- 1Geely University of China, Chengdu, China
- 2Sichuan Normal University, Chengdu, China
Objectives: Emerging evidence highlights the benefits of integrating physical activity and cognitive strategies, such as motor imagery (MI), into educational contexts to support language acquisition and skill development. However, the interactive effects of physical education (PE) and MI on second language learning remain underexplored.
Methods: This study investigated the combined impact of PE, MI, and English instruction on vocabulary retention, motor performance, and student enjoyment in junior secondary students. Using a crossover repeated-measures design, 92 students (aged 12–13) participated in seven randomized class sessions incorporating different combinations of PE, MI, and English (alone, in pairs, or all together) over a two-week period. Vocabulary retention was assessed through pre- and post-class English quizzes, motor skill performance was measured using basketball passing accuracy, and enjoyment was evaluated through post-session surveys.
Results: All combinations led to significant post-session improvements in English quiz scores (p < 0.001), with the highest improvements observed in the PE + MI + English condition. Similar patterns were found for passing accuracy, where PE-containing sessions produced the greatest improvements, particularly when combined with MI and English (p < 0.001). Enjoyment scores were also significantly higher in integrated conditions, especially PE + MI + English, suggesting increased engagement. A significant interaction between class type and time was found for both vocabulary and motor skill performance (p < 0.001), with very large effect sizes (ηp2 = 0.772 and 0.699, respectively).
Conclusion: These findings suggest that integrating physical, cognitive, and linguistic activities may positively impact both learning outcomes and student experience, supporting a multidisciplinary, multimodal approach to education. However, longitudinal studies are needed to identify potential long-term effects on consolidated learning.
1 Introduction
Recent advances in cognitive science and educational psychology suggest the benefits of integrating physical movement with language learning, grounded in the framework of embodied cognition (Shapiro, 2007). According to embodied cognition theory (Leitan and Chaffey, 2014), language and thought are not isolated mental processes but are fundamentally shaped by bodily actions and sensorimotor experiences. In this view, learning a second language becomes more effective when it is tied to physical context—particularly actions that can be seen, felt, or imagined (Atkinson, 2010). Within Physical Education (PE), learners engage in concrete motor tasks that offer natural opportunities to contextualize English vocabulary and grammatical structures, especially action verbs, prepositions, and instructional phrases (Forey and Cheung, 2019). These physically grounded interactions enhance retention by activating dual coding mechanisms, where verbal and non-verbal information is processed in parallel (Mir et al., 2023), supporting stronger memory encoding and recall. By engaging both channels, learners form richer mental representations that are more resistant to forgetting. This dual-channel processing is a core principle of multimodal learning (Paivio, 1986), helping explain why pairing movement with language input can yield stronger memory encoding and retrieval in second language acquisition.
Motor imagery (MI) aims to serve as a cognitive bridge between movement and language by mentally simulating physical actions, engaging brain areas involved in both motor planning and verbal processing (Munzert et al., 2009). Research suggests that MI may activate neural networks similar to those involved in actual movement (Sharma and Baron, 2013), which can reinforce physical skill acquisition (Ingram et al., 2016) and, when guided through language, deepen comprehension of action-related vocabular (Bayram et al., 2023). Thus, it is hypothesized that embedding MI within PE sessions conducted in English may enable learners to rehearse both motor sequences and linguistic structures simultaneously, increasing cognitive engagement.
The available research suggests that integrating PE with English language learning can be mutually beneficial. PE-in-CLIL (Content and Language Integrated Learning) approaches have shown significant improvements in students’ oral communication skills (Coral and Lleixà, 2016). Physical activity may favour cognitive functions and memory, leading to better academic performance, including in English (Yurko et al., 2019). Integrated PE and English lessons can increase motivation for language learning and promote a healthy lifestyle (Bashta and Zalutskaya, 2021). Combining PE theory with practical sessions cultivates collaboration skills and multi-faceted thinking abilities (Yamaki and Iwata, 2020). These integrated approaches not only improve physical skills but also develop critical thinking and create interconnections between disciplines (Yamaki and Iwata, 2020).
MI activates similar neural substrates as motor execution, potentially facilitating motor skill learning (Anwar et al., 2011) and improving language performance (Bayram et al., 2023). Studies have shown that kinesthetic motor-imagery training can lead to better performance in lexical-semantic access tasks compared to static visual imagery training (Bonnet et al., 2022) This improvement in language comprehension can be attributed to the relationship between the language and motor systems. Additionally, technology-based interventions incorporating L2 imagery techniques have showed benefits for learner engagement and language acquisition in English for Academic Purposes courses (Gonzalez Mujico and Lasagabaster, 2019). Although there are some positive preliminary results, research focusing on MI and English acquisition is still very limited, so further studies are needed to fully understand the potential impact.
Although existing evidence points to potentially beneficial effects of PE classes on language acquisition and the use of MI for both motor skill and language learning, findings in this area remain inconsistent. Some studies report positive outcomes of MI for vocabulary retention and skill transfer (Bonnet et al., 2022), yet research replicating and confirming these results is scarce. This scarcity may contribute to discrepancies in findings and practical applications, potentially arising from differences in research approaches, small sample sizes, short intervention durations, and a lack of well-controlled comparisons between instructional combinations (e.g., directly comparing MI with PE). Moreover, no study has examined the interaction between PE and MI in relation to second language acquisition—particularly in English—while systematically addressing these methodological limitations. Combining these interventions may also enhance student engagement through increased enjoyment during classes, yet this potential remains underexplored in integrated designs. This gap underscores the originality of the present study, which adopts a crossover repeated-measures design to investigate the combined and separate effects of PE, MI, and English instruction. Specifically, it examines how different configurations of these elements (individually, in pairs, or all together) influence post-class vocabulary retention, student enjoyment, and skill accuracy, thereby contributing new insights toward the development of engaging, multidisciplinary approaches to skill development.
2 Methods
2.1 Study design
This study employed a crossover repeated-measures design, in which the same group of students participated in seven randomized class sessions (Figure 1). This design was chosen over a between-subjects approach to control for inter-individual variability in factors such as baseline motor skills, cognitive ability, and language proficiency. By having each student serve as their own control, this design minimizes confounding effects of individual differences. Furthermore, the repeated-measures nature of the design allows for a direct comparison of the impact of each instructional condition within the same participants, providing a more accurate evaluation of the effects of PE, MI, and English instruction alone and in combination.

Figure 1. Different class combinations students experienced during 2 weeks. PE, physical education; MI, motor imagery.
The sessions integrated PE, MI, and English instruction in various combinations—delivered individually, in pairs, or all three together. The sessions took place over two consecutive weeks, during which students attended three PE classes per week. Figure 1 illustrates the different class combinations students experienced during this period. Each student participated in one session of each combination, with measurements taken both before and after each class. To minimize sequencing bias, the order and sequence of conditions were randomized. The procedure, detailed later in the section on classes, involved implementing three different condition orders and assigning students classes randomly to the sequences using opaque envelopes. Given the educational context, the randomization was carried out within the three classes of student participants. The tests were administered under similar conditions before and after each session, using reliable measures.
2.2 Participants
To determine the a priori sample size required for two-way repeated measures ANOVA, a power analysis was conducted. Based on the aim of recruiting approximately 90 participants, the effect size was targeted to achieve this sample size. Specifically, assuming an f2 of approximately 0.028, which falls within the range typically considered a small effect size according to Cohen’s conventions. The desired statistical power was set at 0.80, and the alpha level (Type I error rate) was established at 0.05. Assuming a moderate correlation of 0.5 among the repeated measures, and utilizing specialized statistical software (e.g., G*Power 3.1), the estimated minimum total sample size required to detect the hypothesized interaction effect with the specified power and alpha level is approximately 90 participants.
Convenience sampling was conducted at a junior secondary school, involving male and female students aged 12 to 13. The school had implemented various pedagogical and didactic innovations, including a multidisciplinary approach that integrated PE and English. This integration was part of the school’s strategy to enhance teaching effectiveness and support second language learning. As part of this initiative, the PE and English departments collaboratively developed a combined teaching approach. This study followed that initiative, analyzing the effects of the integrated classes through a pre- and post-intervention design, focusing on the interactions between the two disciplines.
Among the students, 92 (46 boys and 46 girls) were identified as volunteers based on the following eligibility criteria: (i) participation in all classes throughout the observation period; (ii) ability to attend both pre- and post-class evaluations; and (iii) being between 12 and 13 years old. Participants were excluded from the analysis if they (i) missed any classes during the period or (ii) declined to participate in the evaluations. At baseline, before the study began, the students were assessed using the Cambridge English: Preliminary for Schools (PET) test. Fifteen students demonstrated a B1 level (Intermediate), 49 an A2 level (Elementary), and 27 an A1 level (Breakthrough).
The students and their parents were informed about the study and the evaluation process. They were told that although the classes were part of the regular curriculum, participation in the pre- and post-evaluations was voluntary. It was made clear that there would be no obligation to take part in the assessments and that non-participation would not result in any penalties. All students and their parents agreed to participate, and the parents signed an informed consent form. Furthermore, the study design was approved by the ethics committee of Sichuan Normal University (approval code: 2025LS0058), in accordance with ethical standards for research involving youth populations.
2.3 The classes
The classes were conducted according to the regular school schedule and formed part of the students’ standard curriculum. To minimize sequencing effects, students experienced the activities in different orders. Given the large number of possible permutations (7 conditions – 5,040 possible sequences) and the practical constraints of implementing all sequences in a school setting, only three specific combinations were used, which were selected by generating random sequences from the multiple possible options. These were: Ordering 1 (PE → MI → ENG → PE + MI → PE + ENG → MI + ENG → PE + MI + ENG), Ordering 2 (ENG → PE + MI + ENG → PE + MI → MI → MI + ENG → PE + ENG → PE), and Ordering 3 (MI + ENG → PE → PE + MI → PE + MI + ENG → PE + ENG → MI → ENG).
Each class was co-taught by an English teacher and a PE teacher, both certified professionals with over five years of experience. The same teachers and teaching methods were used throughout to ensure consistent conditions for all students. The MI and PE classes were conducted in both Mandarin (for general instructions) and English (for specific communication in the sports contexts and interaction with peers). Students were required to communicate in English to describe their actions and to interact with peers and teachers during PE activities. To unify the content across the three main areas—English, MI, and PE—basketball was chosen as the central theme, specifically focusing on passing and match-passing scenarios, a topic of high interest among the students. This thematic approach provided a core foundation across the different sessions. Table 1 shows the main content and pedagogical strategies used in the sessions.
The specific details of the English classes are presented in Table 2, which summarizes the schedule, activities, and their descriptions. All themes were specifically designed around the context of basketball, with a focus on key vocabulary related to the sport and useful phrases to support communication with peers.
Table 3 presents the activities and content covered in the MI classes. These sessions built on the students’ previous familiarity with the MI approach, extended through earlier learning experiences. In this specific context, the focus was on integrating basketball-related content into their imagery, aiming to enhance the overall learning process.
Table 4 presents the activities and content covered in the PE classes. The sessions focused on the passing skill, approached through a variety of exercises—some aimed specifically at developing the skill itself, and others incorporating it into game-like contexts (i.e., mini 3v3). The main instruction was delivered in Mandarin.
Table 5 presents the activities and content covered in the combined English and MI classes. These sessions integrated activities from both subjects, with a specific emphasis on using English as the primary language for both instruction and student communication. This was especially required when referring to key basketball terms and contexts.
Table 6 outlines the activities and content addressed in the integrated English and PE sessions. These classes combined elements from both subjects, with a strong emphasis on using English as the main language for instruction and student interaction—particularly during physical tasks that involved calling for the ball or performing specific movements.
Table 7 presents the activities and contexts explored in the combined MI and PE classes. Each session focused specifically on passing skills, with Mandarin used as the language of instruction.
Table 8 provides a detailed description of the session that integrated content and activities from English, MI, and PE. In this case, the majority of the session was conducted in English, requiring students to express themselves and interact in English to the best of their ability—consistent with all sessions where English was combined with MI and PE.
2.4 Measurements
All students participating in the study were tested both before and after the class using an English quiz and a passing accuracy assessment. Additionally, after the class, participants completed an enjoyment questionnaire related to the content they experienced. These evaluations were administered by members of the research team who did not take part in teaching the classes, in order to minimize potential evaluation bias.
2.5 English quiz
A topic-specific questionnaire was developed by the research team based on the students’ proficiency levels and the class content. After the initial draft, three independent experts reviewed the questionnaire blindly and provided feedback. The researchers then revised the questionnaire accordingly. Following this, a brief pilot test was conducted with 10 students who were not part of the main study to ensure the test was appropriate for their level and relevant to the learning context.
Once finalized, the quiz was administered both before and after the classes. Table 9 presents an example of the English quiz; although minor adjustments were made from class to class, all students received quizzes that followed the same structure and all students received the same quizzes over the time. These adjustments involved only vocabulary and terminology, while the overall format and components remained consistent. The quiz included sections on vocabulary, reading comprehension, writing, and listening. All tests were scored and reviewed by the same researchers, who were blinded to the specific classes the students attended. The final score was the total number of points earned on the quiz, ranging from 0 to a maximum of 30 points.
2.6 Passing skill accuracy
To assess basketball players’ passing precision and side-to-side agility, a 30-s drill involving passing against a wall was conducted (Wei et al., 2025). During the test, participants stood behind a marked line set 2.45 meters from the wall. The wall featured six square targets, each measuring 60 by 60 centimeters, arranged alternately at two different heights: some targets were placed so their bottom edges were 1.5 meters off the ground, while others were positioned at 90 centimeters. Players executed chest passes following a specific order, moving laterally to retrieve the ball while staying behind the designated line. Scoring awarded 2 points for passes hitting any part of a target square or its edges, and 1 point for passes that landed on the wall in the spaces between targets. The sum of these points during the 30-s interval served as a measure of each player’s passing accuracy and skill.
2.7 Short form of foreign language enjoyment scale
The short form of the Foreign Language Enjoyment Scale (S-FLES) was used in this study. This scale has been validated and tested for reliability, and is considered a simpler tool for examining individual differences in language learning (Botes et al., 2021). The questionnaire consists of 9 items: 1. The teacher is encouraging, 2. The teacher is friendly, 3. The teacher is supportive, 4. I enjoy it, 5. I’ve learned interesting things, 6. I am proud of my accomplishments, 7. We form a tight group, 8. We laugh a lot, and 9. We share common “legends.” Items 1 to 3 comprise the Teacher Appreciation subscale, items 4 to 6 form the Personal Enjoyment subscale, and items 7 to 9 make up the Social Enjoyment subscale. Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale, with 1 meaning “strongly disagree” and 5 meaning “strongly agree.” The scale was translated into Mandarin with the support of three experts to ensure accuracy of terminology. Additionally, the scale was introduced to students prior to the study period to familiarize them with the questions and improve the accuracy of their responses. The questionnaires were administered individually, and researchers remained available nearby to clarify any doubts about the interpretation of items. The final scores represented the sum of the individual scores given by the students.
2.8 Statistical analysis
Statistical analysis was conducted using a two-way repeated measures ANOVA to examine the effects of type of class and pre-post assessment on the English quiz and passing accuracy variables. This approach allowed us to assess both effect of the class (pre-post) and analyze variations on the class type effect, as well as their interaction. Effect sizes were calculated using partial eta squared (η2) to determine the magnitude of significant effects, with values interpreted according to conventional benchmarks (small: 0.01, medium: 0.06, large: 0.14). When significant interactions or main effects were found, post hoc pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction were performed to identify specific group differences. For enjoyment scale where only post-session data was available (no pre-post comparison), a one-way ANOVA was conducted to compare the class types at that single time point. To examine the potential effects of sequencing order, a one-way ANOVA was conducted on the mean differences (post–pre) in quiz scores, passing rates, and enjoyment ratings. For pairwise comparisons, Bonferroni corrections were applied. All analyses were conducted using SPSS (version 27), with a significance level set at p < 0.05.
3 Results
Table 10 presents the English quiz scores before and after the classes, along with the post-class passing accuracy rates and enjoyment levels. A significant two-way interaction between class and time, F = 308.785, p < 0.001,ηp2 = 0.772, was observed for the English quiz scores. There was also a significant two-way interaction between condition and time, F = 210.933, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.699 for the passing accuracy. There was also a significant main effect of class on enjoyment scores, F = 4223.896, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.979.

Table 10. Means and standard deviations for the English quiz scores, basketball passing accuracy, and reported enjoyment after the classes.
All class conditions showed a significant increase in English quiz scores from pre- to post-session (all p < 0.001). At pre-session, there were no significant differences in English quiz scores between any of the class conditions (all pairwise comparisons p > 0.05). At post-session, the PE (physical education) + MI (motor imagery) + ENG (English) condition showed significantly higher post-session quiz scores compared to all other conditions (e.g., vs. ENG: mean difference = 5.609, p < 0.001; vs. MI: mean difference = 4.967, p < 0.001; vs. PE: mean difference = 4.848, p < 0.001). The ENG + MI condition also resulted in significantly higher post-session scores compared to MI (mean difference = 4.091, p < 0.001) and PE alone (mean difference = 3.972, p < 0.001). Furthermore, the ENG condition showed significant differences compared to MI (mean difference = 3.652, p < 0.001) and PE (mean difference = 3.533, p < 0.001), and the PE + ENG condition showed significant differences compared to MI (mean difference = 3.880, p < 0.001) and PE (mean difference = 3.758, p < 0.001). Figure 2 presents the mean difference (post–pre) in English quiz scores across the different conditions and classes.

Figure 2. Scatterplots showing the mean differences (post–pre) in English quiz scores across the different conditions and classes. ENG, English; PE, physical education; MI: motor imagery.
Most class conditions showed a significant increase in passing accuracy scores from pre- to post-session (all p < 0.001), with the exception of the ENG class, which did not show a significant change (mean difference = 0.196, p = 0.371). At pre-session, there were no significant differences in passing accuracy scores between any of the class conditions (all pairwise comparisons p > 0.05). At post-session the PE + MI + ENG class showed significantly higher post-session passing accuracy scores compared to all other conditions (e.g., vs. ENG: mean difference = 16.511, p < 0.001; vs. MI: mean difference = 12.467, p < 0.001; vs. PE: mean difference = 3.972, p < 0.001). The MI + PE condition also resulted in significantly higher post-session scores compared to ENG (mean difference = 16.228, p < 0.001), MI (mean difference = 12.184, p < 0.001), and ENG + MI (mean difference = 10.185, p < 0.001). Furthermore, the PE condition showed significantly higher post-session scores compared to ENG (mean difference = 12.609, p < 0.001) and MI (mean difference = 7.337, p < 0.001). Figure 3 presents the mean difference (post–pre) in passing accuracy scores across the different conditions and classes.

Figure 3. Scatterplots showing the mean differences (post–pre) in passing accuracy scores across the different conditions and classes. ENG, English; PE, physical education; MI, motor imagery.
The PE + MI + ENG condition showed significantly higher enjoyment scores compared to all other conditions (e.g., vs. ENG: mean difference = 13.500, p < 0.001; vs. MI: mean difference = 18.533, p < 0.001; vs. PE: mean difference = 3.478, p < 0.001). The MI + PE condition also resulted in significantly higher enjoyment scores compared to ENG (mean difference = 10.989, p < 0.001) and MI (mean difference = 16.022, p < 0.001). Furthermore, the PE condition showed significantly higher enjoyment scores compared to ENG (mean difference = 10.022, p < 0.001) and MI (mean difference = 15.054, p < 0.001). Conversely, the MI condition consistently showed significantly lower enjoyment scores compared to all other conditions (e.g., vs. ENG: mean difference = −5.033, p < 0.001; vs. PE: mean difference = −15.054, p < 0.001). Figure 4 presents the scatterplots of enjoyment across the different conditions and classes.

Figure 4. Scatterplots showing the enjoyment levels across the different conditions and classes. ENG, English; PE, physical education; MI, motor imagery.
Table 11 presents the mean differences and associated p-values from the post-hoc pairwise comparisons between the different classes for English quiz scores (post-session), passing accuracy scores (post-session), and enjoyment scores (post-session). All p-values are adjusted for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni.

Table 11. Mean differences and associated p-values from the post-hoc pairwise comparisons between the different classes for English quiz scores (post-session), passing accuracy scores (post-session), and enjoyment scores (post-session).
To examine the potential effects of the implemented sequencing orders (1, 2, and 3, as detailed in the Methods section), a one-way ANOVA was conducted for each condition. Table 12 presents the statistical results from comparisons between order sequences. No significant differences were found in order sequencing for the English quiz or enjoyment levels across any of the conditions (p > 0.05). However, sequencing order significantly affected passing accuracy in the ENG condition (order 2 had significantly lower passing accuracy than order 3, p = 0.004) and the ENG&MI condition (order 1 had significantly lower passing accuracy than order 3, p = 0.006). No other significant differences in passing accuracy were observed across the remaining conditions (p > 0.05).

Table 12. Comparison of sequencing order effects on English quiz performance, passing accuracy, and enjoyment levels.
4 Discussion
The present study investigated the immediate effects of various class conditions (English, MI, PE, and their combinations) on English quiz scores, passing accuracy, and session enjoyment. For English quiz scores, improvement from pre- to post-session was observed across all conditions, coupled with significant differences between class types and an interaction indicating that the magnitude of improvement varied considerably depending on the specific class combination, with combined modalities generally yielding greater positive impact. Similarly, passing accuracy showed significant overall improvement over time and significant differences between conditions. While most class types led to enhanced passing accuracy, the English-only condition did not show a significant change, whereas conditions incorporating PE and MI, particularly in combination, exhibited the most substantial positive effects. Finally, post-session enjoyment varied significantly across class conditions, with combined approaches, especially those integrating PE and MI, reporting higher levels of enjoyment, contrasting with the relatively lower enjoyment reported for the MI-only condition.
Conditions combining English instruction with other subjects, particularly PE + MI + ENG and ENG + MI, showed the most substantial positive impacts in post-session quiz scores. This finding aligns with the growing body of literature on multimodal learning and integrated curricula, which posits that engaging multiple sensory and cognitive pathways can enhance learning outcomes (Faella et al., 2025). Beyond traditional cognitive benefits such as increased neurotrophic factors and improved synaptic plasticity associated with exercise (Vaynman et al., 2003), these outcomes may be partly explained by principles of embodied cognition (Atkinson, 2010). In this context, PE may provide sensorimotor input, potentially grounding abstract linguistic concepts in concrete bodily experiences (Mazzuca et al., 2021). For instance, learning action verbs while performing the actions can create stronger, more accessible neural representations (Bedny and Caramazza, 2011). Similarly, MI, while a covert process, involves the mental simulation of actions (Decety, 1996), activating neural networks similar to those engaged during actual movement (Sharma and Baron, 2013). This fact could contribute to a more embodied understanding of language, particularly if the language content relates to actions, movements, or spatial relationships (Willems and Casasanto, 2011). However, the results may also be related to specific curriculum designs, and future research is needed to understand how pedagogical approaches and curriculum design may interact.
Regarding passing accuracy, the results showed a significant overall improvement from pre- to post-session, a significant effect of class condition, and an interaction between time and class. This interaction highlights that while most class conditions led to increases in passing accuracy, the magnitude of improvement varied. The English-only condition did not yield a significant change in passing accuracy, which is justified by the absence of any instruction or motor skill engagement. In contrast, conditions incorporating PE and MI, particularly the MI + PE and PE + MI + ENG combinations, exhibited the most substantial improvements. These findings are consistent with research showing the efficacy of skill training practice in basketball motor skill acquisition (Fikri et al., 2023). The improvements seen in classes incorporating MI are also consistent with a systematic review which revealed positive impacts on developing sport-specific motor skills (Lindsay et al., 2023). Studies have showed that mental practice can lead to performance improvements comparable to physical practice, especially when combined as observed in a review about the topic (Behrendt et al., 2021). This dual-modality approach may enhance learning by engaging both direct sensorimotor pathways and cognitive simulation (Guillot et al., 2024). This is particularly relevant for adolescents, who may perceive MI as abstract unless it is supported by concrete scaffolding, highlighting the importance of integrating tangible or guided experiences to make MI more accessible and effective.
Consistent with the pattern observed in the other measures, conditions incorporating multiple approaches, particularly PE + MI + ENG and MI + PE, garnered the highest enjoyment ratings. Conversely, the MI-only condition was associated with significantly lower enjoyment compared to all other groups. These findings align with the need for increasing intrinsic motivation and engagement in educational and physical activity contexts (Adank et al., 2024). PE, by its very nature, often involves dynamic, interactive, and varied movements namely in those using games, which can be inherently enjoyable (Mo et al., 2024). Similarly, multimodal learning approaches, which integrate various sensory inputs and activity types, are generally found to increase student engagement and motivation (Xiao et al., 2022). The lower enjoyment in the MI-only condition, despite its showed benefits, might be attributed to its less overtly interactive nature. While mentally stimulating, the lack of physical exertion, social interaction, and external stimuli typically present in PE or combined classes could lead to a diminished sense of enjoyment for some participants. This highlights a potential trade-off between the focused, internal cognitive work of MI and the broader, more intrinsically rewarding experience offered by physically active and multimodal learning environments.
Despite these findings, the present study has several limitations. One limitation is the absence of a washout period, which may have inflated cumulative learning effects. Future research should incorporate washout periods to minimize the risk of carryover effects and ensure that comparisons between conditions are not compromised. Another limitation is that including all seven conditions, which cover every possible combination of PE, MI, and ENG, made it difficult to implement all 5,040 possible randomization orders. Consequently, only three sequences were used, which do not cover all possible permutations. This may introduce some limitations related to the specific sequences implemented; however, this approach is justified by the practical constraints of conducting the study within a real educational setting. Future research should also incorporate longitudinal designs (e.g., 6 weeks with retention tests) to investigate the sustained impact of these multimodal approaches, as the current study reflects only short-term skill development across sessions. Moreover, a non-crossover control group (following the standard curriculum) could be included in future research. Additionally, mediation analyses (e.g., examining attention or motivation) should be included to explore potential driving effects in future studies. Additionally, the study relied on self-reported enjoyment, which could be subject to social desirability bias. Due to the nature of the study design, it is important to recognize that the novelty of the methods may influence enjoyment. Only in longitudinal studies would it be possible to mitigate such potential bias. Future studies could also delve deeper into the specific mechanisms underlying the observed enjoyment differences, perhaps through qualitative methods to capture participants’ subjective experiences. Furthermore, concurrent activation of motor (PE) and semantic (MI) networks may enhance vocabulary retention, but neurophysiological validation (e.g., electroencephalography or functional near-infrared spectroscopy) is warranted in future studies. Additionally, future research should examine the effects on abstract vocabulary, not just action vocabulary, as well as testing the effects in different age groups.
Practically, these findings suggest that educators and coaches should consider integrating diverse modalities into their teaching practices to enhance student engagement and enjoyment. Incorporating brief physical activity breaks or guided MI sessions into traditional language classes could not only boost learning outcomes but also foster a more positive and motivating learning environment. For example, educators might implement PE-based games (e.g., ‘Simon Says’ for action verbs) and scripted MI audio guides (approximately 5 min in duration) to facilitate feasible classroom integration.
5 Conclusion
In conclusion, this provides evidence that integrating diverse disciplines—English instruction, MI, and PE—can significantly influence language retention (English quiz scores) and motor (passing accuracy) learning outcomes, as well as affective responses (enjoyment). The superior performance and higher enjoyment reported in combined conditions suggest the potential synergistic benefits of multimodal learning approaches. These findings advocate for a more holistic and integrated approach to education and skill development, suggesting that combining traditional academic instruction with physical and mental training can lead to more effective, engaging, and enjoyable learning experiences. Future research should build upon these immediate findings to explore long-term retention and the generalizability of these benefits across different learning contexts and populations.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by The study design was approved by the ethics committee of Sichuan Normal University (approval code: 2025LS0058), in accordance with ethical standards for research involving youth populations. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.
Author contributions
YW: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. XD: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LQ: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AL: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. HZ: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JH: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Adank, A. M., Van Kann, D. H., Borghouts, L. B., Kremers, S. P., and Vos, S. B. (2024). That’s what I like! Fostering enjoyment in primary physical education. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 30, 283–301. doi: 10.1177/1356336X231205686
Anwar, M. N., Tomi, N., and Ito, K. (2011). Motor imagery facilitates force field learning. Brain Res. 1395, 21–29. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.04.030
Atkinson, D. (2010). Extended, embodied cognition and second language acquisition. Appl. Linguist. 31, 599–622. doi: 10.1093/applin/amq009
Bashta, T., and Zalutskaya, O. (2021). “Pedagogical project of integrated classes physical education and English,” in Modern pedagogical technologies in foreign language education: Trends, transformations, vectors of development. eds. E. Uteubayeva and M. Petrova (Veliko Tarnovo, Bulgaria: ACCESS Press Publishing House), 33–38.
Bayram, M., Palluel-Germain, R., Lebon, F., Durand, E., Harquel, S., and Perrone-Bertolotti, M. (2023). Motor imagery training to improve language processing: what are the arguments? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:982849. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.982849
Bedny, M., and Caramazza, A. (2011). Perception, action, and word meanings in the human brain: the case from action verbs. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1224, 81–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06013.x
Behrendt, F., Zumbrunnen, V., Brem, L., Suica, Z., Gäumann, S., Ziller, C., et al. (2021). Effect of motor imagery training on motor learning in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:9467. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18189467
Bonnet, C., Bayram, M., El Bouzaïdi Tiali, S., Lebon, F., Harquel, S., Palluel-Germain, R., et al. (2022). Kinesthetic motor-imagery training improves performance on lexical-semantic access. PLoS One 17:e0270352. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0270352
Botes, E., Dewaele, J. M., and Greiff, S. (2021). The development and validation of the short form of the foreign language enjoyment scale. Mod. Lang. J. 105, 858–876. doi: 10.1111/modl.12741
Coral, J., and Lleixà, T. (2016). Physical education in content and language integrated learning: successful interaction between physical education and English as a foreign language. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 19, 108–126. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2014.977766
Decety, J. (1996). The neurophysiological basis of motor imagery. Behav. Brain Res. 77, 45–52. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00225-1
Faella, P., Digennaro, S., and Iannaccone, A. (2025). Educational practices in motion: a scoping review of embodied learning approaches in school. Front. Educ. 10:1568744. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2025.1568744
Fikri, F. B. R., Sofyan, D., and Yudha, B. A. R. (2023). Analysis of basketball passing basic movement skills in extracurricular students. J. Metrics Stud. Soc. Sci. 2, 10–16.
Forey, G., and Cheung, L. M. E. (2019). The benefits of explicit teaching of language for curriculum learning in the physical education classroom. Engl. Specif. Purp. 54, 91–109. doi: 10.1016/j.esp.2019.01.001
Gonzalez Mujico, F., and Lasagabaster, D. (2019). Enhancing L2 motivation and English proficiency through technology. Complut. J. Engl. Stud. 27, 59–78. doi: 10.5209/cjes.62990
Guillot, A., Rienzo, F. D., Frank, C., Debarnot, U., and MacIntyre, T. (2024). From simulation to motor execution: a review of the impact of dynamic motor imagery on performance. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 17, 420–439. doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2021.2007539
Ingram, T. G. J., Kraeutner, S. N., Solomon, J. P., Westwood, D. A., and Boe, S. G. (2016). Skill acquisition via motor imagery relies on both motor and perceptual learning. Behav. Neurosci. 130, 252–260. doi: 10.1037/bne0000126
Leitan, N. D., and Chaffey, L. (2014). Embodied cognition and its applications: a brief review. Sensoria 10:3. doi: 10.7790/sa.v10i1.384
Lindsay, R. S., Larkin, P., Kittel, A., and Spittle, M. (2023). Mental imagery training programs for developing sport-specific motor skills: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Phys Educ Sport Pedagogy 28, 444–465. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2021.1991297
Mazzuca, C., Fini, C., Michalland, A. H., Falcinelli, I., Da Rold, F., Tummolini, L., et al. (2021). From affordances to abstract words: the flexibility of sensorimotor grounding. Brain Sci. 11:1304. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11101304
Mir, K. J., Fatima, S. A., and Fatima, S. T. (2023). Impact of dual coding strategy to enhance students’ retention of scientific concepts in middle schools. Ann. Hum. Soc. Sci. 4:63. doi: 10.35484/ahss.2023(4-IV)63
Mo, W., Saibon, J. B., Li, Y., Li, J., and He, Y. (2024). Effects of game-based physical education program on enjoyment in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Public Health 24:517. doi: 10.1186/s12889-024-18043-6
Munzert, J., Lorey, B., and Zentgraf, K. (2009). Cognitive motor processes: the role of motor imagery in the study of motor representations. Brain Res. Rev. 60, 306–326. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.12.024
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual-coding approach. New York: Oxford, University Press.
Shapiro, L. (2007). The embodied cognition research Programme. Philos Compass 2, 338–346. doi: 10.1111/j.1747-9991.2007.00064.x
Sharma, N., and Baron, J.-C. (2013). Does motor imagery share neural networks with executed movement: a multivariate fMRI analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:564. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00564
Vaynman, S., Ying, Z., and Gomez-Pinilla, F. (2003). Interplay between brain-derived neurotrophic factor and signal transduction modulators in the regulation of the effects of exercise on synaptic-plasticity. Neuroscience 122, 647–657. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.08.001
Wei, L., Zheng, Y., Li, M., and Dai, S. H. (2025). Testing the effects of player matching in basketball matches and small-sided game training scenarios based on the relative age effect: a 6-month study on physical performance and skill adaptations. J. Sports Sci. Med. 24, 397–405. doi: 10.52082/jssm.2025.397
Willems, R. M., and Casasanto, D. (2011). Flexibility in embodied language understanding. Front. Psychol. 2:116. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00116
Xiao, J., Lin, T. H., and Sun-Lin, H. Z. (2022). The role of different levels of multichannel multimodal learning experience delivery in student engagement. Educ. Inf. Technol. 27, 2939–2954. doi: 10.1007/s10639-021-10731-9
Yamaki, A., and Iwata, S. (2020). Integration of theory and practical sessions to improve health and physical education classes. Ann. Educ. Res. 47, 27–34.
Keywords: physical exercise, second language learning, skill development, sports education, youth
Citation: Wu Y, Dang X, Qiao L, Li A, Zhang H and He J (2025) Enhancing English language acquisition through physical education and motor imagery: assessing enjoyment, English retention, and skill accuracy in adolescent learners. Front. Psychol. 16:1672418. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1672418
Edited by:
André Ramalho, Polytechnic Institute of Castelo Branco, PortugalReviewed by:
Hilmy Aliriad, Nahdlatul Ulama Sunan Giri University, IndonesiaAhmed Ghorbel, University of Jendouba, Tunisia
Copyright © 2025 Wu, Dang, Qiao, Li, Zhang and He. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Hongbo Zhang, MTA1Mzc5MjNAcXEuY29t