2016; Wolfe et al., 2010; Zhu et al., 2017). Additionally, FMS patients experience abnormal somatosensory information processing, including impaired sensory gating—a central nervous system mechanism that filters irrelevant sensory inputs (Montoya et al., 2006). This process prevents higher cortical regions from being overwhelmed with repetitive stimuli (Boutros et al., 1995; Ambrosini et al., 2001; Boutros and Belger, 1999).

Sensory gating is often studied using somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) in paired-pulse paradigms. These paradigms involve two rapid stimuli (S1 and S2), and sensory gating is assessed by analyzing the suppression of responses to the second stimulus (Boutros et al., 1995; Boutros and Belger, 1999; Boutros et al., 2009). This suppression is typically quantified as either the difference in SEP amplitudes between S1 and S2 (S1-S2) or the ratio of amplitudes elicited by S2 relative to S1 (S2/S1) (Fuerst et al., 2007). SEPs include components such as P50, N100, and the Late Positive Complex (LPC), which are measured via EEG. The P50 component, originating from the primary somatosensory cortex, is linked to early stimulus processing and emerges 50 milliseconds after stimulation (Eisenstein and Eisenstein, 2006; Hämäläinen et al., 1990; Desmedt and Tomberg, 1989). N100, generated in the secondary somatosensory cortex, appears around 100 milliseconds post-stimulus and is involved in attention and sensory-motor responses (Staines et al., 2014; Allison et al., 1992; Giard et al., 1988). The LPC, peaking approximately 300 milliseconds after stimulation, reflects higher-order cognitive processes such as decision-making and memory updating (Shen et al., 2020; Rushby et al., 2005). In healthy individuals, P50, N100, and LPC amplitudes decrease in response to the second stimulus, indicating effective sensory gating. However, FMS patients exhibit a failure to suppress these responses, suggesting impaired filtering of irrelevant stimuli (Montoya et al., 2006; Lijffijt et al., 2009).

Given this sensory dysfunction, there is growing interest in non-invasive neuromodulation techniques, particularly transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), as a potential treatment for FMS-related pain (Montoya et al., 2005). tDCS applies a low-intensity current (1–2 mA) to the cortex via electrodes, modulating neuronal excitability by altering resting membrane potentials without inducing action potentials directly (Lloyd et al., 2020; Thair et al., 2017; Lefaucheur et al., 2017). Anodal stimulation is commonly assumed to increase the likelihood of neuronal firing and overall cortical responsiveness, while cathodal stimulation decreases it (Lloyd et al., 2020; Thair et al., 2017). tDCS has been shown to influence brain functional connectivity (Nitsche et al., 2008; Cummiford et al., 2016; Neeb et al., 2019), leading to neurophysiological and psychological changes (Volz et al., 2016; Antal et al., 2010).

Specifically, anodal tDCS targeting the primary somatosensory cortex (SI) can induce changes throughout the somatosensory processing hierarchy, affecting both early sensory and late cognitive stages, including decision-making (Fujimoto et al., 2014; Hilgenstock et al., 2016; Hirtz et al., 2018; Labbé et al., 2016; Ragert et al., 2008). Studies have demonstrated the therapeutic benefits of tDCS for modulating somatosensory function in healthy participants (Vaseghi et al., 2015; Antal et al., 2008; Koyama et al., 2017) and in both acute (Borckardt et al., 2011) and chronic pain patients (Kikkert et al., 2019; Pollonini et al., 2020; Rahimi et al., 2020). Despite the extensive evidence of altered somatosensory processing in FMS, no study has yet examined the effects of tDCS on sensory gating in this population. This gap underscores the need for further research exploring how tDCS might address the sensory gating deficits characteristic of FMS.

In this sense, a previous study by our group demonstrated that anodal tDCS over the somatosensory cortex enhanced the suppression of the LPC component of SEPs during a sensory gating paradigm, indicating a modulation of late-stage inhibitory mechanisms (Montoro et al., 2021). Specifically, we observed a greater attenuation of LPC amplitudes in response to the second stimulus (S2) following stimulation—reflecting improved gating at late cortical processing stages—without significant effects on earlier components such as P50 and N100. The inability to inhibit, or “gate,” irrelevant sensory inputs has been associated with sensory and information overload, potentially leading to neuronal hyperexcitability due to disrupted habituation mechanisms, as observed in conditions such as schizophrenia (Vlcek et al., 2014). Although anodal stimulation is typically regarded as excitatory, our findings suggest that anodal tDCS may enhance top-down inhibitory control over redundant somatosensory input. While the traditional stimulation-dependent model of tDCS posits excitatory effects for anodal and inhibitory effects for cathodal stimulation, emerging evidence points to more complex activation–inhibition patterns (Jacobson et al., 2012). It emphasizes that the effects of tDCS are strongly shaped by ongoing network dynamics (Jacobson et al., 2012; Antal et al., 2014; Thirugnanasambandam et al., 2011; Woods et al., 2016), indicating that tDCS does not directly generate activity in resting neuronal networks but rather modulates existing patterns of spontaneous neuronal activity (Fritsch et al., 2010).

Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine whether anodal tDCS over the left SI can modulate sensory gating—measured by the S1-S2 difference—processing in FMS patients. Considering the reviewed bibliography, it was hypothesized that anodal tDCS would modify SEPs amplitude patterns (P50, N100 and LPC), resulting in attenuated cerebral responses to irrelevant stimulation in FMS patients. Furthermore, we will analyze the potential effect of tDCS on each hemisphere, considering that the stimulation used to induce sensory gating was applied only to the right hand and not bilaterally.

Importantly, this study did not aim to evaluate the clinical efficacy or long-term outcomes of tDCS. Instead, it focused on the basic neurophysiological mechanisms underlying inhibitory control—particularly the S1–S2 difference, which is commonly associated with somatosensory gating—and their modulation following a single session of anodal tDCS. This mechanistic approach is intended to enhance our understanding of the modulation of altered cortical excitability and sensory processing in FMS, thereby providing a foundation for future studies exploring potential links between tDCS, sensory gating, and clinical outcomes in this population.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

A priori power analysis indicated that a minimum sample size of 34 participants was required to achieve 80% statistical power, assuming an alpha level of 0.05 and an effect size of f = 0.25 (approximately equivalent to Cohen’s d ≈ 0.5–0.6, indicating an intermediate effect). Based on this, thirty-nine female volunteers (aged 43–71 years, mean 55.56 ± 7.85) diagnosed with Fibromyalgia Syndrome (FMS) participated in the study. Recruitment occurred through rheumatology units at Son Llátzer and Son Espases hospitals, the Balearic Fibromyalgia Support Association (ABAF), and the Association of Fibromyalgia, Chronic Fatigue Syndrome and Multiple Chemical Sensitivity of Inca and Comarcas (AFIC) in Spain. Eligibility required a confirmed FMS diagnosis by a rheumatology specialist at least 1 year prior, right-handedness, and absence of neurological, psychiatric, or cardiovascular conditions, tDCS contraindications, substance abuse history, or pharmacological treatments affecting the cardiovascular or central nervous systems. Despite this, 89.7% used analgesics, 87.2% antidepressants, 74.4% anxiolytics, and other medications. Participants were randomized into either active anodal tDCS (n = 17) or sham stimulation (n = 22) targeting the left SI, with blinded stimulation codes. All were naive to the procedure and provided written informed consent. The study adhered to the Declaration of Helsinki (1991) and was approved by the Balearic Islands Ethics Committee (protocol IB3681/18PI).

2.2 Psychological assessment

A clinical psychologist obtained the participants´ clinical histories, medication use, sociodemographic data and psychological characteristics. The Spanish versions of the Beck’s Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al., 1961) was used to quantify the severity of depressive symptoms. Current and habitual anxiety levels were quantified by the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger et al., 1970). To evaluate alexithymia levels (i.e., the ability to identify, distinguish, and communicate their feelings) and the impact of the fibromyalgia symptoms in everyday life, the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) (Bagby et al., 1994) and the Fibromyalgia Impact Questionnaire (FIQ) (Burckhardt et al., 1991) were, respectively, used. The McGill Pain Inventory (Melzack, 1975) was administered to assess clinical pain. Finally, positive and negative affect was evaluated by the Manual for the Profile of Mood States (POMS) (McNair et al., 1971) before tDCS. The POMS measures six different dimensions of mood swings over a period of time, namely Tension or Anxiety, Anger or Hostility, Vigour or Activity, Fatigue or Inertia, Depression or Dejection, and Confusion or Bewilderment. In addition, participants reported their confidence and anxiety levels about the upcoming direct current stimulation using a visual-analogical scale (VAS-tDCS; from 0 to 10).

2.3 Transcranial electrical stimulation

The stimulation protocol followed a previously described double-blinded tDCS design (Montoro et al., 2021), using a NeuroConn constant-current stimulator (at 1.5 mA) and gel electrodes (20 mm diameter, 3 cm2 area, 1 mm thickness). The anode was placed at the left SI (CP3 location) and the cathode over the contralateral supraorbital ridge, following previous related papers (Rehmann et al., 2016; Friedrich and Beste, 2018). Active tDCS included a 20-min stimulation with 30-s ramp-up and ramp-down phases. The sham condition replicated active tDCS sensations with a brief current application (1.5 mA for 40 s) followed by no stimulation for the remainder of the session, maintaining electrode impedance below 5 kΩ. Participants sat in an armchair with eyes open during the session. Post-stimulation, they completed a questionnaire on transcranial electrical stimulation (TES)-related sensations (e.g., itching, pain, metallic taste) to assess bodily experiences, their duration, and effects on well-being. The protocol adhered to safety guidelines for tDCS implementation (Thair et al., 2017; Antal et al., 2017).

2.4 Non-painful paired-pulse stimulation task

As in prior studies of our research group (Montoya et al., 2006; Montoro et al., 2021; Terrasa et al., 2018), participants underwent tactile paired-pulse stimulation before and after electrical intervention to assess sensory gating. This involved two identical non-painful pneumatic stimuli (S1 and S2) lasting 100 ms each, with a randomized 550 ms (±50 ms) inter-stimulus interval. Each pair of stimuli was delivered with an inter-pair interval of 12 s. Stimulation was delivered via a pneumatic stimulator (2 bars pressure) through a 10-meter tube to a ductile membrane affixed to the index finger’s ventral first phalanx of the dominant (right) hand using a plastic clip and adhesive strip. Before starting, a tactile test ensured stimulus detection. Participants, instructed to keep their eyes open, received 40 trials administered with standard software (Presentation v18.3, Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc.) immediately before and after anodal tDCS or sham. Stimulus intensity was consistent (2 bars) across trials, and participants were asked to focus on the tactile sensations while seated comfortably with eyes open throughout the session.

2.5 EEG recording and data reduction

During the paired-pulse stimulation task, EEG recording was conducted using a commercial amplifier (QuickAmp, Brain Products GmbH, Munich, Germany) with 62 Ag/AgCl electrodes located according to the 10–10 placement system with common average reference, at a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz. The ground electrode was located at AFz. During EEG recording, eye blinks were recorded using an electrooculogram (EOG) placing one electrode above and another below the left eye. Electrode’s impedance was kept below 10 kOhm.

During offline data pre-processing, using Brain Vision Analyzer, version 1.05 software (Brain Products GmbH, Munich, Germany), EEG signals were segmented in epochs of 600 ms (−100 ms to 500 ms relative to the stimulus onset), filtered digitally (high-pass at 0.10 Hz, low-pass at 30 Hz), and baseline corrected (from −100 ms to 0 ms). Eye blink artifacts were corrected by using Gratton & Coles algorithm (Gratton et al., 1983). Artefact rejection was carried out using the following criteria: maximum voltage step/sampling point = 75 μV, minimum amplitude = −75 μV, maximum amplitude = 75 μV and a maximum absolute difference in the epoch = 75 μV. One participant from the active group did not meet the inclusion criteria for further analyses, which consisted of presenting at least 75% of the epochs free of artefacts for each stimulus. Therefore, 16 participants were included in the active group for EEG analyses. In addition, EEG epochs were separately averaged for S1 and S2.

The amplitudes of the following components of the SEPs were determined: P50, N100 and Late Positive Complex (LPC). The peak amplitude of P50 and N100 was calculated from the baseline for each individual channel within two-time windows after stimulus onset: 20–80 ms for P50, and 80–135 ms for N100. For LPC amplitudes, the area under the curve within the time period of 150–350 ms was calculated.

2.6 Procedure and experimental timeline

The study was conducted across two same-day sessions. In the first session, informed consent, sociodemographic, clinical data and pain ratings from a quantitative sensory testing (QST) were obtained. The QST consisted of three consecutive threshold measurements with a resting period between each measurement of 30 s. Afterwards, the TAS-20, STAI, BDI-II, FIQ, McGill Inventory, POMS, and VAS-tDCS were handed out to the participants. The second session (experimental task) was conducted after a brief 10-min break. During this session, participants were firstly accompanied to the EEG laboratory, where they sat comfortably in an armchair placed inside a Faraday chamber. Then, the experimenter mounted the electrodes for EEG recording and tDCS on the patients’ scalp. EEG recording started with the conduction of the non-painful Paired-Pulse Stimulation Task as described. After the completion of the task and a brief 2-min break, patients received 20 min of brain stimulation (anodal tDCS or sham), followed by another brief 2-min break. Finally, participants had to complete the TES and to perform the non-painful Paired-Pulse Stimulation Task.

2.7 Data analysis

All statistical analyses were carried out using IBM SPSS Statistics 29.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Group differences on sociodemographic data, medication use and self-reports were analyzed with Student t-tests. In order to analyze the SEPs response, EEG data from 30 electrodes (F1, F2, F3, F4, F5, F6, F7, F8, FC1, FC2, FC5, FC6, C1, C2, C3, C4, C5, C6, CP1, CP2, CP5, CP6, P1, P2, P3, P4, P5, P6, P7 and P8) were used for statistical analyses. A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with repeated measures was conducted, using group (tDCS vs. sham) as between-subject factor and time (pre vs. post), hemisphere (left vs. right) and electrode (30 electrodes) as within-subject factors on P50, N100, and LPC amplitude differences elicited by S1 minus S2 as sensory gating indicator. The level of significance was set at p ≤ 0.05 (2-tailed). Greenhouse–Geisser adjustments were applied and post-hoc Bonferroni corrected paired tests were used if necessary. Finally, although it was not one of the objectives of the present study, the effects of tDCS on S1 and S2 were analyzed independently. To achieve this, the same repeated measures MANOVA was conducted for S1 and S2 separately.

3 Results

3.1 Clinical and sociodemographic data

Table 1 shows clinical and sociodemographic data for both groups. No significant differences between groups were found in any of the variables analyzed (all p > 0.093).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Clinical and sociodemographic data.

3.2 Somatosensory ERP amplitudes

Figure 1 displays the SEPs amplitude difference elicited by S1 minus S2 as sensory gating measure in each hemisphere. For LPC component, a significant interaction effect of group x time x hemisphere was found [F(1, 36) = 5.742, p = 0.022, ŋp2 = 0.138], showing that sensory gating was significantly enhanced after stimulation (88.49 ± 19.90 μV*ms) compared with before (39.03 ± 17.75 μV*ms) at right hemisphere electrodes (p = 0.043) in the experimental group (tDCS group) (see Figure 2 for topographical plots). Surprisingly, an inverse significant effect was found at left hemisphere electrodes (p = 0.009), as sensory gating was reduced after stimulation (15.80 ± 26.93 μV*ms) compared with before (77.12 ± 19.97 μV*ms) (see Table 2). No significant effects were obtained in the sham group at any hemisphere (all p > 0.637). The remaining main effects and interactions can be observed in Table 3.

Figure 1
This figure presents average sensory gating waveforms (S1 – S2) recorded across all electrodes, separated into two hemispheric sections: “Left” and “Right.” The top row shows data from the tDCS group, while the bottom row shows data from the SHAM group. In each panel, two lines are plotted: “PRE” (black) representing baseline measurements, and “POST” (red) representing post-stimulation measurements. Grey-shaded areas mark the Late Positive Complex (LPC) time window between 150 and 350 milliseconds. A reference marker below indicates 0.5 microvolts on the vertical axis and 100 milliseconds on the horizontal axis.

Figure 1. Average waveforms across all electrodes representing the sensory gating (S1 – S2) of both tDCS and SHAM groups before (PRE) and after (POST) the brain stimulation at each hemisphere.

Figure 2
Four circular heatmaps represent sensory gating (S1 – S2) by depicting brain activity during the LPC window (150–350 ms). The top row shows the tDCS condition with “PRE” and “POST” maps, while the bottom row shows the SHAM condition. Colors range from blue to red, indicating voltage values from -0.6 to 0.6 µV.

Figure 2. Topographic plots representing the sensory gating (S1 – S2) of both tDCS and SHAM groups before (PRE) and after (POST) the brain stimulation for the LPC component (150–350 ms).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean (±SE) of the sensory gating effect (S1 minus S2) in the tDCS and sham groups, before (PRE) and after (POST) stimulation at each hemisphere.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of the multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with repeated measures for LPC component, indicating degrees of freedom (DF), test statistic value (F), significance level (p), and effect size (ŋp2).

However, no significant effects or interactions were obtained for P50 and N100 components. Finally, the repeated measures MANOVA was also conducted for S1 and S2 independently. Results are depicted in the Supplementary material.

4 Discussion

The present study analyzed for the first time the after-effects of anodal tDCS on brain correlates of somatosensory inhibitory mechanisms in FMS patients. Considering that FMS is characterized by a lack of inhibitory control to repetitive non-painful somatosensory information, the possible modulatory effects of somatosensory gating caused by anodal tDCS was explored by using somatosensory event-related potentials elicited by paired tactile stimuli. Previous studies have shown that brain responses to somatosensory gating were significantly altered in FMS (Montoya et al., 2006). In the same way, previous work has demonstrated that non-invasive brain stimulation techniques such as anodal tDCS can induce significant improvements in the symptomatology of chronic pain patients such as FMS (Pacheco-Barrios et al., 2020; Pinto et al., 2018). The present study demonstrates that anodal tDCS induces significant modulation of brain processing. Specifically, our results revealed that tDCS altered late somatosensory responses during the gating paradigm. Notably, anodal tDCS enhanced the S1-S2 difference—the parameter used to index sensory gating—over the right hemisphere (contralateral to the stimulation site), while, unexpectedly, it reduced this difference over the left hemisphere (ipsilateral to the stimulation site). As expected, no changes in the difference S1-S2-and therefore gating-were observed in the sham group.

Our findings were partially in agreement with previous results in healthy controls indicating that anodal tDCS was associated with a significant amplitude enhancement of early SEP components (<100 ms) elicited by electrical stimulation of the body (Matsunaga, 2004). Previous studies have also reported a beneficial effect of tDCS on auditory hallucinations and negative symptoms, as well as an improvement of sensory gating in the early stages of auditory information processing (P50, N100) in patients with schizophrenia (Kim et al., 2018; Mondino et al., 2016). In line with these findings, our results revealed that anodal tDCS in FMS patients also produced a significant modulatory effect on somatosensory gating, as indicated by changes in the S1–S2 difference of the LPC component. After the stimulation, a widespread enhanced S1-S2 difference—suggestive of a sensory gating effect—was observed in the right hemisphere (contralateral to the place where tDCS was applied). LPC is a positive ongoing response consisting of different independent components, with a peak around 300 ms after stimulus-onset (Rushby et al., 2005). Mainly, LPC amplitudes of event-related potentials are positively correlated to the increase in cognitive demand and the situation’s personal significance, whereas LPC latency seems to be independent of stimulus classification and evaluation time (Rushby et al., 2005; De Pascalis, 2004). Considering the latter, the present results suggested that tDCS may optimally modulate late and cognitively more demanding stages of somatosensory processing in FMS patients, thus improving the altered ability to inhibit repetitive stimuli in FMS (in particular, over contralateral regions to the stimulated brain area). This finding is also in agreement with a previous study from our lab in healthy volunteers (Montoro et al., 2021), showing that anodal tDCS led to an enhancement of inhibitory mechanisms in response to repetitive somatosensory stimulation during late stages of information processing.

However, we observed that brain stimulation impaired the S1-S2 difference—reflecting a disruption of the somatosensory gating effect—of LPC over the left hemisphere (ipsilateral to anode stimulation). This inverse pattern of tDCS modulation (as compared to the right hemisphere) is difficult to explain, and it could be related not only with the tDCS neural target (left primary somatosensory cortex), but also with the tactile stimulation site itself (right hand forefinger). To this regard, most of the studies with FMS patients have stimulated the left primary motor cortex (M1) or the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). M1 has been proposed to be the most effective in modulating cortex excitability and pain sensory processing in FMS patients while DLPFC would have greater effects on emotional symptomatology and affective-cognitive pain processing (Cummiford et al., 2016; Lorenz et al., 2003). Furthermore, although the observed hemispheric asymmetry may be partly attributed to the site of stimulation and the tactile input, lateralized attentional mechanisms might also have contributed (Shulman et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2024). However, spatial attention was not explicitly manipulated in our paradigm, as all stimuli were delivered to the same body location while participants maintained a fixed gaze and posture. Since the modulation emerged only in the experimental group and was confined to the LPC component, with no changes observed in early components (P50, N100), the effect is more likely to reflect higher-order evaluative or integrative processing rather than early attentional orienting. Given the present results, similar comparative studies-and in general, studies delving into the present findings-are required for a better comprehension of the effects of non-invasive brain stimulation on somatosensory information processing in FMS. A bilateral anodal stimulation in both SI cortices could match the pattern toward a generalized improvement of somatosensory gating. Cathodal stimulation (inhibition) rather than anodal stimulation (activation) in patients with FMS, could also stand for the reversion of the observed gating deterioration on the ipsilateral stimulated hemisphere (Vaseghi et al., 2015).

In contrast with previous studies in healthy participants (Beck et al., 1961), the lack of modulatory effects on the difference S1-S2, and therefore, somatosensory gating of early stages of information processing in our FMS participants remains unclear. One possible explanation is that early stages of information processing (such as those mirrored by P50 and N100 amplitudes) of non-painful stimulation are more difficult to modulate using one session of non-invasive electrical brain stimulation in FMS. In this sense, P50 has been associated with the early coding and processing of non-painful stimuli (Desmedt and Tomberg, 1989; Staines et al., 2014). The impaired P50 sensory gating observed in FMS may reflect a less effective filtering of sensory information, indicating an alteration in early-stage cognitive processing mechanisms (Kofler and Halder, 2014). Indeed, former research suggested the impairment in early attenuation of repetitive sensory input in chronic pain patients as a marker of a generalized deficit on multisensory inhibition (Montoya et al., 2006; Ambrosini et al., 2001; Carrillo-De-La-Peña et al., 2015; Siniatchkin et al., 2003). Regarding the N100 component, it has been suggested to be involved in the triggering of attention and early perceptual processing (Shen et al., 2020). It has also been linked to central integrative processes and is widely considered a marker of attentional modulation (Giard et al., 1988; Näätänen and Picton, 1987). N100 abnormalities in FMS patients are assumed to reflect deficient sensory encoding and/or registration (Choi et al., 2016). Thus, it seems that a single tDCS session might not be sufficient to modify the deep alterations in early stages of sensory gating that characterize a chronic pain condition, such as FMS. Congruently, a meta-analysis demonstrated larger tDCS effects in FMS patients using protocols that lasted four weeks or more (Teixeira-Santos et al., 2022). Alternatively, it could be that auditory and electrical stimulation, as those used in previous studies, could be more powerful sensory modalities for eliciting changes in gating mechanisms after non-invasive brain stimulation (Matsunaga, 2004; Kim et al., 2018). Finally, it is important to reiterate that in our previous study with healthy participants (Montoro et al., 2021), we also did not observe any modulation of sensory gating in early components. Therefore, it is possible that the combination of anodal stimulation over the SI and tactile stimulation to generate gating, may be responsible for the reported results.

Several limitations and future directions in forthcoming studies should not be overlooked. First, it is known that certain parameters substantially influence tDCS outcomes, e.g., electrode montage, stimulation intensity and duration, and stimulation protocol (Jürgens et al., 2012; Kunzelmann et al., 2018). Therefore, the present results must be considered with caution before drawing any firm conclusion about the efficacy of anodal tDCS on sensory gating in FMS patients; as differences between our study results and previous research could be attributable to distinct stimulation protocols and study design. Second, the findings of this study might be biased by the small sample size. Third, the inclusion of 30 electrodes in the statistical analyses increases the risk of type I error and may reduce spatial specificity, which should be addressed in future studies through more targeted region-of-interest or data-driven clustering approaches. Fourth, all our participants were taking pharmacological medication. Although there were no significant differences in medication use between groups, the role of medication in the observed brain activity cannot be completely ruled out. Many participants were under medications such as analgesics or antidepressants, which can alter cortical excitability and affect electrophysiological responses (Minzenberg and Leuchter, 2019). These medications might have modulated baseline neural activity or interacted with tDCS-induced changes, potentially confounding the interpretation of sensory gating effects. Due to the sample size and study design, controlling for medication effects statistically was not feasible. Future studies should aim to include medication-free patients or control for medication variables more rigorously to clarify their impact on neurophysiological outcomes in FMS research. Fifth, it is important to highlight that although the understanding of the electrophysiological effects of tDCS has progressed over the last years, its precise mechanisms of action still have to be unveiled (Chase et al., 2020; Yamada and Sumiyoshi, 2021). TDCS has not only been demonstrated to cause diffuse and widespread effects across cortical functional networks (Bestmann et al., 2015) but also these effects have been shown to depend on the resting-state of the brain at the moment of stimulation (Woods et al., 2016). Furthermore, even though it was ensured that all participants were given the same instructions to rest and relax at the initial adaptation phase, it was not possible to control for the individuals´ expectations, beliefs or thoughts; factors that are also known to influence tDCS outcomes (Gill et al., 2015; Segrave et al., 2014). The absence of a healthy control group also limits the generalization of findings exclusively to the FMS population. To address these limitations, a within-subject crossover design including both active anodal tDCS and sham stimulation, as well as healthy controls, is strongly recommended for future studies. This approach would better control for individual differences, placebo effects, and disease-specific responses, thereby enhancing the reliability and validity of conclusions about tDCS effects on somatosensory processing in FMS patients.

Finally, it is important to acknowledge that only a single tDCS session was applied. While this design was appropriate for exploring fundamental neurophysiological mechanisms, clinical effects of tDCS typically emerge after multiple sessions. Moreover, as we did not examine correlations between LPC sensory gating changes and clinical characteristics such as symptom severity, our conclusions regarding long-term or therapeutic efficacy are limited. The observed changes in LPC amplitudes reflect modulation of late-stage somatosensory processing but represent a neurophysiological effect whose clinical significance remains uncertain. Since LPC is not a direct marker of symptom improvement and its translational relevance to therapeutic outcomes in FMS remains to be established, these findings should be interpreted as evidence of cortical modulation rather than clinical benefit. Future studies integrating both neurophysiological and clinical measures will be crucial to determine the potential therapeutic value of tDCS-induced sensory gating modulation.

5 Conclusion

A single and short (20 min.) session of anodal tDCS (1.5 mA) elicited a significant modulation of the S1-S2 difference, employed as a functional marker of the somatosensory gating process, in FMS. This effect was mirrored by an enhancement of S1-S2 difference—indicating increased inhibition to repetitive somatosensory stimulation—over the right hemisphere (contralateral to the stimulation), and by a reduction of this difference—indicating diminished inhibition—over the left hemisphere. Moreover, we observed that these effects appeared in the later stages of the somatosensory brain response, such as LPC amplitudes, but not in the early brain responses, such as P50 and N100 amplitudes. Thus, it seemed that the effects of anodal tDCS in FMS was mainly restricted to cognitive evaluation, and not to the coding and perceptual processing of bodily information. Importantly, these effects reflect a modulation of cortical processing, but their direct relevance to clinical improvement remains to be determined. Furthermore, given that clinical effects of tDCS seem to appear after several sessions, our findings suggest the need to explore the modulatory effects of tDCS (and other non-invasive brain stimulation techniques) in somatosensory processing after multiple sessions and follow-up sessions. Such research should incorporate both neurophysiological and clinical endpoints to determine whether repeated stimulation can contribute to reversing the maladaptive plasticity often associated with chronic pain syndromes. Finally, future studies should be conducted to explore the effects of both anodal and cathodal brain stimulation aimed at reversing the alterations in brain activity commonly observed in FMS patients.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article are held by the first author and will be made available upon request, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Balearic Islands Ethics Committee (protocol IB3681/18PI). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

JT: Writing – original draft, Visualization, Validation, Resources, Investigation, Formal analysis. CW: Investigation, Validation, Resources, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Visualization, Writing – original draft. PM: Visualization, Investigation, Validation, Conceptualization, Resources, Methodology, Project administration, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. AJ: Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing, Resources. CM: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Investigation, Validation, Resources, Visualization, Methodology, Conceptualization.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by grants (#PSI2017-88388-C4-1-R, #PID2022-140561NB-I00) from Spanish Ministry of Science and European Regional Development Funds (ERDF), a competitive contract (Juan de la Cierva-Formación, FJCI-2016-29088) funded by the Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities, the State Research Agency and the University of the Balearic Islands (author CM), and a competitive postdoctoral contract (PostDoc: Margalida Comas, PD/018/2021) funded by Conselleria de Fons Europeus, Universitat i Cultura of the Balearic Islands Government and the University of the Balearic Islands (author JT).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1607317/full#supplementary-material

References

Allison, T., McCarthy, G., and Wood, C. C. (1992). The relationship between human long-latency somatosensory evoked potentials recorded from the cortical surface and from the scalp. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 84, 301–314. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(92)90082-M

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ambrosini, M. V., Mariucci, G., Bruschelli, G., and Vitali, R. M. (2001). Slow wave sleep and EEG power spectra in the rat: effects of a chronic treatment with the antidepressant desipramine. Neurosci. Lett. 307, 33–36. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01871-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., Alekseichuk, I., Bikson, M., Brockmöller, J., Brunoni, A. R., Chen, R., et al. (2017). Low intensity transcranial electric stimulation: safety, ethical, legal regulatory and application guidelines. Clin. Neurophysiol. 128, 1774–1809. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.06.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., Ambrus, G. G., and Chaieb, L. (2014). Toward unraveling reading–related modulations of tDCS–induced neuroplasticity in the human visual cortex. Front. Psychol. 5:642. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00642

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., Brepohl, N., Poreisz, C., Boros, K., Csifcsak, G., and Paulus, W. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation over somatosensory cortex decreases experimentally induced acute pain perception. Clin. J. Pain 24, 56–63. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0b013e318157233b

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., Terney, D., Kühnl, S., and Paulus, W. (2010). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the motor cortex ameliorates chronic pain and reduces short Intracortical inhibition. J. Pain Symptom Manag. 39, 890–903. doi: 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2009.09.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., and Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto alexithymia scale—I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J. Psychosom. Res. 38, 23–32. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., and Mock, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depres-Sion. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4:561. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bestmann, S., De Berker, A. O., and Bonaiuto, J. (2015). Understanding the behavioural consequences of noninvasive brain stimulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.10.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borckardt, J. J., Romagnuolo, J., Reeves, S. T., Madan, A., Frohman, H., Beam, W., et al. (2011). Feasibility, safety, and effectiveness of transcranial direct current stimulation for decreasing post-ERCP pain: a randomized, sham-controlled, pilot study. Gastrointest. Endosc. 73, 1158–1164. doi: 10.1016/j.gie.2011.01.050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boutros, N. N., and Belger, A. (1999). Midlatency evoked potentials attenuation and augmentation reflect different aspects of sensory gating. Biol. Psychiatry 45, 917–922. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00253-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boutros, N. N., Brockhaus-Dumke, A., Gjini, K., Vedeniapin, A., Elfakhani, M., Burroughs, S., et al. (2009). Sensory-gating deficit of the N100 mid-latency auditory evoked potential in medicated schizophrenia patients. Schizophr. Res. 113, 339–346. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.05.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boutros, N. N., Torello, M. W., Barker, B. A., Tueting, P. A., Wu, S.-C., and Nasrallah, H. A. (1995). The P 50 evoked potential component and mismatch detection in normal volunteers: implications for the study of sensory gating. Psychiatry Res. 57, 83–88. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(95)02637-C

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burckhardt, C. S., Clark, S. R., and Bennett, R. M. (1991). The fibromyalgia impact questionnaire: development and validation. J. Rheumatol. 18, 728–733

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Carrillo-De-La-Peña, M. T., Triñanes, Y., González-Villar, A., Gõmez-Perretta, C., and García-Larrea, L. (2015). Filtering out repetitive auditory stimuli in fibromyalgia: a study of P 50 sensory gating. Eur. J. Pain. U K 19, 576–584. doi: 10.1002/ejp.627

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chase, H. W., Boudewyn, M. A., Carter, C. S., and Phillips, M. L. (2020). Transcranial direct current stimulation: a roadmap for research, from mechanism of action to clinical implementation. Mol. Psychiatry 25, 397–407. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0499-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chinn, S., Caldwell, W., and Gritsenko, K. (2016). Fibromyalgia pathogenesis and treatment options update. Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 20:25. doi: 10.1007/s11916-016-0556-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, W., Lim, M., Kim, J. S., and Chung, C. K. (2016). Habituation deficit of auditory N100m in patients with fibromyalgia. Eur. J. Pain. U K 20, 1634–1643. doi: 10.1002/ejp.883

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummiford, C. M., Nascimento, T. D., Foerster, B. R., Clauw, D. J., Zubieta, J.-K., Harris, R. E., et al. (2016). Changes in resting state functional connectivity after repetitive transcranial direct current stimulation applied to motor cortex in fibromyalgia patients. Arthritis Res. Ther. 18:40. doi: 10.1186/s13075-016-0934-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Pascalis, V. (2004). “On the psychophysiology of extraversion” in On the psychobiology of personality : Essays in Honor of Marvin Zuckerman. ed. R. M. Stelmack (Amsterdam, Boston, MA: Elsevier), 295–327.

Google Scholar

Desmedt, J., and Tomberg, C. (1989). Mapping early somatosensory evoked potentials in selective attention: critical evaluation of control conditions used for titrating by difference the cognitive P 30, P 40, P 100 and N140. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 74, 321–346. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(89)90001-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenstein, E. M., and Eisenstein, D. (2006). A behavioral homeostasis theory of habituation and sensitization: II. Further developments and predictions. Rev. Neurosci. 17:533. doi: 10.1515/REVNEURO.2006.17.5.533

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedrich, J., and Beste, C. (2018). Paradoxical, causal effects of sensory gain modulation on motor inhibitory control – a tDCS, EEG-source localization study. Sci. Rep. 8:17486. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-35879-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritsch, B., Reis, J., Martinowich, K., Schambra, H. M., Ji, Y., Cohen, L. G., et al. (2010). Direct current stimulation promotes BDNF-dependent synaptic plasticity: potential implications for motor learning. Neuron 66, 198–204. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.035

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuerst, D. R., Gallinat, J., and Boutros, N. N. (2007). Range of sensory gating values and test–retest reliability in normal subjects. Psychophysiology 44, 620–626. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00524.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujimoto, S., Yamaguchi, T., Otaka, Y., Kondo, K., and Tanaka, S. (2014). Dual-hemisphere transcranial direct current stimulation improves performance in a tactile spatial discrimination task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 1669–1674. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.12.100

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Giard, M. H., Perrin, F., Pernier, J., and Peronnet, F. (1988). Several attention-related wave forms in auditory areas: a topographic study. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 69, 371–384. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90008-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gill, J., Shah-Basak, P. P., and Hamilton, R. (2015). It’s the thought that counts: examining the task-dependent effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on executive function. Brain Stimulat. 8, 253–259. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.10.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., and Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 55, 468–484. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hämäläinen, H., Kekoni, J., Sams, M., Reinikainen, K., and Näätänen, R. (1990). Human somatosensory evoked potentials to mechanical pulses and vibration: contributions of SI and SII somatosensory cortices to P 50 and P 100 components. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 75, 13–21. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(90)90148-D

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilgenstock, R., Weiss, T., Huonker, R., and Witte, O. W. (2016). Behavioural and neurofunctional impact of transcranial direct current stimulation on somatosensory learning: tDCS and somatosensory learning. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 1277–1295. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23101

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirtz, R., Weiss, T., Huonker, R., and Witte, O. W. (2018). Impact of transcranial direct current stimulation on structural plasticity of the somatosensory system. J. Neurosci. Res. 96, 1367–1379. doi: 10.1002/jnr.24258

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, L., Koslowsky, M., and Lavidor, M. (2012). tDCS polarity effects in motor and cognitive domains: a meta-analytical review. Exp. Brain Res. 216, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2891-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jürgens, T. P., Schulte, A., Klein, T., and May, A. (2012). Transcranial direct current stimulation does neither modulate results of a quantitative sensory testing protocol nor ratings of suprathreshold heat stimuli in healthy volunteers: tDCS in experimental pain. Eur. J. Pain 16, 1251–1263. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2012.00135.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kikkert, S., Mezue, M., O’Shea, J., Henderson Slater, D., Johansen-Berg, H., Tracey, I., et al. (2019). Neural basis of induced phantom limb pain relief: neural basis of PLP relief. Ann. Neurol. 85, 59–73. doi: 10.1002/ana.25371

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, M., Yoon, Y. B., Lee, T. H., Lee, T. Y., and Kwon, J. S. (2018). The effect of tDCS on auditory hallucination and P 50 sensory gating in patients with schizophrenia: a pilot study. Schizophr. Res. 192, 469–470. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.04.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kofler, M., and Halder, W. (2014). Clinical neurophysiology alterations in excitatory and inhibitory brainstem interneuronal circuits in fibromyalgia: evidence of brainstem dysfunction. Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 593–601. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.08.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koyama, S., Nakagawa, K., and Tanaka, S. (2017). Transcranial direct current stimulation over the opercular somatosensory region does not influence experimentally induced pain: a triple blind, sham-controlled study. Neuro Report 28, 158–162. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000723

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunzelmann, K., Meier, L., Grieder, M., Morishima, Y., and Dierks, T. (2018). No effect of transcranial direct current stimulation of the auditory cortex on auditory-evoked potentials. Front. Neurosci. 12:880. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00880

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Labbé, S., Meftah, E.-M., and Chapman, C. E. (2016). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation of primary somatosensory cortex on vibrotactile detection and discrimination. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 1978–1987. doi: 10.1152/jn.00506.2015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lefaucheur, J.-P., Antal, A., Ayache, S. S., Benninger, D. H., Brunelin, J., Cogiamanian, F., et al. (2017). Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Clin. Neurophysiol. 128, 56–92. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.10.087

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lijffijt, M., Moeller, F. G., Boutros, N. N., Burroughs, S., Lane, S. D., Steinberg, J. L., et al. (2009). The role of age, gender, education, and intelligence in P 50, N100, and P 200 auditory sensory gating. J. Psychophysiol. 23, 52–62. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.23.2.52

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lloyd, D. M., Wittkopf, P. G., Arendsen, L. J., and Jones, A. K. P. (2020). Is transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) effective for the treatment of pain in fibromyalgia? A systematic review and Meta-analysis. J. Pain 21, 1085–1100. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2020.01.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenz, J., Minoshima, S., and Casey, K. L. (2003). Keeping pain out of mind: the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in pain modulation. Brain 126, 1079–1091. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Minzenberg, M. J., and Leuchter, A. F. (2019). The effect of psychotropic drugs on cortical excitability and plasticity measured with transcranial magnetic stimulation: implications for psychiatric treatment. J. Affective Disorder. 253, 126–140. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.04.084

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marques, A. P., Santo, A. D. S. D. E., Berssaneti, A. A., Matsutani, L. A., and Yuan, S. L. K. (2017). Prevalence of fibromyalgia: literature review update. Rev. Bras Reumatol. Engl. Ed. 57, 356–363. doi: 10.1016/j.rbre.2017.01.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsunaga, K. (2004). Effect of transcranial DC sensorimotor cortex stimulation on somatosensory evoked potentials in humans. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 456–460. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00362-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McNair, D., Lorr, M., and Droppleman, L. (1971). Manual for the profile of mood states. San Diego: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.

Google Scholar

Melzack, R. (1975). The McGill pain questionnaire: major properties and scoring methods. Pain 1, 277–299. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(75)90044-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mondino, M., Jardri, R., Suaud-Chagny, M.-F., Saoud, M., Poulet, E., and Brunelin, J. (2016). Effects of Fronto-temporal transcranial direct current stimulation on auditory verbal hallucinations and resting-state functional connectivity of the left Temporo-parietal junction in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 42, 318–326. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv114

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Montoro, C. I., Winterholler, C., Terrasa, J. L., and Montoya, P. (2021). Somatosensory gating is modulated by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation. Front. Neurosci. 15, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.651253

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Montoya, P., Sitges, C., García-Herrera, M., Izquierdo, R., Truyols, M., Blay, N., et al. (2005). Abnormal affective modulation of somatosensory brain processing among patients with fibromyalgia. Psychosom. Med. 67, 957–963. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000188401.55394.18

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Montoya, P., Sitges, C., García-Herrera, M., Rodríguez-Cotes, A., Izquierdo, R., Truyols, M., et al. (2006). Reduced brain habituation to somatosensory stimulation in patients with fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheum. 54, 1995–2003. doi: 10.1002/art.21910

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Näätänen, R., and Picton, T. (1987). The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: a review and an analysis of the component structure. Psychophysiology 24, 375–425. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1987.tb00311.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Neeb, L., Bayer, A., Bayer, K.-E., Farmer, A., Fiebach, J. B., Siegmund, B., et al. (2019). Transcranial direct current stimulation in inflammatory bowel disease patients modifies resting-state functional connectivity: a RCT. Brain Stimulat 12, 978–980. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.03.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., Wassermann, E. M., Priori, A., Lang, N., Antal, A., et al. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimulat 1, 206–223. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pacheco-Barrios, K., Cardenas-Rojas, A., Thibaut, A., Costa, B., Ferreira, I., Caumo, W., et al. (2020). Methods and strategies of tDCS for the treatment of pain: current status and future directions. Expert Rev. Med. Devices 17, 879–898. doi: 10.1080/17434440.2020.1816168

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinto, C. B., Teixeira Costa, B., Duarte, D., and Fregni, F. (2018). Transcranial direct current stimulation as a therapeutic tool for chronic pain. J. ECT 34, e36–e50. doi: 10.1097/YCT.0000000000000518

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollonini, L., Miao, H., and Ahn, H. (2020). Longitudinal effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on knee osteoarthritis patients measured by functional infrared spectroscopy: a pilot study. Neurophotonics 7:1. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.7.2.025004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ragert, P., Vandermeeren, Y., Camus, M., and Cohen, L. G. (2008). Improvement of spatial tactile acuity by transcranial direct current stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 119, 805–811. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.12.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahimi, M. D., Fadardi, J. S., Saeidi, M., Bigdeli, I., and Kashiri, R. (2020). Effectiveness of cathodal tDCS of the primary motor or sensory cortex in migraine: a randomized controlled trial. Brain Stimulat 13, 675–682. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2020.02.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rehmann, R., Sczesny-Kaiser, M., Lenz, M., Gucia, T., Schliesing, A., Schwenkreis, P., et al. (2016). Polarity-specific cortical effects of transcranial direct current stimulation in primary somatosensory cortex of healthy humans. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:10. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00208

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rushby, J. A., Barry, R. J., and Doherty, R. J. (2005). Separation of the components of the late positive complex in an ERP dishabituation paradigm. Clin. Neurophysiol. 116, 2363–2380. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.06.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarzi-Puttini, P., Giorgi, V., Marotto, D., and Atzeni, F. (2020). Fibromyalgia: an update on clinical characteristics, aetiopathogenesis and treatment. Nat. Rev. Rheumatol. 16, 645–660. doi: 10.1038/s41584-020-00506-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Segrave, R. A., Arnold, S., Hoy, K., and Fitzgerald, P. B. (2014). Concurrent cognitive control training augments the antidepressant efficacy of tDCS: a pilot study. Brain Stimulat. 7, 325–331. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.12.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, C.-L., Chou, T.-L., Lai, W.-S., Hsieh, M. H., Liu, C.-C., Liu, C.-M., et al. (2020). P 50, N100, and P 200 auditory sensory gating deficits in schizophrenia patients. Front. Psych. 11:868. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00868

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shulman, G. L., Pope, D. L., Astafiev, S. V., McAvoy, M. P., Snyder, A. Z., and Corbetta, M. (2010). Right hemisphere dominance during spatial selective attention and target detection occurs outside the dorsal frontoparietal network. J. Neurosci. 30, 3640–3651. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4085-09.2010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Siniatchkin, M., Kropp, P., and Gerber, W.-D. (2003). What kind of habituation is impaired in migraine patients? Cephalalgia 23, 511–518. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-2982.2003.00434.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., and Lushene, R. E. (1970). The state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI): Test manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Staines, W. R., Popovich, C., Legon, J. K., and Adams, M. S. (2014). Early modality-specific somatosensory cortical regions are modulated by attended visual stimuli: interaction of vision, touch and behavioral intent. Front. Psychol. 5:5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00351

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Teixeira-Santos, A. C., Moreira, C. S., Pereira, D. R., Pinal, D., Fregni, F., Leite, J., et al. (2022). Working memory training coupled with transcranial direct current stimulation in older adults: a randomized controlled experiment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:827188. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.827188

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Terrasa, J. L., Montoya, P., González-Roldán, A. M., and Sitges, C. (2018). Inhibitory control impairment on somatosensory gating due to aging: an event-related potential study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:12. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00280

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thair, H., Holloway, A. L., Newport, R., and Smith, A. D. (2017). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): a beginner’s guide for design and implementation. Front. Neurosci. 11:11. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00641

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thirugnanasambandam, N., Sparing, R., Dafotakis, M., Meister, I. G., Paulus, W., Nitsche, M. A., et al. (2011). Isometric contraction interferes with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)–induced plasticity–evidence of state-dependent neuromodulation in human motor cortex. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 29, 311–320. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2011-0601

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaseghi, B., Zoghi, M., and Jaberzadeh, S. (2015). Differential effects of cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal, motor and somatosensory cortices on cortical excitability and pain perception - a double-blind randomised sham-controlled study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 42, 2426–2437. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13043

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vlcek, P., Bob, P., and Raboch, J. (2014). Sensory disturbances, inhibitory deficits, and the P 50 wave in schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 10, 1309–1315. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S64219

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Volz, M. S., Farmer, A., and Siegmund, B. (2016). Reduction of chronic abdominal pain in patients with inflammatory bowel disease through transcranial direct current stimulation: a randomized controlled trial. Pain 157, 429–437. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000386

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, T., de Graaf, T., Tanner, L., Schuhmann, T., Duecker, F., and Sack, A. T. (2024). Hemispheric asymmetry in TMS-induced effects on spatial attention: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 34, 838–849. doi: 10.1007/s11065-023-09614-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfe, F., Clauw, D. J., Fitzcharles, M.-A., Goldenberg, D. L., Katz, R. S., Mease, P., et al. (2010). The American College of Rheumatology Preliminary Diagnostic Criteria for fibromyalgia and measurement of symptom severity. Arthritis Care Res. 62, 600–610. doi: 10.1002/acr.20140

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, A. J., Antal, A., Bikson, M., Boggio, P. S., Brunoni, A. R., Celnik, P., et al. (2016). A technical guide to tDCS, and related non-invasive brain stimulation tools. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 1031–1048. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.11.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamada, Y., and Sumiyoshi, T. (2021). Neurobiological mechanisms of transcranial direct current stimulation for psychiatric disorders; neurophysiological, chemical, and anatomical considerations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:631838. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.631838

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, C., Yu, B., Zhang, W., Chen, W., Qi, Q., and Miao, Y. (2017). Effiectiveness and safety of transcranial direct current stimulation in fibromyalgia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Rehabil. Med. 49, 2–9. doi: 10.2340/16501977-2179

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: transcranial direct current stimulation, somatosensory cortex, sensory gating, late positive complex, Fibromyalgia Syndrome

Citation: Terrasa JL, Winterholler C, Montoya P, Juan A and Montoro CI (2025) Modulatory effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on sensory gating in Fibromyalgia Syndrome. Front. Psychol. 16:1607317. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1607317

Received: 07 April 2025; Accepted: 28 July 2025;
Published: 19 August 2025.

Edited by:

Nicola Canessa, University Institute of Higher Studies in Pavia, Italy

Reviewed by:

Eduardo Henrique Loreti, Centro Universitário da Grande Dourados (UNIGRAN), Brazil
Danielle Hewitt, University of Oxford, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2025 Terrasa, Winterholler, Montoya, Juan and Montoro. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Pedro Montoya, cGVkcm8ubW9udG95YUB1aWIuZXM=; Casandra I. Montoro, aW1vbnRvcm9AdWphZW4uZXM=

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.