Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 10 September 2025

Sec. Human Developmental Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1620235

Fine motor skills assessment instruments for preschool children with typical development: a scoping review

Bai-Fa Zhang
Bai-Fa Zhang1*Zhi-Cheng LinZhi-Cheng Lin1Chao LiChao Li2
  • 1School of Sports, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
  • 2School of Sports, Qingdao University, Qingdao, China

Purpose: This study aims to systematically evaluate the available scientific knowledge on fine motor skills assessment instruments for typically developing preschool children.

Methods: Relevant literature was systematically retrieved from Web of Science Core Collection, PubMed, Medline, CNKI, and Wanfang databases from their inception to June 9, 2023. Following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR), included literature was compiled and analyzed.

Results: In total, 58 studies reporting 14 instruments met inclusion criteria: performance-based tests (n = 11), informant-based questionnaires (n = 2), and one computer-assisted tool. Based on different types and development purposes, the measurement dimensions and items had varying emphases, and there is no recognized gold standard yet. The reliability of measurements was generally good, while validity needs improvement.

Conclusion: Given the distinct strengths and limitations of available FMS assessment tools, we suggest that selection should align with specific objectives: the ASQ may be most suitable for large-scale screening, the MABC for general FMS assessment in small groups, the Beery VMI for visual-motor integration, and the TIHM for fine motor control assessment.

1 Introduction

Early childhood professionals and curricula have long emphasized the significance of fine motor skills (FMS) (Fischer et al., 2020). The Chinese early learning and development guidelines for preschoolers identify FMS as a key dimension in the holistic development of the child. FMS refer to the use of small muscle movements in the hands and fingers to hold or manipulate objects, requiring precise hand-eye coordination (Suggate et al., 2018). These skills can be delineated into visual motor integration (VMI), also referred to as visuomotor or graphomotor skills, and fine motor coordination (FMC), which relate to separate abilities that follow distinct developmental trajectories (Carlson et al., 2013; Flores et al., 2023a). VMI is defined as the ability to integrate visual–spatial stimuli and attention control with fine motor output, requiring greater hand-eye coordination than FMC (Carlson et al., 2013; Flores et al., 2024). This integration emerges from the combined effects of various cognitive and neuromotor processes, including visual–spatial perception, visual size discrimination, visual retrieval, and orientation discrimination (Smits-Engelsman et al., 2001; Dinehart and Manfra, 2013). In VMI assessments, children are typically required to perform tasks involving writing, drawing, cutting, and folding (Grissmer et al., 2010; Martzog et al., 2019). The development of VMI is an important predictor of children’s handwriting skills, with higher VMI levels associated with better performance on literacy tasks (Suggate et al., 2019; Lu et al., 2024). Evidence also supports a link between VMI and mathematical outcomes, with neuroimaging studies revealing overlapping brain networks underlying both visuospatial and numerical processing (Becker et al., 2014). FMC focuses on finger movement flexibility, requiring rapid and accurate finger movements within specified time constraints, such as inserting coins and threading beads—skills also termed non-graphomotor abilities (Carlson et al., 2013; Flores et al., 2023a). FMC assessment tasks fall into three categories, each measured by completion time: (1) translation tasks, which measure the ability to transfer objects between palm and fingertips, such as inserting coins; (2) shift tasks, which assess the ability to manipulate objects through sequential finger movements, such as threading beads; and (3) rotation tasks, which evaluate the ability to rotate objects using finger coordination, such as turning wooden pegs (Pont et al., 2008). Another component of FMC assessment is grasping, which involves adjusting hand movements based on object shape and size to ensure proper manipulation, such as grasping blocks, chopsticks, and writing implements (Strooband et al., 2023). Grasping forms the foundation for developing complex tool-based movements, with assessment measures typically emphasizing process-oriented aspects of movement execution rather than outcome metrics. Researchers have pre-established multi-level scoring criteria based on the developmental trajectories of holding different tools (chopsticks, pens, etc.), analyzing the developmental level of these skills from the perspective of movement developmental sequences, which facilitates specialized evaluation and guidance for preschool children (Burton and Dancisak, 2000; Payne and Isaacs, 2024). Related research has found significant relationships between early FMC and both concurrent and future language development, with FMC demonstrating the ability to predict subsequent language delays (Choi et al., 2018; Bhat et al., 2022).

National education goals identify FMS as a key aspect of school readiness, given that kindergarten children spend between 36 and 66% (with an average of 46%) of their in-class time engaged in fine motor activities, such as writing, cutting, and manipulative play (Marr et al., 2003; Becker et al., 2014). Furthermore, cognitive and fine motor development are closely linked, with established associations between fine motor skills and crystallized intelligence, memory, and fluid reasoning (Davis et al., 2011). Evidence from neuroimaging studies also suggests strong functional coupling between brain regions once thought to support either exclusively cognitive or exclusively fine motor processes (Stoodley, 2012). While FMS play a critical role in early childhood development, research demonstrates that 10–24% of children exhibit developmental delays in this domain, and an additional 13–40% show risk factors for suboptimal skill acquisition (Strooband et al., 2020). Early identification of FMS developmental delays is crucial for timely intervention (Cameron et al., 2012; Oberklaid et al., 2013). Failure to identify and remediate fine motor skill issues in preschoolers in a timely manner may adversely affect their cognitive development and academic achievement in elementary and middle school (Dinehart and Manfra, 2013). Children with fine motor skill impairments commonly experience peer rejection, reduced self-efficacy, and lower self-esteem. These psychological consequences lead to avoidance of activities that highlight their impairments, such as play and social interaction (Gaul and Issartel, 2016). Given the need for timely intervention, effective and reliable assessment tools for FMS are essential (Strooband et al., 2023). Current FMS assessment tools can be broadly categorized into performance-based tests and informant-based questionnaires. Performance-based FMS tests require examiners to evaluate children’s discrete motor abilities through direct observation of structured tasks, using predetermined criteria (Matheis and Estabillo, 2018). These tests require qualified professionals with specialized training in administration, scoring, and interpretation protocols to ensure reliable assessment results, limiting their accessibility primarily to researchers and occupational therapists. Informant-based questionnaires collect FMS ratings from adults who regularly observe the child, such as parents, teachers, and caregivers (Lalor et al., 2016). These tests offer efficiency and cost-effectiveness for large-scale screening, while they are limited by potential observer bias and reduced utility for educational intervention planning (Howard et al., 2019; Chen et al., 2021).

However, the lack of clarity and empirical evidence for distinguishing between the overlapping components of FMS has resulted in considerable variation in how different instruments conceptualize and measure it. Thus, it is difficult for early childhood educators and researchers to know which assessment tool is most appropriate for accurately identifying children who struggle with FMS development. This evaluation is particularly challenging as there is a lack of clarity in the previous review regarding the specific items, validity and reliability of the available FMS measures. A defined literature review is essential to identify effective assessment tools that enable early childhood educators to detect fine motor delays and support children’s development in educational settings. Therefore, this scoping review aimed to systematically map FMS assessment instruments for typically developing preschool children described in the literature. The following research question guided this scoping review: What is the current state of international research on fine motor skills assessment instruments for typically developing preschool children?

2 Methods

2.1 Search strategy

The main English-language literature databases included Web of Science Core Collection, PubMed, and Medline, while China National Knowledge Infrastructure (CNKI) and Wanfang Database served as the primary Chinese literature sources. The search period covered from database inception to June 9, 2023. The English search terms or keywords consisted of three groups: ① Population: (child* OR preschool OR pre-school* OR boy OR girl) NOT (disorder* OR illness OR disease* OR disab*② Fine motor skills: “fine motor” OR “visual motor” OR graphomot). ③Methodology: tool OR instrument OR assessment OR evaluation OR measurement. Boolean operator “AND” was used between groups in both searches. Additionally, reference lists of included studies were manually reviewed to identify any relevant literature that might have been missed through the database searches.

2.2 Identifying relevant studies

This review included full-text articles published in peer-reviewed journals in English or Chinese. Studies were eligible if they involved original research on the development, validation, or application of fine motor skills assessment instruments for preschool children. Participants were limited to typically developing children aged 3–6 years, excluding those with neurodevelopmental conditions (e.g., autism, cerebral palsy). Studies were excluded if they were non-full-text articles, non-English or non-Chinese literature, books, book chapters, unpublished papers, conference proceedings, dissertations, non-empirical research (such as reviews, commentaries, and book reviews), or studies with participants having a mean age below 3 years or above 6 years.

2.3 Study selection

Following duplicate removal, two researchers independently screened titles and abstracts for preliminary eligibility. Full texts of potentially eligible studies were then retrieved and reviewed thoroughly. Any disagreements during the screening process were resolved through discussion with a third researcher to determine final inclusion. Information extracted from the included literature comprised: author, country, year, study type, sample characteristics, assessment instruments name, tool type, content measured, evaluation method, reliability and validity, and research findings relevant to the main research questions.

3 Results

3.1 Characteristics of the included studies

The initial search yielded 2,647 articles. Following rigorous screening according to the inclusion and exclusion criteria, 58 studies were ultimately included: 20 focused on instruments development and validation, 16 addressed cross-cultural adaptation (localized standardization across different countries), 16 reported cross-sectional investigations, 3 conducted prospective studies, and 3 implemented non-randomized controlled trials. These studies involved 14 different fine motor skills assessment instruments for preschool children, categorized as performance-based tests (n = 11), informant-based questionnaires (n = 2), and computer-assisted assessments (n = 1). These instruments originated from researchers in the United States (n = 7), China (n = 3), the United Kingdom (n = 1), Canada (n = 1), Switzerland (n = 1), and Australia (n = 1), as shown in Table 1. The literature screening process is illustrated in Figure 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Overview of assessment instruments for fine motor skills in preschool children.

Figure 1
Flowchart depicting the screening process for articles. It starts with retrieving 2,647 articles from databases, removing 1,512 duplicates, and initially screening 1,135 articles. After exclusions, 200 articles underwent secondary screening, with 58 included in the final analysis. Ultimately, 14 fine motor development assessment tools for preschool children were identified.

Figure 1. Flowchart of literature screening process.

3.2 Characteristics of the FMS assessment instruments

Seven assessment instruments were comprehensive developmental tools that include fine motor skills as one of multiple domains: Ages and Stages Questionnaires (ASQ), Developmental Motor Screening Questionnaire (DMSQi), Movement Assessment Battery for Children (MABC), Bruininks–Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency (BOT), Peabody Developmental Motor Scales (PDMS), Zurich Neuromotor Assessment (ZNA), and the Evaluation Index System of Physical and Cognitive Abilities for Children Aged 3–6. Regarding the applicable age range, 8 instruments were specifically designed for preschool children, 4 tools covered school-aged children, and 2 tools extended to adults. In terms of assessment content, the 14 instruments collectively evaluated two categories of fine motor skills: visual motor integration (n = 9) and fine motor coordination (n = 12). Regarding administration time, 11 tools reported testing duration. The PDMS required the most time (20–30 min), while the Nine-Hole Peg Test (9-HPT) required the least (1–3 min). Across cross-sectional, prospective, and non-randomized controlled studies, the most frequently used assessment tools were the Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual–Motor Integration (Beery VMI, n = 10), MABC (n = 5), and ASQ (n = 4) (Table 2).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Basic characteristics of assessment instruments for fine motor skills in preschool children.

3.3 Reliability and validity of the FMS assessment instruments

Regarding reliability testing, 8 tools were validated for internal consistency, using Cronbach’s α and split-half reliability as measurement indicators. Eight instruments were validated for test–retest reliability, using the Intraclass Correlation Coefficient and Pearson correlation coefficient as measurement indicators. Five instruments were validated for inter-rater reliability, using Kappa coefficient, Intraclass Correlation Coefficient, and Pearson correlation coefficient as indicators. For validity testing, 8 instruments were validated for criterion validity. Measurement indicators included true positive rate, Pearson correlation coefficient, and differences in area under the ROC curve between the target tool and external criteria. External criteria included the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Denver Developmental Screening Test, Beijing Gesell Developmental Schedule, Test of Visual Perceptual Skills, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire, assessment tools for writing readiness, and the Taylor Hand Function Test. Six instruments were validated for construct validity, with measurement indicators including Infit Mean Square from Rasch analysis, chi-square to degrees of freedom ratio (χ2/df), and Comparative Fit Index (CFI) from confirmatory factor analysis. Additionally, 5 instruments were introduced and standardized across multiple countries, with reliability and validity testing conducted in typically developing preschool populations. These tools include ASQ, Beery VMI, MABC, PDMS, and BOT. Among them, the first four were introduced by Chinese researchers and applied to fine motor development assessment of typically developing preschool children in China (Table 3).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Reliability and validity of assessment instruments for fine motor skills in preschool children.

4 Discussion

4.1 Informant-based questionnaires

There are two questionnaire instruments used for assessing preschool children’s fine motor skills (Table 1), among which the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ) is more widely applied. ASQ was developed in 1995 by the Human Development Center and Early Intervention Research Institute at the University of Oregon, with the latest version ASQ-3 released in 2009. It is suitable for children aged 1–66 months and focuses on screening for early developmental delays, with the child’s caregiver serving as the proxy reporter. ASQ is divided into 21 age groups, with each age group questionnaire containing 30 questions evaluating children’s developmental progress from five comprehensive aspects: communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving, and personal-social skills. The fine motor assessment comprises six questions related to VMI and FMC, with response options of “yes” (10 points), “sometimes” (5 points), and “no” (0 points). The sum of the six scores constitutes the fine motor development score. ASQ has been translated into multiple languages and widely used worldwide (Heo et al., 2008; Filgueiras et al., 2013; Hsiao et al., 2017), demonstrating good reliability and validity. It was introduced to China by Bian Xiaoyan, who completed its localization standardization and normative reliability and validity testing (Wei et al., 2015). This questionnaire has numerous advantages, including simplicity, speed, low cost, and flexibility, making it suitable for large-scale child screening. However, it provides too little information and cannot eliminate the subjective tendencies of caregivers when evaluating children.

Furthermore, Chen et al. (2021) designed the Developmental Motor Screening Questionnaire (DMSQi) as a culturally adapted, parent- or caregiver-reported screening tool for motor development in children aged 3 months to 6 years. The purpose of the DMSQi is to enable clinicians to identify children with motor development delays early. The DMSQi consists of 78 items, with 42 measuring the fine motor domain across VMI and FMC subdomains. Items are scored on a four-point scale (1–4) reflecting developmental progression: not yet developed (1), beginning (2), intermediate (3), and proficient (4). The complete assessment requires approximately 10 min for administration. The fine motor domain and its subdomains of the DMSQi demonstrated excellent internal consistency, with Cronbach’s alpha values ranging from 0.94 to 0.97, and strong test–retest reliability (ICC = 0.95). Concurrent validity assessment using the PDMS as the reference standard demonstrated strong correlation coefficients ranging from r = 0.85 to 0.91 (Chen et al., 2021). A notable strength of the DMSQi is its integration of illustrated and textual descriptions for each item, facilitating more accurate and efficient item interpretation compared to text-only formats. The recruitment of participants exclusively from New Taipei City constitutes a limitation, potentially restricting the generalizability of findings to broader populations of typically developing children.

The structure of preschool children’s fine motor skills is relatively complex, and the measurement dimensions varying according to assessment instruments and developmental objectives. The informant-based questionnaires included in this review assess grasping components of FMC but lack evaluation of other essential FMC components. This is because informant-based questionnaires are widely used for large-scale, rapid screening of children with developmental delays to facilitate early intervention. Since proxy reporters are typically children’s caregivers, these instruments’ items are more closely aligned with daily life situations (e.g., “Can your child unbutton multiple buttons?”), thereby reducing measurement errors due to information asymmetry from caregivers. In contrast, other fine motor control components (e.g., turning pegs, threading beads) require specific operational contexts and standardized tools, thereby presenting significant challenges for data collection.

4.2 Performance-based tests

4.2.1 Fine motor assessments

There are 11 performance-based tests used for assessing typical developing preschool children’s fine motor skills, which fall into two categories. The first category comprises specialized fine motor assessments: the Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (Beery VMI), Nine-Hole Peg Test (9-HPT), Test of In-Hand Manipulation (TIHM), Functional Dexterity Test (FDT), Grip Scale (GS), and Fine Motor Growth Assessment (FINGA). Among these instruments, the Beery VMI is the most widely used. Originally developed by Beery in 1967, it has been revised repeatedly, with the sixth edition (Beery VMI-6) released in 2010. The Beery VMI is standardized for individuals aged 2 years through adulthood and primarily assesses VMI abilities during critical developmental stages. The Beery VMI employs a geometric figure-copying format comprising 30 dichotomously scored items, with a 21-item short form available for children aged 2 to 7 years that requires approximately 10 min for administration (Flores et al., 2023b). Additionally, Beery VMI includes two supplementary tests for visual perception and motor coordination, each taking 5 min to administer. Marr and Cermak (2002) and Brown et al. (2009) conducted comprehensive reliability and validity testing for Beery VMI, using split-half reliability, ICC, correlation coefficients, and MnSq values to determine Beery VMI’s internal consistency, test–retest reliability, inter-rater reliability, and construct validity. Research shows that Beery VMI’s split-half reliability ranges between 0.82–0.92, test–retest reliability ICC value is 0.92, correlation coefficients range between 0.92–0.98, and MnSq values range between 0.77–1.30, indicating that Beery VMI has good psychometric properties. Due to its unique testing format, which is not limited by cultural or educational level, Beery VMI has good cross-cultural adaptability and has been widely used in different countries and linguistic-cultural environments (Josman et al., 2006; Lim et al., 2015). Chinese scholars have introduced this tool in Taiwan (Mao et al., 1999), Hong Kong (Ng et al., 2015), and mainland China (Lu and Zhao, 1996; Hu et al., 1999; Lan and Li, 2001; Zhang and Lin, 2001; Xu et al., 2019) and completed standardization research for urban children in Shaanxi Province (Shi et al., 1995). However, there is no Chinese normative study for Beery VMI-6 yet. Beery VMI’s advantages lie in its testing convenience and cross-cultural applicability, but the tool mainly evaluates visual motor integration skills and cannot comprehensively reflect children’s fine motor development level.

The 9-HPT, TIHM, and FDT are timed pegboard-based assessments of FMC, with only the TIHM specifically designed for children aged 3 to 6 years (Aaron and Jansen, 2003; Pont et al., 2008; Feys et al., 2017). The three pegboard tests demonstrate efficient administration times: the 9-HPT requires less than 5 min, the TIHM takes 5 to 7 min for administration and scoring, and the FDT requires 15 s to 2 min (Smith et al., 2000; Pont et al., 2008). The 9-HPT and TIHM use a 9-hole pegboard, whereas the FDT uses a board with 16 peg holes arranged in a 4 × 4 grid. The 9-HPT requires children to insert nine pegs individually into a pegboard and subsequently remove them, completing two trials with the dominant hand. The faster trial time serves as the assessment score. The TIHM test comprises five tasks. The first task requires children to use their fingertips to rotate five figurines 180 degrees onto their heads and return them to their original holes. Tasks 2 through 5 are translation-with-stabilization activities in which children pick up two, three, four, or five pegs, respectively, using their fingertips, transfer them to the palm, and replace them in the pegboard. Performance measures include completion time, number of dropped or externally stabilized pegs, and quality of in-hand manipulation skills. The FDT measures completion time in seconds for one-handed peg turning. Five-second penalties are added for each instance of compensatory supination or board contact for assistance. When a peg is dropped, a 10-s penalty is applied and timing is paused. The child retrieves the peg, replaces it in its unturned position, and continues from that point, with timing resumed from where it stopped. These assessment tools are efficiently administered, procedurally straightforward, and commercially available. The TIHM’s assessment of both translation and rotation components of FMC, unlike the 9-HPT and FDT which measure only rotation, better correlates test results with children’s everyday functional abilities (Pont et al., 2009).

The Grasp Scale (GS), developed by Schneck, evaluates grasping aspects of FMC through 10 developmentally ordered patterns. The GS is recommended for documenting individual children’s grip patterns and their developmental changes, as well as for designing and evaluating interventions (Burton and Dancisak, 2000). However, it cannot evaluate other essential grasping skills required by the Chinese Learning and Development Guidelines for Children Aged 3–6, such as the proper use of scissors and chopsticks.

Strooband et al. (2023) developed the Fine Motor Growth Assessment (FINGA), an observational tool that measures both FMC and VMI in children aged 3 to 5 years by rating their performance on two standardized tasks: an individual paper plane building task and a group card copying task. This test takes approximately 14–20 min to complete. The test offers two primary advantages: its clear alignment with activities naturally occurring within early childhood education contexts and its direct application in informing evidence-based planning for children’s learning and development. The limitation of FINGA is that it has not yet been validated when administered by early childhood educators in practice.

4.2.2 Subscales of fundamental motor skill assessment

The second category of performance-based tests includes subscales of fundamental motor skill assessments, such as the Zurich Neuromotor Assessment (ZNA), Bruininks-Oseretsky Test (BOT), Peabody Developmental Motor Scales (PDMS), Movement Assessment Battery for Children (MABC), and the Evaluation Index System of Physical and Cognitive Abilities for Children aged 3 to 6. The fine motor subscales of the ZNA, MABC, BOT, and PDMS are internationally recognized assessment tools for evaluating fine motor skills, requiring testers to possess foundational knowledge of test theory and principles, standardized training, and familiarity with the child participant’s background (Cools et al., 2009). The ZNA measures FMC exclusively, while the MABC, BOT, and PDMS assess both FMC and VMI. The PDMS is specifically designed for children aged 0 to 6 years, while the remaining instruments encompass broader age ranges spanning both preschool children and adolescents. For children in the 3–6 year age band, the MABC provides differentiated content and the ZNA reduces the number of repetitions, whereas the BOT employs identical tasks across all ages. The MABC is the most frequently used instrument among the aforementioned measures, comprising the fewest assessment items and requiring the shortest administration time, typically under 5 min (Flores et al., 2023b).

Lv et al. (2022) recently developed an assessment instrument, the Evaluation Index System of Physical and Cognitive Abilities for Children aged 3–6. The fine motor subscale of this assessment instrument comprises four tasks: Dotting in Circles, Tracing a Path, Bead Stringing, and Figure Copying. It demonstrated satisfactory internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.81), strong test–retest reliability (r = 0.95), and strong inter-rater reliability (r = 0.96). However, the validation samples for this instrument were limited to 4- to 6-year-old children from the Shanghai region, excluding 3-year-old children and lacking national representation, thereby limiting generalizability.

4.3 Computer-assisted assessment tools

The Computerized VMI Assessment Tool Using Basic Strokes (C-VMI), developed by the National Taichung University of Education in Taiwan, is used to measure visual motor integration ability in Taiwanese preschool children, focusing on screening and intervention for children with insufficient writing readiness (Li et al., 2018). C-VMI includes 34 basic strokes announced by the Taiwan education department, requiring children to control an electronic pen on a touch panel to copy the strokes. It completes scoring by real-time analysis of stroke parameters (such as path length, average speed, pressure, presence of pauses or tremors, etc.) to determine whether children have writing difficulties. The developers recruited 551 preschool children in Taiwan for validity testing, using the evaluation results from preschool education experts as validity criteria, and found that the tool has good validity (true positive rate = 68.1% ~ 90.2%). C-VMI’s advantages lie in its objective and standardized assessment process, simplicity, and convenience, with test content conforming to Chinese local culture. However, this tool has only been applied in research with Taiwanese preschool children and has not been used in studies in other regions. Additionally, it mainly evaluates children’s visual motor integration ability and cannot comprehensively assess children’s fine motor development level.

4.4 Reliability and validity

An important prerequisite for motor development research is ensuring the reliability and accuracy of assessment tools. Reliability refers to the dependability of tests, manifested as stability and consistency of test results. Most instruments included in this review demonstrated good internal consistency (α > 0.65 or s > 0.7) and test–retest reliability (ICC > 0.60) but lacked data on inter-rater reliability. Among them, ASQ, Beery VMI, and the Evaluation Index System of Physical and Cognitive Abilities for Children Aged 3–6 underwent the most comprehensive reliability testing in Chinese preschool populations, covering internal consistency, test–retest reliability, and inter-rater reliability. Notably, MABC, frequently used by Chinese researchers (Hua et al., 2012; Ma et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2020) for assessing preschool children’s fine motor skills, includes only three relevant test tasks (threading beads, inserting coins, and drawing tasks) that measure two distinct fine motor domains (FMC and VMI). This results in questionable internal consistency, requiring appropriate revisions. For instance, Hua et al. (2012) found that removing the drawing task significantly improved the task homogeneity of MABC. Inter-rater reliability, as an important indicator for ensuring standardized test administration, is crucial for verifying the stability of process-oriented assessment tools. Grasping tasks for assessing FMC represent typical process-oriented evaluation methodologies. However, among the instruments that incorporate such tasks examined in this study, only the ASQ and GS reported inter-rater reliability, with the GS demonstrating values far below the acceptable threshold (r > 0.85). Burton and Dancisak (2000) attributed GS’s low inter-rater reliability to complex grading standards for different pencil grips and lack of example guidance, increasing identification difficulty for testers.

Therefore, the measurement performance of grasping tasks assessment tools in preschool populations still requires further validation. Validity refers to the degree of consistency between actual and expected results of a test or scale, reflecting the accuracy and scientific nature of measured results. Regarding validity testing of the tools included in this review, except for Beery VMI and MABC, most assessment tools have not undergone comprehensive validity testing in typically developing preschool populations, relying solely on either construct validity or criterion validity testing methods. Construct validity, an important indicator for verifying the degree of conformity between assessment tool measurement dimensions and the construct dimensions being measured, is now tested using Rasch models from Item Response Theory rather than being limited to statistical methods from Classical Test Theory, particularly for cross-cultural adaptation. Chien and Bond (2009) tested the construct validity of PDMS in Taiwanese children using the Rasch model and found that four grasping tasks items showed poor fit and ceiling effects, indicating the necessity of revising this tool for use with Chinese children. Additionally, due to the lack of a recognized effective gold standard, the external criteria for tools included in this review are diverse, with significant differences in assessment content and indicators, hindering horizontal comparison of criterion validity between different tools and their widespread promotion. For example, cross-cultural adaptation studies of ASQ (Heo et al., 2008; Wei et al., 2015; Rubio-Codina et al., 2016) reported criterion validity using Denver, Beijing Gesell, and Bayley tools as external criteria, showing polarized correlations between ASQ and these three types of criteria.

5 Conclusion

This scoping review conducted a comprehensive search and analysis of literature related to fine motor skills assessment instruments for preschool children. The findings reveal that Performance-based tests predominate among preschool fine motor skills assessment instruments, with varying emphases on measurement dimensions and items. There is currently no recognized gold standard, and the overall validity of existing tools requires improvement. Given that FMS assessment tools each offer distinct strengths and limitations, tool selection should align with specific research objectives. For large-scale screening of early developmental delays, the ASQ is widely recommended given its simplicity, speed, low cost, and flexibility, as evidenced by its translation into multiple languages and extensive worldwide implementation. For assessing general FMS in small preschool groups, the internationally recognized MABC is particularly recommended, as it measures both FMC and VMI using the fewest items and shortest administration time among comparable instruments. When the focus is specifically on VMI, the widely used Beery VMI is recommended given its combination of good psychometric properties, convenience, and demonstrated cross-cultural applicability. For FMC specific assessments, the commercially available TIHM is recommended because it evaluates multiple FMC components including translation and rotation, is designed for children aged 3–6 years, and offers efficient administration with straightforward procedures.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

B-FZ: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft. Z-CL: Data curation, Writing – review & editing, Methodology. CL: Data curation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by The National Social Science Fund of China (grant number 23CTY017) and Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities, Southwest University (grant number SWU2309726).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank staff and faculty at the School of Sports at Southwest University.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript. Generative AI was used to refine language, style, and grammar throughout the manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aaron, D. H., and Jansen, C. W. S. (2003). Development of the functional dexterity test (FDT): construction, validity, reliability, and normative data. J. Hand Ther. 16, 12–21. doi: 10.1016/s0894-1130(03)80019-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Avi-Itzhak, T., and Obler, D. R. (2008). Clinical value of the VMI supplemental tests: a modified replication study. Optom. Vis. Sci. 85, 1007–1011. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0b013e318188891b

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bart, O., Hajami, D., and Bar-Haim, Y. (2007). Predicting school adjustment from motor abilities in kindergarten. Infant Child Dev. 16, 597–615. doi: 10.1002/icd.514

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, D. R., Miao, A., Duncan, R., and McClelland, M. M. (2014). Behavioral self-regulation and executive function both predict visuomotor skills and early academic achievement. Early Child Res. Q. 29, 411–424. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhat, A. N., Boulton, A. J., and Tulsky, D. S. (2022). A further study of relations between motor impairment and social communication, cognitive, language, functional impairments, and repetitive behavior severity in children with ASD using the SPARK study dataset. Autism Res. 15, 1156–1178. doi: 10.1002/aur.2711

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T., and Rodger, S. (2008). Validity of the developmental test of visual-motor integration supplemental developmental test of visual perception. Percept. Mot. Skills 106, 659–678. doi: 10.2466/pms.106.3.659-678

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T., Unsworth, C., and Lyons, C. (2009). An evaluation of the construct validity of the developmental test of visual-motor integration using the rasch measurement model. Aust. Occup. Ther. J. 56, 393–402. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1630.2009.00811.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burton, A. W., and Dancisak, M. J. (2000). Grip form and graphomotor control in preschool children. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 54, 9–17. doi: 10.5014/ajot.54.1.9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, C. E., Brock, L. L., Murrah, W. M., Bell, L. H., Worzalla, S. L., Grissmer, D., et al. (2012). Fine motor skills and executive function both contribute to kindergarten achievement. Child Dev. 83, 1229–1244. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01768.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, A. G., Rowe, E., and Curby, T. W. (2013). Disentangling fine motor skills’ relations to academic achievement: the relative contributions of visual-spatial integration and visual-motor coordination. J. Genet. Psychol. 174, 514–533. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2012.717122

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Charafeddine, L., Sinno, D., Ammous, F., Yassin, W., Al-Shaar, L., and Mikati, M. A. (2013). Ages and stages questionnaires: adaptation to an Arabic speaking population and cultural sensitivity. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 17, 471–478. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2013.03.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, C.-L., Chen, P.-H., Chang, C.-J., Wang, C.-C., Hsieh, Y.-W., Huang, H.-H., et al. (2021). An illustration-based developmental motor screening questionnaire for young children: development and a rasch analysis. Infant Child Dev. 30, 1–14. doi: 10.1002/icd.2229

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chien, C.-W., and Bond, T. G. (2009). Measurement properties of fine motor scale of Peabody developmental motor scales-second edition: a rasch analysis. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 88, 376–386. doi: 10.1097/phm.0b013e318198a7c9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, B., Leech, K. A., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Nelson, C. A. (2018). Development of fine motor skills is associated with expressive language outcomes in infants at high and low risk for autism spectrum disorder. J. Neurodev. Disord. 10:14. doi: 10.1186/s11689-018-9231-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chui, M. M. Y., Ng, A. M. Y., Fong, A. K. H., Lin, L. S. Y., and Ng, M. W. F. (2007). Differences in the fine motor performance of children in Hong Kong and the United States on the Bruininks-Oseretsky test of motor proficiency. Hong Kong J. Occup. Ther. 17, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/S1569-1861(07)70002-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coetzee, D., Pienaar, A. E., and Van Wyk, Y. (2020). Relationship between academic achievement, visual-motor integration, gender and socio-economic status: north-west child health integrated with learning and development study. S. Afr. J. Child. Educ. 10, 1–11. doi: 10.4102/sajce.v10i1.646

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cools, W., Martelaer, K. D., Samaey, C., and Andries, C. (2009). Movement skill assessment of typically developing preschool children: a review of seven movement skill assessment tools. J. Sports Sci. Med. 8, 154–168.

Google Scholar

Daly, C. J., Kelley, G. T., and Krauss, A. (2003). Relationship between visual-motor integration and handwriting skills of children in kindergarten: a modified replication study. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 57, 459–462. doi: 10.5014/ajot.57.4.459

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Darrah, J., Senthilselvan, A., and Magill-Evans, J. (2009). Trajectories of serial motor scores of typically developing children: implications for clinical decision making. Infant Behav. Dev. 32, 72–78. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, E. E., Pitchford, N. J., and Limback, E. (2011). The interrelation between cognitive and motor development in typically developing children aged 4-11 years is underpinned by visual processing and fine manual control: cognitive and motor development. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 569–584. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02018.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vries, L., van Hartingsveldt, M. J., Cup, E. H. C., Nijhuis-van der Sanden, M. W. G., and de Groot, I. J. M. (2015). Evaluating fine motor coordination in children who are not ready for handwriting: which test should we take? Occup. Ther. Int. 22, 61–70. doi: 10.1002/oti.1385

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinehart, L., and Manfra, L. (2013). Associations between low-income children’s fine motor skills in preschool and academic performance in second grade. Early Educ. Dev. 24, 138–161. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2011.636729

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feys, P., Lamers, I., Francis, G., Benedict, R., Phillips, G., LaRocca, N., et al. (2017). The nine-hole peg test as a manual dexterity performance measure for multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler. J. 23, 711–720. doi: 10.1177/1352458517690824

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Filgueiras, A., Pires, P., Maissonette, S., and Landeira-Fernandez, J. (2013). Psychometric properties of the brazilian-adapted version of the ages and stages questionnaire in public child daycare centers. Early Hum. Dev. 89, 561–576. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.02.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, U., Suggate, S. P., and Stoeger, H. (2020). The implicit contribution of fine motor skills to mathematical insight in early childhood. Front. Psychol. 11:1143. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01143

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Flores, P., Coelho, E., Mourão-Carvalhal, M. I., and Forte, P. (2023a). Association between motor and math skills in preschool children with typical development: systematic review. Front. Psychol. 14:1105391. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1105391

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Flores, P., Coelho, E., Mourão-Carvalhal, M. I., and Forte, P. M. (2023b). Preliminary adaptation of motor tests to evaluate fine motor skills associated with mathematical skills in preschoolers. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 13, 1330–1361. doi: 10.3390/ejihpe13070098

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Flores, P., Coelho, E., Mourão-Carvalhal, M. I., and Forte, P. (2024). Motor activities to improve maths performance in pre-school children with typical development. Front. Psychol. 15:1332741. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1332741

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaul, D., and Issartel, J. (2016). Fine motor skill proficiency in typically developing children: on or off the maturation track? Hum. Mov. Sci. 46, 78–85. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2015.12.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gharaei, E., Iran, T., Shojaei, M., Iran, T., Daneshfar, A., and Iran, T. (2019). The validity and reliability of the bruininks–oseretsky test of motor proficiency, 2nd edition brief form, in preschool children. Ann. Appl. Sport Sci. 7, 3–12. doi: 10.29252/aassjournal.7.2.3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grissmer, D., Grimm, K. J., Aiyer, S. M., Murrah, W. M., and Steele, J. S. (2010). Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: two new school readiness indicators. Dev. Psychol. 46, 1008–1017. doi: 10.1037/a0020104

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heo, K. H., Squires, J., and Yovanoff, P. (2008). Cross-cultural adaptation of a pre-school screening instrument: comparison of korean and US populations. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 52, 195–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007.01000.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, S. J., Neilsen-Hewett, C., de Rosnay, M., Vasseleu, E., and Melhuish, E. (2019). Evaluating the viability of a structured observational approach to assessing early self-regulation. Early Child Res. Q. 48, 186–197. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.03.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsiao, C., Richter, L., Makusha, T., Matafwali, B., van Heerden, A., and Mabaso, M. (2017). Use of the ages and stages questionnaire adapted for South Africa and Zambia. Child Care Health Dev. 43, 59–66. doi: 10.1111/cch.12413

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, Y., Deng, J., Zhang, J., and Cao, J. (1999). Assessment of results of visual–motion integration test in 517 children. Chin. J. Sch. Health 20, 362–363.

Google Scholar

Hua, J., Wu, Z., Gu, G.-X., and Meng, W. (2012). Assessment on the application of “movement assessment battery” for children. Chin. J. Epidemiol. 33, 1010–1015.

Google Scholar

Josman, N., Abdallah, T. M., and Engel-Yeger, B. (2006). A comparison of visual-perceptual and visual-motor skills between palestinian and israeli children. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 60, 215–225. doi: 10.5014/ajot.60.2.215

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kakebeeke, T. H., Knaier, E., Chaouch, A., Caflisch, J., Rousson, V., Largo, R. H., et al. (2018). Neuromotor development in children. Part 4: new norms from 3 to 18 years. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 60, 810–819. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13793

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kakebeeke, T. H., Lanzi, S., Zysset, A. E., Arhab, A., Messerli-Bürgy, N., Stuelb, K., et al. (2017). Association between body composition and motor performance in preschool children. Obes. Facts 10, 420–431. doi: 10.1159/000477406

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kambas, A., and Aggeloussis, N. (2006). Construct validity of the bruininks-oseretsky test of motor proficiency–short form for a sample of greek preschool and primary school children. Percept. Mot. Skills 102, 65–72. doi: 10.2466/pms.102.1.65-75

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerstjens, J. M., Bos, A. F., ten Vergert, E. M. J., de Meer, G., Butcher, P. R., and Reijneveld, S. A. (2009). Support for the global feasibility of the ages and stages questionnaire as developmental screener. Early Hum. Dev. 85, 443–447. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2009.03.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokštejn, J., Musálek, M., and Tufano, J. J. (2017). Are sex differences in fundamental motor skills uniform throughout the entire preschool period? PLoS One 12, e0176556–e0176510. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0176556

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lalor, A., Brown, T., and Murdolo, Y. (2016). Relationship between children’s performance-based motor skills and child, parent, and teacher perceptions of children’s motor abilities using self/informant-report questionnaires. Aust. Occup. Ther. J. 63, 105–116. doi: 10.1111/1440-1630.12253

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lan, L., and Li, G. (2001). The compilation of the VMI and its supplement test norms for children in Xi’an city. J. Xi’an Med. Univ. 22, 159–161.

Google Scholar

Li, B., Lin, L., Dong, Q., and Claes, V. H. (2002). The development of fine motors and their relations to children’s academic achievement. Acta Psychol. Sin. 34:52.

Google Scholar

Li, C.-H., Wu, H.-M., Kuo, B.-C., Yang, Y.-M., Lin, C.-K., and Wang, W.-H. (2018). The validity of computerized visual motor integration assessment using Chinese basic strokes. Interact. Learn. Environ. 26, 1074–1089. doi: 10.1080/10494820.2018.1442867

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, C. Y., Tan, P. C., Koh, C., Koh, E., Guo, H., Yusoff, N. D., et al. (2015). Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration (beery-VMI): lessons from exploration of cultural variations in visual-motor integration performance of preschoolers. Child Care Health Dev. 41, 213–221. doi: 10.1111/cch.12190

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, H., Leung, F. K. S., and Fan, Z. (2024). A meta-analysis on the relation between handwriting and visual-motor integration. Learn. Individ. Differ. 109:102404. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2023.102404

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, A., and Zhao, W. (1996). Comparison of the VMI developmental level in city and countryside preschoolers. Chin. J. Child Health Care 4, 219–220.

Google Scholar

Lv, H., Dong, C., Song, Y., and Ji, L. (2022). Construction and empirical study on evaluation index system of bodily intelligence for children aged 3–6 years old. J. Phys. Educ. 29, 113–121. doi: 10.16237/j.cnki.cn44-1404/g8.2022.02.014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, R., Lin, M., Song, H., Pang, J., Ma, D., Zhang, L., et al. (2019). Effects of movement skill development on behavioral self-regulation ability of preschool children. Chin. Sport Sci. 39, 40–47. doi: 10.16469/j.css.201911004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mao, H.-F., Li, W., and Lo, J.-L. (1999). Construct validity of Beery’s developmental test of visual-motor integration for Taiwanese children. Occup. Ther. J. Res. 19, 241–257. doi: 10.1177/153944929901900402

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marr, D., and Cermak, S. (2002). Predicting handwriting performance of early elementary students with the developmental test of visual-motor integration. Percept. Mot. Skills 95, 661–669. doi: 10.2466/pms.2002.95.2.661

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marr, D., Cermak, S., Cohn, E. S., and Henderson, A. (2003). Fine motor activities in head start and kindergarten classrooms. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 57, 550–557. doi: 10.5014/ajot.57.5.550

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martzog, P., Stoeger, H., and Suggate, S. (2019). Relations between preschool children’s fine motor skills and general cognitive abilities. J. Cogn. Dev. 20, 443–465. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2019.1607862

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matheis, M., and Estabillo, J. A. (2018). “Assessment of fine and gross motor skills in children” in Handbook of childhood psychopathology and developmental disabilities assessment. ed. J. L. Matson (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 467–484.

Google Scholar

Maurer, M. N., and Roebers, C. M. (2020). Is the fine motor–executive functions link stronger for new compared to repeated fine motor tasks? PLoS One 15, e0241308–e0241309. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0241308

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, M., Chui, M., Lin, L., Fong, A., and Chan, D. (2015). Performance of the visual-motor integration of preschool children in Hong Kong. Hong Kong J. Occup. Ther. 25, 7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.hkjot.2015.06.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oberklaid, F., Baird, G., Blair, M., Melhuish, E., and Hall, D. (2013). Children’s health and development: approaches to early identification and intervention. Arch. Dis. Child. 98, 1008–1011. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2013-304091

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Olhaberry, M., León, M. J., Sieverson, C., Escobar, M., Iribarren, D., Morales-Reyes, I., et al. (2019). Is it possible to improve early childhood development with a video-feedback intervention directed at the mother-father-child triad? Res. Psychother. 22, 244–255. doi: 10.4081/ripppo.2019.324

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, V. G., and Isaacs, L. D. (2024). Human motor development: A lifespan approach. 11th Edn. New York: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Pont, K., Wallen, M., and Bundy, A. (2009). Conceptualising a modified system for classification of in-hand manipulation. Aust. Occup. Ther. J. 56, 2–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1630.2008.00774.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pont, K., Wallen, M., Bundy, A., and Case-Smith, J. (2008). Reliability and validity of the test of in-hand manipulation in children ages 5 to 6 years. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 62, 384–392. doi: 10.5014/ajot.62.4.384

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramos, M. M. A., and Barba, P. C. S. D. (2021). Ages and stages questionnaires Brazil in monitoring development in early childhood education. An. Acad. Bras. Ciênc. 93, 1–13. doi: 10.1590/0001-3765202120201838

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rebelo, M., Serrano, J., Duarte-Mendes, P., Monteiro, D., Paulo, R., and Marinho, D. A. (2021). Evaluation of the psychometric properties of the Portuguese Peabody developmental motor Scales-2 edition: a study with children aged 12 to 48 months. Child.-Basel 8, 1–12. doi: 10.3390/children8111049

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubio-Codina, M., Araujo, M. C., Attanasio, O., Muñoz, P., and Grantham-McGregor, S. (2016). Concurrent validity and feasibility of short tests currently used to measure early childhood development in large scale studies. PLoS One 11, 1–17. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160962

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, X., Li, G., and Yao, K. (1995). The revising of the developmental test of visual–motor integration. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 3, 79–82.

Google Scholar

Smith, Y. A., Hong, E., and Presson, C. (2000). Normative and validation studies of the nine-hole peg test with children. Percept. Mot. Skills 90, 823–843. doi: 10.2466/pms.2000.90.3.823

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M., Niemeijer, A. S., and van Galen, G. P. (2001). Fine motor deficiencies in children diagnosed as DCD based on poor grapho-motor ability. Hum. Mov. Sci. 20, 161–182. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(01)00033-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sommerfeld, A., McFarland, A., Waliczek, T. M., and Zajicek, J. (2021). Use of gardening programs as an intervention to increase children’s visual-motor integration. HortTechnology 31, 589–594. doi: 10.21273/horttech04887-21

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoodley, C. J. (2012). The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional imaging studies. Cerebellum 11, 352–365. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0260-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Strooband, K. F., De Rosnay, M., Okely, A. D., and Veldman, S. L. (2020). Systematic review and meta-analyses: motor skill interventions to improve fine motor development in children aged birth to 6 years. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 41, 319–331. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000779

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Strooband, K. F. B., Howard, S. J., Okely, A. D., Neilsen-Hewett, C., and De Rosnay, M. (2023). Validity and reliability of a fine motor assessment for preschool children. Early Child. Educ. J. 51, 801–810. doi: 10.1007/s10643-022-01336-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suggate, S., Pufke, E., and Stoeger, H. (2018). Do fine motor skills contribute to early reading development? J. Res. Read. 41, 1–19. doi: 10.1111/1467-9817.12081

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suggate, S., Pufke, E., and Stoeger, H. (2019). Children’s fine motor skills in kindergarten predict reading in grade 1. Early Child Res. Q. 47, 248–258. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.12.015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tissue, C. M., Velleman, P. F., Stegink-Jansen, C. W., Aaron, D. H., Winthrop, B. G., and Gogola, G. R. (2017). Validity and reliability of the functional dexterity test in children. J. Hand Ther. 30, 500–506. doi: 10.1016/j.jht.2016.08.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, J., Curatolo, S., Leblanc, M., Patulli, C., Tang, T., Darsaklis, V., et al. (2019). Establishing normative data for the functional dexterity test in typically developing children aged 3-5 years. J. Hand Ther. 32, 93–102.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jht.2017.09.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Der Veer, G., Kamphorst, E., Minnaert, A., Cantell, M., Kakebeeke, T. H., and Houwen, S. (2021). Assessing motor performance in preschool children: the Zurich neuromotor assessment-2 and the movement assessment battery for children-2. Percept. Mot. Skills 128, 2014–2032. doi: 10.1177/00315125211025246

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Heerden, A., Hsiao, C., Matafwali, B., Louw, J., and Richter, L. (2017). Support for the feasibility of the ages and stages questionnaire as a developmental screening tool: a cross-sectional study of south african and zambian children aged 2-60 months. BMC Pediatr. 17:55. doi: 10.1186/s12887-017-0802-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Venetsanou, F., Kambas, A., Aggeloussis, N., Fatouros, I., and Taxildaris, K. (2009). Motor assessment of preschool aged children: a preliminary investigation of the validity of the bruininks–oseretsky test of motor proficiency – short form. Hum. Mov. Sci. 28, 543–550. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2009.03.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, M., Bian, X., Jane, S., Yao, G., Wang, X., Xie, H., et al. (2015). Studies of the norm and psychometrical properties of the ages and stages questionnaires, third edition, with a Chinese national sample. Chin. J. Pediatr. 53, 913–918. doi: 10.3760/cma.j.issn.0578-1310.2015.12.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, M., Jane, S., Song, W., Yu, Q., Yao, G., Bian, X., et al. (2011). Study on the neuropsychological development differences in the ages and stages questionnaires (Chinese version) of Shanghai children aged 3 to 66 months in seasons of spring, summer and autumn. Chin. J. Spec. Educ. 6, 425–430.

Google Scholar

Williams, A. A., and Johnson, E. E. (2022). A programmatic evaluation of school preparedness for latinx children and families: a partnership program. Hisp. Health Care Int. 20, 231–237. doi: 10.1177/15404153211063639

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Q., Qu, J., Zhang, Q., Jiang, B., Wang, J., Yang, L., et al. (2019). Evaluation and influencing factors analysis of the visual–motor integration of preschool children in Nanjing. Chin. J. Child Health Care 27, 355–357. doi: 10.11852/zgetbjzz2018-0673

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, C., Zhou, H., Liu, X., and Wang, Y. (2018). Prevalence of suspected developmental delay in fine motor and its correlation with preterm among children aged 1~59 months in 8 counties of central and western China. Chin. J. Child Health Care 26, 127–129. doi: 10.11852/zgetbjzz2018-26-02-04

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Li, H., Wang, H., Hu, S., and Wang, Z. (2020). Relationship between fundamental movement skills and physical fitness in children. Chin. J. Sch. Health. 41, 554–557. doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2020.04.020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, H., and Lin, L. (2001). Development of visual-motor integration and its relation to academic achievement in 4-8-year-old children. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 17, 5–8.

Google Scholar

Zheng, P., and Sun, J. (2022). Preschool children’s use of digital devices and early development in Hong Kong: the role of family socioeconomic status. Early Educ. Dev. 33, 893–911. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2021.1920767

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: preschooler, children, fine motor skill, instruments, scoping review

Citation: Zhang B-F, Lin Z-C and Li C (2025) Fine motor skills assessment instruments for preschool children with typical development: a scoping review. Front. Psychol. 16:1620235. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1620235

Received: 29 April 2025; Accepted: 25 August 2025;
Published: 10 September 2025.

Edited by:

Jordan Ashton Booker, University of Missouri, United States

Reviewed by:

Pedro Flores, Higher Institute of Educational Sciences of the Douro, Portugal
Hellen Vretudaki, University of Crete, Rethymno, Crete, Greece

Copyright © 2025 Zhang, Lin and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Bai-Fa Zhang, emhhbmdiYWlmYUBzd3UuZWR1LmNu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.