Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 05 September 2025

Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1663529

Researchers aren't the story: why we should cite theories and findings, not people

  • Department of Psychology, University of Bamberg, Bamberg, Germany

Authors, especially those of review papers or book chapters, often explicitly mention the name of a specific researcher or research group when describing an experimental procedure that led to the detection of a phenomenon, or they highlight the big name behind a theory. Here, I argue that such practices, despite being popular, are subject to a variety of illusions and biases, and that referring to researchers indirectly (in parentheses, at the end of sentences) would be more advisable.

Simultaneous discoveries

Letterpress printing was invented by Johannes Gutenberg in 1450—a fact many pupils learn in school. However, it's less well known that, 400 years earlier in China, another one (Bi Cheng) invented another form of letterpress printing (Needham, 1974). Despite using a different technique, Cheng's work was recognized by Europeans in the fourteenth century, and craftsman in Italy and the Netherlands worked on similar printing methods. So, would there be no letterpress without Gutenberg? Similarly, Thomas Alva Edison is commonly viewed as the inventor of the light bulb. However, several other people worked on prototypes or earlier versions that brought wires to light (Friedel and Israel, 2010). So, would we sit in the dark at night today if there had been no Edison?

These two examples briefly illustrate that important historical discoveries cannot be easily attributed to a single person. The invention of new technologies and the progression of knowledge would likely have occurred even without the people we most strongly associate with them. It could be argued that historical narratives often favor clear, single names (like Gutenberg, Edison) because they are easier to teach and remember (Basalla, 2010; Brooks, 2024). However, innovation is typically cumulative and collaborative—many people contribute in overlapping ways, but fame often comes down to the person who commercialized or popularized the invention most successfully (Bazerman, 1999).

This also applies to several psychological theories and phenomena that were discovered—or at least described—around the same time by independent researchers or labs, but only gained widespread recognition through later researchers. For examples across social, personality, developmental, and cognitive psychology, see Table 1. Moreover, psychological research often suffers from the jangle fallacy, meaning that two constructs are identical but are labeled differently (Block, 1995; Lawson and Robins, 2021). Researchers may gain recognition for introducing a “new” concept, even though it might be the same as an existing one under a different name (see Altgassen et al., 2024; Ponnock et al., 2020). Focusing on the underlying construct or theory and citing that theory rather than individual researchers who use different labels can help avoid such jangle fallacies.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Psychological theories and phenomena discovered by independent researchers.

Psychological biases in understanding scientific progress

Simultaneous discoveries support the validity, robustness, and generalizability of phenomena, but they also show that often no single person or research group is completely responsible for their exploration (Merton, 1961). Rather, they suggest that some phenomena are “in the air” (e.g., due to newly available methods, pressing societal issues), and would be discovered sooner or later (Kelly, 2010). Thus, attributing a unique contribution to any particular individual or research lab is limited by these considerations. This may even lead to the strong conclusion that attributing discoveries to a single researcher may be misleading. However, I do not intend to diminish the contributions of those who help develop or detect new psychological phenomena or technologies. In fact, we are all subject to the illusion of control (Langer, 1975; Presson and Benassi, 1996), that is, we believe we control things we actually cannot control because they are determined by external forces. In other words, no single person fully controls the emergence of new theories or phenomena, because scientific, societal, historical, and cultural factors often have a stronger influence than any one individual. Typically, chance plays a stronger role than we assume (Frank, 2016; Pluchino et al., 2018). Similarly, the intentional stance (Dennett, 1989) suggests that we interpret people's actions as purposeful. Again, we tend to be biased in our perceptions: We overestimate internal factors, such as the intention to develop a new theory, and underestimate external factors. In social psychology, this is similar to the fundamental attribution error (or correspondence bias, Gawronski, 2004).

Moreover, when someone gains attention for a new theory or phenomenon, it can spread widely: The more often people read about the new theory, the more they may like it (mere-exposure effect, Zajonc, 1968). Thus, people (or even universities or countries) who are good at marketing their research may also be rewarded by people liking their research. For example, confident individuals and people in countries with a more individualistic orientation may be especially skilled at promoting their ideas (Avia et al., 1998; Hofstede, 1980) and, in turn, be more likely to receive credit. This can lead others to adopt the new theory in their own work, eventually making the theory more visible and popular because people use it and talk about it (social contagion, Levy and Nail, 1993). Over time, this creates a self-reinforcing cycle in which the theory becomes increasingly successful. Psychological research supports the idea that there is truth in the saying, “To him that hath shall be given” (Matthew effect, Merton, 1968). For example, theories with many citations often continue to get even more attention and citations, because people think that this is a key work (Beel and Gipp, 2009; Perc, 2014). Therefore, it is advisable to evaluate the content of a theory rather than relying solely on its citation count.

Conclusions for citing behavior

Now that I have briefly argued that theories and phenomena are often not attributable to a single person or research group—and that several psychological biases lead to some researchers being placed on a pedestal while others do not receive similar recognition—what does this mean for citation practices? When a researcher's name is used as the subject of a sentence, it reinforces the illusion of sole discovery or intent (“this researcher is solely responsible for intentionally detecting this phenomenon”), which boosts the popularity of the research (social contagion processes and “success breeds success”) more than when the focus is on the theory or phenomenon itself. This practice may even be irrational, especially when the “founders” of key theories are no longer alive or active—meaning that citing their work is unlikely to lead to more favorable peer reviews or publication decisions. By using indirect citations (referring to researchers in parentheses), we still acknowledge their scientific contributions, but we shift the focus away from the individual. Instead, we emphasize the theory, phenomena, or finding (e.g., Bem, 1995). In addition, indirect citations often improve flow and readability. For example, instead of writing, “Bem (1995) suggests using indirect citations to focus on findings,” it is recommended to write “Indirect citations can shift the focus to findings (Bem, 1995).”

Further, if there are multiple people associated with a theory or phenomenon, their names can be placed in parentheses at the end of the sentence (“e.g.,”) to denote that the list may not be exhaustive. Similarly, if there are multiple studies that demonstrate a finding, citing a meta-analytic review in parentheses at the end of the sentence would be advisable. These practices improve readability compared to listing names at the beginning of a sentence. Such considerations—along with the fact that placing names at the end of sentences shifts the focus from the people to the theory or finding—are especially important for students who try to memorize theories and names for exams. In fact, it may even be advisable to avoid testing names in exams, in order to reduce biases related to sole discovery or the Matthew effect, as outlined above. Indirect citations are also important for other audiences who might “get lost in the weeds,” such as laypeople, politicians, journalists, or pediatricians. Overall, using indirect citations may help reduce psychological biases and illusions that contribute to inequality in the recognition of scientific contributions.

Author contributions

RK: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. The publication of this article was supported by the Open Access publication fund of the University of Bamberg.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Altgassen, E., Geiger, M., and Wilhelm, O. (2024). Do you mind a closer look? A jingle-jangle fallacy perspective on mindfulness. Eur. J. Pers.38, 365–387. doi: 10.1177/08902070231174575

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Avia, M. D., Sanchez-Bernardos, M. L., Sanz, J., Carrillo, J., and Rojo, N. (1998). Self-presentation strategies and the five-factor model. J. Res. Pers. 32, 108–114. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.1997.2205

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Basalla, G. (2010). The Evolution of Technology. Cambridge: Cambridge History of Science Series.

Google Scholar

Bazerman, C. (1999). The Languages of Edison's Light. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/4130.001.0001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beel, J., and Gipp, B. (2009). “Google Scholar's ranking algorithm: an introductory overview,” in Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Scientometrics and Informetrics (ISSI 09) 1, 230–241.

Google Scholar

Bem, D. J. (1995). Writing a review article for psychological bulletin. Psychol. Bull. 118, 172–177. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.2.172

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description. Psychol. Bull. 117, 187–215. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.187

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss (No. 79). New York, NY: Random House.

Google Scholar

Brooks, L. (2024). “Nonanalytic concept formation and memory for instances,” in Cognition and Categorization (Routledge), 169–211. doi: 10.4324/9781032633275-10

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carney, D. R., Cuddy, A. J., and Yap, A. J. (2010). Power posing: brief nonverbal displays affect neuroendocrine levels and risk tolerance. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1363–1368. doi: 10.1177/0956797610383437

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dennett, D. C. (1989). The Intentional Stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.

Google Scholar

Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cogn. Emot. 6, 169–200. doi: 10.1080/02699939208411068

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, R. H. (2016). Success and Luck: Good Fortune and The Myth of Meritocracy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. doi: 10.1515/9781400880270

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedel, R., and Israel, P. B. (2010). Edison's Electric Light: The Art of Invention. Baltimore, MD: JHU Press.

Google Scholar

Gawronski, B. (2004). Theory-based bias correction in dispositional inference: the fundamental attribution error is dead, long live the correspondence bias. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 15, 183–217. doi: 10.1080/10463280440000026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. Am. Psychol. 13, 673–685. doi: 10.1037/h0047884

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values. Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Jones, E. E., and Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 3, 1–24. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(67)90034-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, K. (2010). What Technology Wants. London: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Laird, J. D. (1974). Self-attribution of emotion: the effects of expressive behavior on the quality of emotional experience. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 29, 475–486. doi: 10.1037/h0036125

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 32, 311–328. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.32.2.311

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanzetta, J. T., Cartwright-Smith, J., and Eleck, R. E. (1976). Effects of nonverbal dissimulation on emotional experience and autonomic arousal. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 33, 354–370. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.33.3.354

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawson, K. M., and Robins, R. W. (2021). Sibling constructs: What are they, why do they matter, and how should you handle them? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 25, 344–366. doi: 10.1177/10888683211047101

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, D. A., and Nail, P. R. (1993). Contagion: a theoretical and empirical review and reconceptualization. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 119, 233–284.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Merton, R. K. (1961). Singletons and multiples in scientific discovery: a chapter in the sociology of science. Proc. Am. Philos. Soc. 105, 470–486.

Google Scholar

Merton, R. K. (1968). The Matthew effect in science: the reward and communication systems of science are considered. Science 159, 56–63. doi: 10.1126/science.159.3810.56

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Needham, J. (1974). Science and Civilisation in China Vol. 5. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Perc, M. (2014). The Matthew effect in empirical data. J. R. Soc. Interface 11:20140378. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2014.0378

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pluchino, A., Biondo, A. E., and Rapisarda, A. (2018). Talent versus luck: the role of randomness in success and failure. Adv. Complex Syst. 21:1850014. doi: 10.1142/S0219525918500145

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ponnock, A., Muenks, K., Morell, M., Yang, J. S., Gladstone, J. R., and Wigfield, A. (2020). Grit and conscientiousness: Another jangle fallacy. J. Res. Pers. 89:104021. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2020.104021

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Premack, D., and Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav. Brain Sci. 1, 515–526. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00076512

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Presson, P. K., and Benassi, V. A. (1996). Illusion of control: a meta-analytic review. J. Soc. Behav. Person. 11, 493–510.

Google Scholar

Riskind, J. H., and Gotay, C. C. (1982). Physical posture: could it have regulatory or feedback effects on motivation and emotion? Motiv. Emot. 6, 273–298. doi: 10.1007/BF00992249

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, L. (1977). “The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: distortions in the attribution process,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (London: Academic Press), 10, 173–220 doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60357-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Strack, F., Martin, L. L., and Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 768–777. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.768

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect imagery consciousness: Vol 1, the positive affects. London: Tavistock.

Google Scholar

Wimmer, H., and Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition 13, 103–128. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 9, 1–27 doi: 10.1037/h0025848

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: indirect citation, citation behavior, authorship, illusion of control, intentional stance

Citation: Körner R (2025) Researchers aren't the story: why we should cite theories and findings, not people. Front. Psychol. 16:1663529. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1663529

Received: 10 July 2025; Accepted: 07 August 2025;
Published: 05 September 2025.

Edited by:

Kay Brauer, Martin Luther University of Halle-Wittenberg, Germany

Reviewed by:

Lisa-Marie Rau, Vestische Kinder- und Jugendklinik Datteln, Germany

Copyright © 2025 Körner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Robert Körner, cm9iZXJ0LmtvZXJuZXJAdW5pLWJhbWJlcmcuZGU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.