Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 26 September 2025

Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1641481

Influence of attributional style, anger, and self-control on college students’ impulsive consumption

Guohua Zhou,&#x;Guohua Zhou1,2Wentao Sun,&#x;Wentao Sun1,2Weiguo Qu,
Weiguo Qu1,2*
  • 1School of Education Science, Hunan Normal University, Changsha, China
  • 2Cognition and Human Behavior Key Laboratory of Hunan Province, Changsha, China

Introduction: With the rapid development of the digital economy and the prevalence of consumerism in recent years, impulsive consumption among college students has become increasingly prominent, a phenomenon closely associated with their psychological traits and emotional states. Even though research has examined self-control and consumption, few studies have integrated attributional style. Therefore, this study systematically examines the effects of attributional style, anger, and self-control on impulsive consumption behaviors among college students through three experiments.

Methods: Undergraduate participants were recruited from a university in Changsha, China. Experiment 1 investigated the impact of attributional style on impulsive consumption. Experiment 2 explored the interactive effects of anger and attributional style on impulsive consumption. Finally, Experiment 3 further examined the interaction between self-control and attributional style.

Results: Experiment 1 found that participants with optimistic attributional styles exhibited significantly lower levels of impulsive consumption than those with pessimistic attributional styles. Experiment 2 revealed that anger significantly suppressed impulsive consumption behaviors and interacted with attributional style. Experiment 3 demonstrated that higher levels of self-control effectively reduced impulsive consumption and produced significant interactive effects with attributional style.

Discussion: These findings indicate that optimistic attributional styles, anger induction, and high self-control significantly inhibit impulsive consumption among college students. These results provide novel theoretical insights into the psychological mechanisms underlying impulsive consumption in this population and offer empirical support for targeted psychological interventions in consumer behavior.

1 Introduction

Impulsive consumption resembles a sudden gust of wind—overpowering in the moment, but is often followed by regret. It refers to irrational purchasing decisions made under weakened self-control, owing to strong emotional responses triggered by external or internal stimuli. Among college students, impulsive consumption manifests distinctively through the excessive pursuit of branded products and heightened sensitivity to promotional campaigns (Xie et al., 2023). During purchase decisions, students frequently prioritize rapid emotional reactions over rational deliberation, reflecting both their desire for immediate gratification and their vulnerability to external temptations. Frequent impulsive consumption exacerbates financial strain and adversely affects academic performance and quality of life (Guo et al., 2024). Studies indicate a strong correlation between excessive spending and compromised mental health in this demographic, often leading to anxiety and self-reproach (Rook and Fisher, 1995; Yi and Baumgartner, 2011). Moreover, impulsive consumption perpetuates a vicious cycle by undermining financial management capabilities as individuals neglect self-regulatory strategies (Guo et al., 2024). These findings underscore the need to investigate impulsive consumption among college students.

Attributional style plays a pivotal role in emotional regulation and decision-making (Chen et al., 2025). Defined as an individual’s habitual explanation for events, attributional style is broadly categorized into optimistic and pessimistic types (Weiner, 1985; Sanders, 2024). Optimistic individuals attribute success to internal efforts and failures to external factors, fostering resilience and emotional stability that mitigate impulsive tendencies (Wang et al., 2020). Conversely, those with pessimistic styles tend to internalize failures as personal inadequacies, heightening emotional distress and impulsive behaviors (Antosz-Rekucka and Prochwicz, 2024). College students, often navigating academic pressures and financial constraints, exhibit divergent consumption patterns influenced by attributional styles. Research suggests that optimistic attributional style correlates with stronger self-control, thus enabling rational resistance to consumption temptations (Hofmann et al., 2012a). Understanding these dynamics offers critical insights into the psychological mechanisms of impulsive consumption and can inform targeted interventions. Thus, we hypothesized the following:

Hypothesis 1: As attributional style influences impulsive consumption, college students with optimistic attributional style exhibit lower levels of impulsive consumption than those with pessimistic style.

Anger, a fundamental negative emotion, is defined as the intense discomfort arising from conflicts in personal values or perceived rights (Lazarus, 1991; Renström et al., 2023). Characterized by low valence and high arousal (Scherer, 2005), anger is a discrete emotion with distinct cognitive and behavioral signatures. Unlike other negative emotions, anger is transient yet highly activating. Its impact on consumption decisions is complex. While positive emotions generally enhance purchasing behaviors (Herabadi et al., 2009), findings on negative emotions remain inconsistent. Some studies suggest that anger amplifies impulsive consumption by driving individuals toward immediate gratification (Rook, 1987; Verplanken and Herabadi, 2001; Redine et al., 2023), whereas others propose that anger has inhibitory effects (Herabadi et al., 2009). Notably, anger may uniquely suppress impulsive consumption compared with other negative emotions (Lerner and Keltner, 2000). College students with underdeveloped emotional regulation skills (Gross, 2015) and frequent exposure to anger-inducing stressors (e.g., academic competition and financial strain) are particularly vulnerable. Optimistic individuals may externalize anger (e.g., attributing frustration to unethical marketing) and employ rational coping strategies (e.g., resisting purchases), whereas pessimistic individuals exhibit heightened dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation during anger, reflecting a greater cognitive effort to regulate emotions and suppress impulses (Disner et al., 2011). This interplay between attributional style and anger highlights the need to explore their combined effects on impulsive consumption. Thus, we hypothesized the following:

Hypothesis 2: Anger influences impulsive consumption; thus, following anger induction, college students exhibit lower impulsive consumption than those under neutral conditions.

Hypothesis 3: Attributional style and anger interact to influence impulsive consumption. Thus, following anger induction, optimistic individuals show the lowest impulsive consumption, whereas pessimistic individuals under neutral conditions exhibit the highest impulsive consumption.

Self-control—a core metacognitive ability—serves as a critical “braking system” in impulsive decision-making (Baumeister et al., 2007). In college students, who face relentless consumption stimuli (e.g., social media marketing and time-limited discounts) but lack financial literacy, self-control directly determines resistance to immediate temptations. It also interacts with emotional regulation; optimistic individuals consider temptations as external and transient (e.g., “marketing traps”), conserving cognitive resources for self-control, whereas pessimistic individuals internalize failures (e.g., “I lack willpower”), depleting self-regulatory capacity (Hofmann et al., 2012b; Wang et al., 2020). Even though research has examined self-control and consumption, few studies have integrated attributional style, a gap that this study addresses to comprehensively unravel the psychological mechanisms of impulsive behavior. Thus, we hypothesized the following:

Hypothesis 4: Self-control influences impulsive consumption; thus, students with high self-control exhibit lower impulsive consumption than those with low self-control.

Hypothesis 5: Attributional style and self-control interact to influence impulsive consumption; thus, optimistic individuals with high self-control show the lowest impulsive consumption, whereas pessimistic individuals with low self-control exhibit the highest impulsive consumption.

In summary, attributional style, anger, and self-control collectively shape impulsive consumption among college students. In today’s consumerist environment, where marketing tactics relentlessly target this demographic, understanding these interactions is critical for fostering rational consumption habits and enhancing self-regulatory capacities (Xie et al., 2023). This study employs simulated scenarios to systematically investigate these dynamics, offering theoretical and practical implications for mitigating impulsive consumption.

2 Experiment 1: Impact of attributional style on college students’ impulsive consumption

2.1 Research purpose and hypotheses

This experiment examines the influence of attributional style (optimistic vs. pessimistic) on impulsive consumption among college students by simulating impulsive purchase scenarios. The hypothesis tested by Experiment 1 was as follows:

Hypothesis 1: Attributional style significantly affects impulsive consumption; thus, students with pessimistic attributional styles exhibit higher levels of impulsive consumption than those with optimistic styles.

2.2 Methodology

2.2.1 Participants

A power analysis conducted via G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007) indicated that a sample size of 128 participants was required to achieve 80% power (1–β) with a medium effect size (f = 0.25) at α = 0.05. Using convenience sampling, we recruited 138 undergraduate students from a university in Changsha, China. After excluding 8 invalid responses (incomplete, duplicate, or inconsistent responses), 130 valid datasets were retained. Participants were classified into optimistic (n = 36) and pessimistic (n = 36) attributional style groups based on the top and bottom 27% scores from the Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ). The final sample comprised 72 participants (51 female; mean age: Mage = 20.11 years, standard deviation: SD = 1.181). All participants were physically and mentally healthy with normal or corrected vision, had native Chinese proficiency, were right-handed, had basic computer skills, and had no prior exposure to similar experiments. All signed informed consent forms.

2.2.2 Experimental design

A single-factor, between-subjects design was employed with attributional style (optimistic vs. pessimistic) as the independent variable (categorized via ASQ total scores) and impulsive consumption levels as the dependent variable.

2.2.3 Experimental materials

2.2.3.1 ASQ

This study employed the ASQ developed by Peterson and Seligman (1984) and revised by Wang and Zhang (2006). The revised version was adapted to better align with the Chinese cultural context and national conditions, thereby ensuring its applicability to the Chinese student population. Based on the analytical results of this study, the questionnaire demonstrated good internal consistency with a Cronbach’s α coefficient of 0.85. The ASQ comprises 12 hypothetical situations: 6 positive and 6 negative, with 3 questions per situation covering diverse aspects of daily life and work. Responses are recorded on a 7-point Likert scale. For positive situations, 1 represents the least optimistic attribution and 7 the most optimistic, whereas the scoring is reversed for negative situations; scores for positive attribution style and negative attribution style are calculated by dividing the total scores of positive and negative situations by 6, respectively. Higher scores indicate stronger attributional tendencies in the corresponding domains. The overall attribution style is derived by subtracting the negative attribution style score from the positive one. Specifically, an overall attribution style score above 0 signifies an optimistic attribution style, below 0 signifies a pessimistic style, and 0 indicates a neutral attribution style (Yang et al., 2021). In the subsequent experiments, participants from the top and bottom 27% of the ASQ score distribution were selected to form distinct optimistic and pessimistic groups. This extreme-groups approach is a well-established methodological practice in individual differences research (Preacher et al., 2005) that is employed to enhance statistical power for detecting hypothesized effects between theoretically distinct groups by maximizing between-group variance. Notably, this approach excludes mid-scoring participants and may consequently inflate effect size estimates compared to a full-spectrum analysis; therefore, the findings should be interpreted as reflecting differences between relatively pure, extreme groups.

2.2.3.2 Impulsive consumption scenario simulation

This experiment adopted the “simulated scenario” method (Rook and Fisher, 1995) to investigate impulsive consumption (for similar approaches, see Zhou et al., 2017; Song et al., 2021; Xue et al., 2023). Participants were instructed to imagine their shopping decisions and consumption behaviors in hypothetical third-person virtual scenarios. This method was selected not only for its strong experimental control, but also for its ability to standardize stimuli across participants and prevent ethical and practical concerns (e.g., real financial incentives or deception). While acknowledging that hypothetical scenarios may not fully capture the emotional arousal associated with real-world spending, this approach has been widely validated in impulse buying literature and has demonstrated good predictive validity for assessing behavioral tendencies. Specifically, the experimental design included three distinct shopping scenarios, each offering four response options, to measure participants’ impulsive consumption levels. In each scenario, options were structured to reflect varying degrees of impulsive consumption characteristics, with higher scores indicating stronger impulsive tendencies. This method is widely utilized in studies of individual impulsive consumption behavior owing to its efficacy in capturing decision-making processes within controlled contexts. The simulation of impulsive consumption scenarios was conducted online using E-Prime 2.0 software to ensure procedural standardization and accurate data collection.

2.2.4 Procedure

Prior to the formal experiment, participants were required to complete the ASQ. Based on their total ASQ scores, they were divided into an optimistic attribution style group (top 27%, n = 36) and a pessimistic attribution style group (bottom 27%, n = 36). Participants in both groups were then assigned to separate computer laboratories to ensure environmental independence and procedural control.

During the formal experiment, participants in the optimistic and pessimistic attribution style groups were individually guided to complete the impulsive consumption scenario simulation on computers. Each participant responded to three distinct types of shopping decision-making tasks, with their scores directly reflecting their level of impulsive consumption. This was approved by the ethics committee (Figure 1).

Figure 1
Flowchart illustrating a process starting with completing an attribution style questionnaire, leading to two groups: optimistic attribution style (top 27 percent) and pessimistic attribution style (bottom 27 percent). Both groups are linked to an impulsive consumption scenario simulation.

Figure 1. Experimental procedure of Experiment 1.

2.2.5 Data analysis

All the statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 27.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Independent samples t-tests were used to compare the levels of impulsive consumption between participants with different attributional styles (optimistic vs. pessimistic).

2.3 Results

The independent-samples t-test revealed significant differences in impulsive consumption between the attributional style groups (t(1,70) = 2.25, p = 0.027, 95% [CI] (0.13, 2.15)). Participants with pessimistic styles (M = 7.58, SD = 2.34) exhibited higher impulsive consumption than those with optimistic styles (M = 6.44, SD = 1.93; Figure 2).

Figure 2
Bar chart comparing impulsive consumption levels between pessimistic and optimistic attribution styles. The blue bar for pessimistic attribution style shows a higher average, just above 7, compared to the green bar for optimistic attribution style, slightly below 7. A significance marker indicates a notable difference between the styles.

Figure 2. Differences in impulse consumption levels among different attribution styles.

2.4 Discussion

The results of Experiment 1 demonstrated a significant influence of attributional style on impulsive consumption levels among college students. Analysis indicated marked differences in impulsive consumption, with the optimistic group exhibiting significantly lower levels than the pessimistic group, consistent with Hypothesis 1. This disparity may stem from the optimistic group’s broader information-gathering scope and higher-quality decision-making processes, which likely predisposed them to more rational consumption behaviors (Wang and Liu, 2008).

Research has identified emotional factors alongside attributional style as critical determinants of impulsive consumption (Loewenstein, 2000; Verhagen and van Dolen, 2011). However, the role of anger, a salient emotional factor that shapes impulsive consumption among university students, remains underexplored. To address this gap, Experiment 2 introduced anger induction to investigate the interplay between attributional style, anger, and impulsive consumption. Participants with differing attributional styles engaged in simulated impulsive consumption scenarios under controlled anger conditions, enabling an examination of how these variables jointly influence decision-making.

3 Experiment 2: Effects of attributional style and anger on college students’ impulsive consumption

3.1 Research purpose and hypotheses

This experiment explored the influence of attribution style and anger on college students’ impulsive consumption by inducing anger and simulating impulsive consumption scenarios in participants with different attribution styles. Accordingly, the following hypotheses were proposed:

Hypothesis 2: Anger significantly influences impulsive consumption; thus, students under neutral conditions exhibit higher impulsive consumption than those following anger induction.

Hypothesis 3: Attributional style and anger interact to influence impulsive consumption. Thus, under neutral conditions, pessimistic individuals show the highest impulsive consumption, whereas following anger induction, optimistic individuals exhibit the lowest impulsive consumption.

3.2 Methodology

3.2.1 Participants

A power analysis (G*Power 3.1; Faul et al., 2007) determined a required sample size of 201 participants for 80% power (1–β) with α = 0.05 and a medium effect size (f = 0.25). Using convenience sampling, 234 undergraduate students were recruited from a university in Changsha, China. After excluding 11 invalid responses (incomplete or inconsistent answers), 223 valid datasets were retained. Participants who scored in the top and bottom 27% of the ASQ score were classified as optimistic (n = 60) or pessimistic (n = 60), respectively. Each group was further divided into anger induction (n = 30) and neutral (n = 30) conditions, yielding 120 participants (59 female; Mage = 17.9 years, SD = 0.53). All participants met the same health, linguistic, and procedural criteria as in Experiment 1. All signed informed consent forms.

3.2.2 Experimental design

A 2 (attributional style: optimistic versus pessimistic) × 2 (anger: induced versus neutral) between-subjects design was employed. The independent variables included attributional style (categorized by ASQ scores) and anger (induced by validated video stimuli). The dependent variable was impulsive consumption level.

3.2.3 Experimental materials

3.2.3.1 ASQ

These were the same as those employed in Experiment 1.

3.2.3.2 Impulsive consumption scenario simulation

These were the same as those employed in Experiment 1.

3.2.3.3 Emotional induction materials

The anger-inducing video clip was obtained from Nanjing! Nanjing!, part of the Chinese Emotional Video System (Xu et al., 2010). The 4-min-and-49-s segment depicts the atrocities committed by Japanese soldiers against Chinese civilians. Following prior methodologies (Li et al., 2025), anger was targeted as the focal emotion in subsequent analyses. The control group viewed a neutral 5-min segment from Weather Forecast that aired on September 1, 2023.

3.2.3.4 Positive and negative affect schedule

The participants’ emotional valence was assessed using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson et al., 1988; see Appendix 3). This 5-point Likert scale ranges from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely intense), measuring 7 basic emotional dimensions: pleasure, surprise, fear, disgust, sadness, anger, and calmness. Similar protocols were adopted by Liang et al. (2017) and Wong et al. (2021). Baseline emotional states were recorded before video exposure, followed by post-induction measurements. The questionnaire demonstrated high internal consistency in this study (Cronbach’s α = 0.87).

3.2.4 Procedure

Prior to the formal experiment, all participants completed the ASQ. Based on their total ASQ scores, participants were stratified into two groups: those with optimistic attributional styles (top 27%, n = 60) and those with pessimistic attributional styles (bottom 27%, n = 60). Each group was then randomly subdivided into 2 subgroups of 30 participants each: 1 subgroup received anger induction and the other served as a control. Four experimental conditions were assigned to separate computer laboratories to prevent cross-conditioning interference. The experimenter thoroughly explained the experimental protocol and operational guidelines, emphasizing strict compliance with instructional prompts throughout the procedure.

At the start of the experiment, participants completed the Emotional Self-Assessment Scale as a pretest measure of baseline affective states. For the emotion induction phase, 30 optimistic and 30 pessimistic participants viewed a neutral documentary (“Weather Forecast”), whereas 30 optimistic and 30 pessimistic participants viewed an anger-inducing film clip (“Nanjing! Nanjing!”).

Immediately after viewing, participants completed the Emotional Self-Assessment Scale again as a post-test assessment. Subsequently, all participants engaged in three standardized impulse consumption scenarios, with their impulsive consumption levels quantified using validated scoring metrics (Figure 3).

Figure 3
Flowchart illustrating a study design. Participants complete an attribution style questionnaire, divided into optimistic (top twenty-seven percent) and pessimistic (bottom twenty-seven percent) groups. Each group further splits into anger induction and no anger induction groups. All groups undergo a Positive and Negative Affect Schedule pre-test, video emotion induction, and post-test, culminating in an impulsive consumption scenario simulation.

Figure 3. Experimental procedure of Experiment 2.

After completing the experiment, participants underwent a structured emotional recovery protocol, including a standardized debriefing session that explained the purpose of the emotion induction and its temporary nature. Participants were then exposed to several minutes of upbeat, relaxing music. Thereafter, they were monitored for signs of residual distress and received a small gift as a token of appreciation. The participants were explicitly informed that they would be provided immediate, on-site psychological support services available at no cost, should they experience residual discomfort. The experimental procedure, including the specific use of the anger-inducing stimulus, was reviewed and approved by the ethics committee of Hunan Normal University, which confirmed that all measures were in strict compliance with ethical guidelines for the protection of human participants.

3.2.5 Data analysis

Two-way ANOVA was used to analyze the effects of attributional style and anger on impulsive consumption. Simple effects tests were used to examine interaction patterns.

3.3 Results

Statistical analyses of the participants’ emotional valence scores before and after video exposure were conducted to validate the efficacy of emotion induction (Tables 1, 2).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participants’ emotional changes in the no anger induced group before and after watching the video.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Participants’ emotional changes in the anger-induced group before and after watching the video.

Table 1 demonstrates that the participants in the neutral (no anger induction) condition exhibited no significant changes in emotional scores across all dimensions after viewing the video (p > 0.05).

Table 2 reveals significant post-induction changes in emotional scores for the anger group (p < 0.01) across all dimensions except surprise (p = 0.831). Specifically, participants in the anger-induction condition showed marked reductions in joy and calmness and significant increases in anger, disgust, sadness, and fear.

A two-way ANOVA with impulsive consumption as the dependent variable (2 attributional styles × 2 anger conditions) yielded the following results (Figure 4).

Figure 4
Bar chart showing impulsive consumption levels under anger and no-anger conditions. Blue bars represent pessimistic attribution style, and green represent optimistic. No-anger shows higher impulsive consumption, especially for pessimistic style, with significant differences indicated.

Figure 4. Mean impulsive consumption scores across attributional styles and anger conditions.

Main effect of attributional style: F(1, 116) = 11.977, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.094, indicating significant differences in impulsive consumption between optimistic and pessimistic groups;

Main effect of anger: F(1, 116) = 59.456, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.339, reflecting significantly higher impulsive consumption in the neutral condition than under anger induction;

Interaction effect: F(1, 116) = 4.516, p = 0.036, η2 = 0.037. This suggests that while anger induction moderated the relationship between attributional style and impulsive consumption, the effect was modest in magnitude.

Simple effects analysis (Figure 4) was used to further clarify these patterns. Pessimistic group: participants in the neutral condition scored significantly higher on impulsive consumption than those under anger induction (p < 0.001). Optimistic group: Similarly, the neutral condition participants exhibited higher impulsivity than the anger induction participants (p < 0.001). Neutral condition: Pessimistic individuals demonstrated significantly higher impulsive consumption than optimistic individuals (p < 0.001);

Anger induction: No significant differences emerged between the attributional styles (p = 0.347).

3.4 Discussion

These findings confirm the effectiveness of the emotional manipulation, with participants in the anger-induction group displaying significantly heightened anger levels following stimulus exposure. Notably, the induction also led to increases in other negative emotions such as fear, disgust, and sadness—consistent with the complex and multifaceted nature of responses to traumatic historical footage. Therefore, the observed inhibitory effects on impulsive consumption might be attributed to the general activation of high-arousal negative affect, rather than to the unique effect of anger, which is a limitation of the current study. This also warrants that future investigations use more specific induction paradigms (e.g., recalling personal angering events) or physiological measures to better understand the unique influences of discrete negative emotions on consumer decision-making.

These results support Hypothesis 2, indicating that anger induction exerts inhibitory effects on impulsive consumption—which contradicts some prior work linking anger to increased impulsivity (e.g., Rook, 1987; Redine et al., 2023). However, this discrepancy may be reconciled by considering the nature of the anger induction and its consequent cognitive appraisals. Our paradigm utilized a film clip depicting historical injustices, which may have induced a more complex, morally laden form of anger. According to cognitive appraisal theories (Lerner and Keltner, 2000), such anger can be characterized by a high sense of certainty and individual control, potentially leading to more deliberative (rather than impulsive) decision-making as a form of regained autonomy. This enhanced deliberative processing likely involves the recruitment of neural circuits associated with cognitive control, such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), which is critical for suppressing dominant impulsive responses (Hare et al., 2009; Disner et al., 2011). The appraisals of certainty and control may thus facilitate top-down regulation from these prefrontal regions, thereby curbing purchase impulses. Therefore, the anger induced in our study may have amplified the participants’ cognitive restraint capacity, thereby curbing purchase impulses. This pattern aligns with emerging theoretical propositions regarding anger’s nuanced regulatory role in self-control processes (Koley and Warren, 2025).

However, it is important to note that the interaction effect between attributional style and anger, while statistically significant, was small in magnitude (partial η2 = 0.037). This indicates that while the pattern of our results is reliable, its practical significance and influence in real-world settings may be limited. The boundaries of this effect—such as under what conditions it is most pronounced—require further clarification. As such, future research with larger samples should investigate potential moderating variables (e.g., gender, socioeconomic status, or monthly consumption level) that might strengthen or weaken this interaction, thereby better defining its applicable scope.

Nevertheless, the current findings should be interpreted within the broader context of metacognitive influences on impulsive consumption, as emphasized by Verhagen and van Dolen (2011). Critical questions remain regarding how self-regulatory mechanisms interact with attributional biases to modulate emotional impacts on purchasing behaviors. Subsequent analyses employing impulse consumption simulations should systematically investigate the tripartite relationships among attributional tendencies, self-control thresholds, and affective states. This approach disentangles the relative contributions of emotional and cognitive components in shaping university students’ consumption patterns.

4 Experiment 3: Interactive effects of attributional style and self-control on college students’ impulsive consumption

4.1 Research purpose and hypotheses

This experiment examined the combined effects of attributional style and self-control on impulsive consumption using simulated purchasing scenarios. The hypotheses were as follows:

Hypothesis 4: Self-control significantly influences impulsive consumption; thus students with low self-control exhibit higher impulsive consumption than those with high self-control.

Hypothesis 5: Attributional style and self-control interact to influence impulsive consumption; thus, pessimistic individuals with low self-control exhibit the highest impulsive consumption, whereas optimistic individuals with high self-control exhibit the lowest impulsive consumption.

4.2 Methodology

4.2.1 Participants

A power analysis (G*Power 3.1; Faul et al., 2007) determined a required sample size of 201 participants for 80% power (1–β) with α = 0.05 and a medium effect size (f = 0.25). Using convenience sampling, 248 undergraduate students were recruited from a university in Changsha, China. After the exclusion of 26 invalid responses (incomplete or inconsistent answers), 222 valid datasets were retained. Participants who scored in the top and bottom 27% of the ASQ were classified as optimistic (n = 60) or pessimistic (n = 60), respectively. Each group was further divided into high (n = 30) and low (n = 30) self-control subgroups based on Self-Control Scale scores, yielding 120 participants (53 female; Mage = 18.4 years, SD = 0.76). All participants met the same health, linguistic, and procedural criteria as in the previous experiments. All signed informed consent forms.

4.2.2 Experimental design

A 2 (attributional style: optimistic versus pessimistic) × 2 (self-control: high versus low) between-subjects design was employed. The independent variables included attributional style (ASQ scores) and self-control (Self-Control Scale scores), with impulsive consumption level as the dependent variable.

4.2.3 Experimental materials

4.2.3.1 ASQ

These were the same as those employed in Experiment 1.

4.2.3.2 Impulsive consumption scenario simulation

These were the same as those employed in Experiment 1.

4.2.3.3 Self-control scale

Adapted from Mamayek et al. (2017) and revised by Tan and Guo (2008), this 19-item scale assesses 5 dimensions of the self-control trait on a 5-point Likert scale. Lower total scores indicate stronger self-control. The Chinese adaptation demonstrated strong reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.86) and cultural validity (Bian et al., 2025). For the purpose of data analysis and to facilitate intuitive interpretation, the raw scores were reverse-scored prior to grouping. Consequently, higher scores on the transformed scale correspond to higher levels of self-control, and the groups were labeled as “high self-control” and “low self-control” based on these reversed scores.

4.2.4 Procedure

Participants first completed the ASQ and were then classified into optimistic (n = 60) or pessimistic (n = 60) groups. Subsequently, based on the Self-Control Scale score, each group was divided into high (n = 30) and low (n = 30) self-control subgroups. Participants completed three impulsive consumption scenarios with scores indexing impulsivity (Figure 5). This was approved by the ethics committee.

Figure 5
Flowchart showing a process beginning with completing an attribution style questionnaire. Participants are divided into optimistic (top 27 percent) and pessimistic (bottom 27 percent) groups. They then fill out the self-control scale, leading to high or low self-control group categorization, followed by an impulsive consumption scenario simulation.

Figure 5. Experimental procedure of Experiment 3.

4.2.5 Data analysis

Two-way ANOVA was used to analyze the effects of attributional style and self-control on impulsive consumption. Simple effects tests were used to examine interaction patterns.

4.3 Results

A two-way ANOVA (two attributional styles × two self-control levels), with impulsive consumption as the dependent variable, yielded the following results: (Figure 6).

Figure 6
Bar chart displaying impulsive consumption levels for high and low self-control groups. Blue bars represent pessimistic attribution style; green bars represent optimistic attribution style. Higher consumption is seen in the low self-control group. Statistical significance is marked with asterisks above the bars.

Figure 6. Mean impulsive consumption scores across attributional styles and self-control levels.

Main effect of attributional style: F(1, 116) = 169.561, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.594, indicating significant differences in impulsive consumption between optimistic and pessimistic attributional styles.

Main effect of self-control: F(1, 116) = 55.429, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.323, reflecting significantly higher impulsive consumption in participants with low self-control compared to those with high self-control.

Interaction effect: F(1, 116) = 19.747, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.145, demonstrating that the influence of attributional style on impulsive consumption varied depending on self-control levels.

Simple effects analysis was used to further clarify these patterns. Pessimistic group: participants with low self-control exhibited significantly higher impulsive consumption than those with high self-control (p < 0.001). Optimistic group: similarly, low self-control participants scored higher in impulsivity than high self-control participants (p < 0.05). Low self-control: pessimistic individuals demonstrated significantly higher impulsive consumption than optimistic individuals (p < 0.001). High self-control: despite higher self-control, pessimistic participants still showed greater impulsivity than optimistic participants (p < 0.001).

4.4 Discussion

Experiment 3 confirmed that both attributional style and self-control significantly influenced impulsive consumption. Optimistic individuals exhibited lower impulsivity than pessimistic individuals, consistent with Experiment 1. Low self-control predicted higher impulsive consumption, supporting Hypothesis 4 and echoing Vohs and Faber’s (2007) findings on self-control resource depletion. The interaction effect revealed that pessimistic individuals with low self-control displayed the highest impulsivity, whereas optimistic individuals with high self-control showed the lowest, validating Hypothesis 5. These results underscore the synergistic roles of cognitive framing (attributional style) and regulatory capacity (self-control) in impulsive decision making.

5 General discussion

5.1 Optimistic attributional style significantly inhibits college students’ impulsive consumption

This study confirms the significant influence of attributional style on impulsive consumption, with pessimistic individuals exhibiting higher impulsivity and optimists demonstrating more rational decision making. These findings align with the attribution theory (Weiner, 1985; Sanders, 2024), which posits that explanatory styles shape subsequent behaviors, and with self-regulation theory (Carver and Scheier, 2001), which suggests that optimistic attributional styles enhance self-control to curb impulsive consumption (Baumeister et al., 2007). Furthermore, the results extend the cognitive-affective personality system theory (Mischel and Shoda, 1995) by revealing how attributional style, as a stable cognitive predisposition, modulates emotional regulation and decision evaluation to influence consumption behaviors.

This study challenges the traditional overemphasis on situational factors in consumer behavior research (Rook, 1987; Redine et al., 2023), highlighting instead the critical role of individual cognitive differences. Pessimistic individuals, who tend to attribute negative events to stable, global causes (Abramson et al., 1978), may adopt a “what-the-hell” mentality (Puri, 1996) when confronted with promotions or temptations, exacerbating impulsive spending. Conversely, optimists employ problem-focused strategies (Folkman and Lazarus, 1988), weighing long-term consequences to reduce irrational expenditures. These insights enrich our understanding of the cognitive mechanisms underlying impulsive consumption and provide a novel theoretical framework for exploring the relationships between attributional style and other consumption biases (e.g., compulsive buying and conspicuous consumption). Practically, the findings on the role of attributional style may inform the development of targeted interventions. Evidence-based approaches such as attributional retraining, which has proven effective in improving academic outcomes and mental health by modifying maladaptive explanatory styles (Kabat-Zinn, 2003; Seligman et al., 1999), could be explored as a potential strategy to foster adaptive cognitive patterns that may reduce impulsive consumption tendencies. Similarly, integrating assessments of attributional style into financial literacy programs (Bajtelsmit, 2024) could allow for more personalized guidance. However, the efficacy of these specific applications for reducing impulsive spending requires direct empirical testing in future intervention studies. A specific ART module might involve: (1) psychoeducation: helping students identify their own pessimistic attributional patterns (e.g., “I always fail to control my spending”) through guided self-assessment; (2) cognitive restructuring: teaching students to challenge these patterns and adopt a more flexible, optimistic style (e.g., “that advertisement was designed to be tempting, but I can make a conscious choice”); and (3) behavioral experimentation: using role-playing or simulated shopping scenarios to practice new attributional styles and strengthen adaptive cognitive habits. This structured approach moves beyond general advice to provide actionable skills for improving self-regulation in consumer contexts.

5.2 Anger induction suppresses college students’ impulsive consumption

This study elucidates the complex interplay between attributional style and anger in shaping impulsive consumption. Under neutral conditions, pessimistic individuals exhibited the highest impulsivity, while optimists showed greater restraint, a pattern consistent with attribution theory (Weiner, 1985; Sanders, 2024) and the role of optimism in promoting self-efficacy and future-oriented thinking (Peterson and Seligman, 1984). However, anger induction eliminated these differences, suggesting that anger temporarily overrides stable attributional effects via emotion-cognition integration mechanisms (Lerner and Keltner, 2000). These results challenge conventional emotion decision theories by demonstrating the unexpected inhibitory effects of anger. While anger typically activates behavioral approach systems (Harmon-Jones et al., 2013), it paradoxically enhanced cognitive control in this context, possibly by increasing perceived agency (Lerner and Tiedens, 2006) or narrowing attentional focus (Gable and Harmon-Jones, 2010). This underscores the need to differentiate motivational dimensions of anger (e.g., directed vs. diffuse; Carver and Harmon-Jones, 2009) in future research. Intervention strategies should integrate cognitive restructuring (Schaeuffele et al., 2024) and emotion regulation training (Gross, 2015) for pessimistic individuals while fostering emotion-cognition awareness (Salovey et al., 2009) across student populations. Scenario-based simulations (Loewenstein, 2005) could further help students practice rational decision making under emotional arousal. It is important to note that although using anger induction was effective, it co-activated other negative emotions such as fear and disgust. Thus, the suppression of impulsive consumption may reflect a broader response to high-arousal negative states besides general anger. Future research should aim to isolate the unique cognitive and motivational aspects of anger (e.g., sense of certainty, approach motivation) using more targeted manipulations to clarify its distinct role in consumption behavior.

5.3 High self-control effectively mitigates college students’ impulsive consumption

This study revealed the synergistic mechanisms between attributional style and self-control in impulsive consumption. Optimistic attributional styles buffered impulsivity significantly, with protective effects varying across self-control levels. These findings refine the social cognitive theory (Bandura, 2023) by demonstrating how attributional styles modulate self-control resource allocation. Optimists maintained rational decision-making even under self-control depletion, challenging the universality of ego depletion theory (Baumeister et al., 2007) and supporting control-process theory (Fujita et al., 2024) on goal clarity. The results advocate for a tripartite intervention framework: (1) Tiered interventions: combining cognitive restructuring and executive function training for high-risk students (low self-control + pessimistic attribution; Duckworth et al., 2016); (2) Attributional training: integrating growth mindset cultivation (Dweck, 2006) and metacognitive skill development into curricula; and (3) Environment redesign: applying behavioral economics principles (Leonard, 2008) to implement decision prompts and delay mechanisms. Universities should prioritize three core competencies: rational attribution, impulse control, and emotion regulation, adopting a “pilot-refine-scale” strategy for program implementation.

5.4 Limitations

The study sample was confined to students aged 17–22 years from a single university in Changsha, which may limit the generalizability of our findings. Regional and cultural specificities could affect the applicability of the results to broader student populations. Future research should expand the sample to include students from diverse geographical regions and higher education institutions to enhance the external validity of the findings. Furthermore, the reliance on self-reported responses to hypothetical third-person scenarios, while methodologically convenient, may limit the generalizability of findings to actual purchasing behavior. Future studies could employ more ecologically valid measures, such as behavioral economic tasks involving real monetary incentives or first-person virtual shopping simulations, to corroborate our findings. This study employed two separate 2 × 2 between-subject designs to examine variable relationships. Future studies could adopt a 2 × 2 × 2 mixed-design to simultaneously investigate the interaction effects of three variables, and utilize longitudinal tracking studies to elucidate the dynamic processes underlying causal relationships between variables. Furthermore, the gender distribution across our experiments was uneven. This imbalance may limit the generalizability of our findings and potentially confound the observed effects, as prior empirical theory suggests the prevalence of significant gender differences in impulsive consumption behavior. Future studies should strive for a more balanced gender representation to explicitly examine and control for the influence of this variable. Additionally, employing physiological measures (e.g., skin conductance response, heart rate variability) could provide objective, convergent evidence to robustly support the efficacy of the emotional induction procedures used in such research, and move beyond self-report measures.

6 Conclusion

College students’ impulsive consumption exhibits significant attributional style differences: optimists suppress impulsivity, while pessimists are more prone to impulsive behaviors; anger induction amplifies these attributional differences, highlighting the emotion’s critical moderating role in consumption decisions; and high self-control effectively buffers the negative impact of pessimistic attributional styles on impulsive consumption.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee of Hunan Normal University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

GZ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. WS: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft. WQ: Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

We sincerely thank the research team for their invaluable support and contributions to this study. We are particularly grateful to Huang Zijun for her guidance on revising this manuscript. We also extend our appreciation to our research group supervisors, peers, and the reviewers for their valuable feedback, which has significantly enhanced the quality of this paper.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E., and Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 87, 49–74. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.87.1.49

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antosz-Rekucka, R., and Prochwicz, K. (2024). The relationship between trauma, stress, and premenstrual symptoms: the role of attributional style and trait anger. J. Reprod. Infant Psychol. 1-14, 1–14. doi: 10.1080/02646838.2024.2377099

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bajtelsmit, V. L. (2024). Personal finance. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (2023). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective on human nature. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., and Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 351–355. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bian, J. F., Li, X., Huang, T. J., Zhou, Y. X., and Fu, X. L. (2025). Adverse childhood experiences and adolescent hypocrisy: the chain mediating role of self-depletion and self-control. China J. Health Psychol. 33, 1–14. doi: 10.13342/j.cnki.cjhp.2025.06.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., and Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: evidence and implications. Psychol. Bull. 135, 183–204. doi: 10.1037/a0013965

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., and Scheier, M. F. (2001). On the self-regulation of behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Chen, S., Lai, J., Tian, C., Tian, Y., and Du, X. (2025). The double-edged sword effect of differentiated empowering leadership on employee voice. Curr. Psychol. 44, 4705–4723. doi: 10.1007/s12144-025-07474-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Disner, S. G., Beevers, C. G., Haigh, E. A., and Beck, A. T. (2011). Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 467–477. doi: 10.1038/nrn3027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duckworth, A. L., Gendler, T. S., and Gross, J. J. (2016). Situational strategies for self-control. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 11, 35–55. doi: 10.1177/1745691615623247

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York: Random House.

Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Folkman, S., and Lazarus, R. S. (1988). Coping as a mediator of emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 466–475. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.3.466

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujita, K., Trope, Y., and Liberman, N. (2024). Understanding self-control as a problem of regulatory scope. Psychol. Rev. 132, 50–75. doi: 10.1037/rev0000501

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gable, P., and Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The motivational dimensional model of affect: implications for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive categorisation. Cognit. Emot. 24, 322–337. doi: 10.1080/02699930903378305

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: current status and future prospects. Psychol. Inq. 26, 1–26. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, T., Cai, C., Xu, Y., Huang, M., and Ni, Y. (2024). Self-esteem and college students’ online impulse buying: the independent and interactive moderating role of interdependent self-construal and interpersonal relationships. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 17, 4447–4461. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S494636

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., and Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science 324, 646–648. doi: 10.1126/science.1168450

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., and Price, T. F. (2013). Does negative affect always narrow and positive affect always broaden the mind? Considering the influence of motivational intensity on cognitive scope. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 301–307. doi: 10.1177/0963721413481353

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Herabadi, A. G., Verplanken, B., and van Knippenberg, A. (2009). Consumption experience of impulse buying in Indonesia: emotional arousal and hedonistic considerations. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 12, 20–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-839X.2008.01266.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Förster, G., and Vohs, K. D. (2012a). Everyday temptations: an experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 102, 1318–1335. doi: 10.1037/a0026545

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., and Baddeley, A. D. (2012b). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 10, 144–156. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpg016

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koley, S., and Warren, C. (2025). Anger and self-control: how the need to dominate can lead to better choices. J. Public Policy Mark. 44, 242–260. doi: 10.1177/07439156241265076

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Leonard, T. C. (2008). Richard H. Thaler, Cass R. Sunstein, nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. New Haven: Yale University Press.

Google Scholar

Lerner, J. S., and Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognit. Emot. 14, 473–493. doi: 10.1080/026999300402763

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lerner, J. S., and Tiedens, L. Z. (2006). Portrait of the angry decision maker: how appraisal tendencies shape anger's influence on cognition. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 19, 115–137. doi: 10.1002/bdm.515

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X. Y., Zhang, J., Zhang, Z. Y., and Mu, L. (2025). The response inhibition ability of impulsive violent offenders under two types of anger emotions. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 1, 174–179. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2025.01.032

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, S. W., Zhao, J. B., and Zhao, J. B. (2017). Effect of mental-health curriculum on the emotion and attitudes toward seeking professional help of college students. Chin. J. Health Educ. 33, 745–748. doi: 10.16168/j.cnki.issn.1002-9982.2017.08.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loewenstein, G. (2000). Emotions in economic theory and economic behavior. Am. Econ. Rev. 90, 426–432. doi: 10.1257/aer.90.2.426

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loewenstein, G. (2005). Hot-cold empathy gaps and medical decision making. Health Psychol. 24, S49–S56. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.24.4.S49

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mamayek, C., Paternoster, R., and Loughran, T. A. (2017). Self-control as self-regulation: a return to control theory. Deviant Behav. 38, 895–916. doi: 10.1080/01639625.2016.1206730

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mischel, W., and Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychol. Rev. 102, 246–268. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.246

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, C., and Seligman, M. E. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: theory and evidence. Psychol. Rev. 91, 347–374. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.91.3.347

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., MacCallum, R. C., and Nicewander, W. A. (2005). Use of the extreme groups approach: a critical reexamination and new recommendations. Psychol. Methods 10, 178–192. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.10.2.178

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Puri, R. (1996). Measuring and modifying consumer impulsiveness: a cost-benefit accessibility framework. J. Consum. Psychol. 5, 87–113. doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp0502_01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Redine, A., Deshpande, S., Jebarajakirthy, C., and Surachartkumtonkun, J. (2023). Impulse buying: a systematic literature review and future research directions. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 47, 3–41. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12862

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Renström, E. A., Bäck, H., and Carroll, R. (2023). Threats, emotions, and affective polarization. Polit. Psychol. 44, 1337–1366. doi: 10.1111/pops.12899

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rook, D. W. (1987). The buying impulse. J. Consum. Res. 14, 189–199. doi: 10.1086/209105

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rook, D. W., and Fisher, R. J. (1995). Normative influences on impulsive buying behavior. J. Consum. Res. 22, 305–313. doi: 10.1086/209452

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., and Yoo, S. H. (2009). “The positive psychology of emotional intelligence” in The Oxford handbook of positive psychology. eds. Lopez, J. J. and Snyder, C. R. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Sanders, K. (2024). “Attribution theory” in A guide to key theories for human resource management research. (Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing), 44–51.

Google Scholar

Schaeuffele, C., Meine, L. E., Schulz, A., Weber, M. C., Moser, A., Paersch, C., et al. (2024). A systematic review and meta-analysis of transdiagnostic cognitive behavioural therapies for emotional disorders. Nat. Hum. Behav. 8, 493–509. doi: 10.1038/s41562-023-01787-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherer, K. R. (2005). What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Soc. Sci. Inf. 44, 695–729. doi: 10.1177/0539018405058216

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E., Schulman, P., DeRubeis, R. J., and Hollon, S. D. (1999). The prevention of depression and anxiety. Prev. Treat. 2:Article 8a. doi: 10.1037/1522-3736.2.1.28a

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, X., Cheng, Y., Xie, Z., Gong, N., and Liu, L. (2021). The influence of anger on delay discounting: the mediating role of certainty and control. Acta Psychol. Sin. 53:456. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00456

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, S. H., and Guo, Y. Y. (2008). Revision of self-control scale for Chinese college students. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 16, 468–470. Available at: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2008-19347-007

Google Scholar

Verhagen, T., and Van Dolen, W. (2011). The influence of online store beliefs on consumer online impulse buying: a model and empirical application. Inf. Manag. 48, 320–327. doi: 10.1016/j.im.2011.08.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Verplanken, B., and Herabadi, A. (2001). Individual differences in impulsive buying tendency: feeling and no thinking. Eur. J. Personal. 15, 71–83. doi: 10.1002/per.423

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vohs, K. D., and Faber, R. J. (2007). Spent resources: self-regulatory resource availability affects impulse buying. J. Consum. Res. 33, 537–547. doi: 10.1086/510228

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, D., and Liu, Y. (2008). An experimental research on the effects of attributive style and time pressure on purchasing decision. Psychol. Sci. 31, 905–913. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2008.04.024

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Lu, H., and Wang, D. (2020). Buy or not: how the presence of others affects the occurrence of consumers' impulsive buying behavior. J. Contemp. Mark. Sci. 3, 207–224. doi: 10.1108/JCMARS-01-2020-0002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C., and Zhang, N. (2006). A study on scale of attributional style questionnaire. Chin J Behav Med Sci. 15, 470–471. doi: 10.3760/cma.j.issn.1674-6554.2006.05.041

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychol. Rev. 92, 548–573. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.92.4.548

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, M. Y., Croarkin, P. E., Lee, C. K., and Lee, P. F. (2021). Validation of pictorial mood assessment with Ottawa mood scales and the positive and negative affect scale for young adults. Community Ment. Health J. 57, 529–539. doi: 10.1007/s10597-020-00679-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, J., Yoon, N., and Choo, H. J. (2023). How online shopping festival atmosphere promotes consumer participation in China. Fash. Text. 10:5. doi: 10.1186/s40691-022-00325-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, P., Huang, Y., and Luo, Y. (2010). Establishment and assessment of native Chinese affective video system. Chin. Ment. Health J. 24, 551–554. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2010.07.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xue, S., Junru, H., Zhao, L., and Ning, L. (2023). Having or lacking power leads to impulse buying? The influence of power and buying impulsiveness trait on impulse buying. J. Psychol. Sci. 46:1188. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20230520

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, S. Q., Wang, W. F., Wu, S., and Wang, Y. (2021). Influence of attribution style on job performance in employee: mediating effect of decision-making style. China J. Health Psychol. 29, 1225–1230. doi: 10.13342/j.cnki.cjhp.2021.08.024

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yi, S., and Baumgartner, H. (2011). Coping with guilt and shame in the impulse buying context. J. Econ. Psychol. 32, 458–467. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2011.03.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Y., Hu, Y., and Zhao, Y. (2017). What do you listen to under the pressure of time? The moderator effects of reference group on impulsive buying. Acta Psychol. Sin. 49:1439. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.01439

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: college students impulsive consumption, attributional style, anger, self-control, educational interventions

Citation: Zhou G, Sun W and Qu W (2025) Influence of attributional style, anger, and self-control on college students’ impulsive consumption. Front. Psychol. 16:1641481. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1641481

Received: 01 July 2025; Accepted: 08 September 2025;
Published: 26 September 2025.

Edited by:

Dawei Cao, Huaibei Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Chaoxin Ji, Northeastern University, China
Yuehang Yang, Huaibei Normal University, China

Copyright © 2025 Zhou, Sun and Qu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Weiguo Qu, MTI3MjhAaHVubnUuZWR1LmNu

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.