EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol.

Sec. Psychology of Language

Volume 16 - 2025 | doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1636418

This article is part of the Research TopicStance-Taking in Embodied and Virtual InteractionView all 16 articles

Editorial: Multimodal Stance-Taking in Embodied and Virtual Interaction

Provisionally accepted
  • 1University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
  • 2Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 3Georg-August-Universitat Gottingen, Gttingen, Germany

The final, formatted version of the article will be published soon.

A plethora of conversation-analytic, discourse-functional, cognitive and psycholinguistic studies from recent years emphasize the central role of stance-taking in social interaction: "Whenever we engage in interaction, we are taking stances: there is never a time out from the social action of taking stances and adopting positions" (Du Bois & Kärkkäinen 2012: 438). This act of positioning can involve three different axes, which are often considered to represent different types of stance-taking: epistemic, affective, and deontic. While epistemic stance-taking is understood as "the stance that a participant in an interaction takes with their turn in relation to a certain object of knowledge" (Deppermann 2018: 121, our translation), e.g. by expressing (un)certainty, deontic stances refer to judgements about the necessity or desirability of actions, events, suggestions or similar stance objects (Du Bois 2007), e.g. in cases of invitations or orders. Finally, the category of affective stances includes attitudinal and evaluative expressions which, according to Ochs (1996: 104), express "mood, attitude, feeling, and disposition, as well as degrees of emotional intensity vis-à-vis some focus of concern".Following Du Bois (2007) and Jaffe (2009), stance-taking does not merely reside in the expression of one's own feelings and opinions but crucially serves to position oneself and others and thus to negotiate the interpersonal relationship by either expressing (dis-)alignment with other participants and their stances. While aligning stances serve the purpose of creating or emphasising commonalities and thus enforcing and displaying togetherness (interpersonal alignment, Du Bois 2007), disalignment may serve the purpose of setting oneself apart from others. This is reminiscent of Stivers' concept of affiliation, which she defines as the actions by which "the hearer displays support of and endorses the teller's [or interactant's, our addition] conveyed stance" (Stivers 2008: 35). In contrast, disaffiliative stances express an attitude that diverges from the stance expressed by interaction partners (also Steensig & Drew 2008, Drew & Walker 2009and especially Stivers 2010, 2020). As social actions, however, affiliation and disaffiliation are not to be understood as dichotomous categories, but as two end points of a continuum (cf. Thompson et al. 2015: 176-178 and their analyses of stronger and weaker affiliative and disaffiliative evaluations). Similarly, the triadic relationship between stance(taking) subjects and objects (cf. "the stance triangle", Du Bois 2007) is not a static construct, but fundamentally dynamic in nature and subject to interactional negotiation.Research paradigms such as Conversation Analysis, Interactional Linguistics and psycholinguistics have a long track record in the study of stance-taking as a socially contextualized and recognized interpersonal phenomenon, focusing on the lexical and grammatical resources that language users have at their disposal to communicate stance. Among these linguistic resources, special attention has gone to pre-positioned elements (Auer & Lindström 2015) such as well, okay, I don't know etc. (Auer & Uhmann 1982, Pomerantz 1984, Szczepek Reed 2015, Pekarek Doehler et al. 2021), but also to concessive constructions, modal verb constructions, and hedges. These verbal means to express stance have been studied extensively in different communicative settings (from spontaneous face-to-face communication to institutional and mediated forms of interaction), from different disciplinary angles (Interactional Linguistics, Ethnomethodology, Cognitive Psychology, HCI Research, etc.) and using different empirical methods (from controlled experiments to qualitative and quantitative corpus analysis).However, the expression of stance -both in face-to-face and in virtual communication -is by no means limited to verbal means. Recent studies have explored the ways in which embodied resources such as hand gestures (see also the contributions to this Topic by Cienki, Inbar, Ladewig), body movements (Betz & Gubina), body posture, facial expressions and eye gaze (de Vries, Laner, and Clift in this Topic) play a role in the expression of stance (see Andries et al. 2023 for a systematic literature review). The goal of this Research Topic is to highlight this recent trend in the study of stance-taking and to present a state-of-the-art collection of original research that zooms in on either the co-occurrence of (i.e. 'multimodal packages', Mondada 2014) or interdependence between different semiotic resources (i.e. the sequential relationship within or across speakers).The contributions to this Topic take a broad perspective on multimodal interaction. They unite a variety of methodological approaches, ranging from the study of naturally occurring interactions to carefully controlled experiments that tap into the cognitive underpinnings of multimodal stance-taking. Apart from the basic question on how stance is multimodally construed and negotiated in spoken and signed face-to-face interaction, the Research Topic also explores the strategies that interlocutors employ to express stance in mediated forms of interaction. It presents original research, with a particular focus on empirical studies that adopt a decidedly multimodal approach to this phenomenon. Ladewig and Li are furthermore concerned with the impact of medium -or in Laner's case the spatial formation and the interaction with the surrounding nature -on the strategies that interlocutors employ to express stance. Finally, the studies by Cienki, Rühlemann & Trujillo as well as Zheng & Sun broaden our perspective on the interactional phenomenon of stance-taking by inquiring into the cognitive processes underpinning stance-taking in interaction with Cienki also focusing on cross-linguistic differences.In sum, this Research Topic is the first collection of original work that is specifically devoted to the study of how different semiotic resources contribute to stance-taking activities in a wide array of (interactional) contexts. What the fifteen contributions to this collection show, is that the topic of multimodal stance-taking can and must be approached from various angles, each of which adds to a better understanding of what is one of the most fundamental social activities realized in/through language and embodied behavior. While qualitative studies in the tradition of Conversation Analysis and Interactional Linguistics provide us with relevant insights into the sequential organization and social dynamics of multimodal stance-taking, the more quantitatively oriented experimental and corpus-based studies help us in identifying patterns and relevant factors that drive the expression and negotiation of stance. It was the explicit intention of this Research Topic to unite these different perspectives and thus to set the agenda for future research on the multifaceted nature of stance-taking in embodied and virtual interaction.

Keywords: Stance-taking, multimodality, embodied interaction, Virtual interaction, Gesture, multimodal packages, cognitive underpinnings

Received: 27 May 2025; Accepted: 03 Jun 2025.

Copyright: © 2025 Zima, Brône, Feyaerts and Ladewig. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

* Correspondence: Elisabeth Zima, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.