ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Sports Act. Living, 14 April 2025

Sec. Sport Psychology

Volume 7 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fspor.2025.1586484

This article is part of the Research TopicMotivations For Physical Activity - Volume IIIView all 18 articles

High-groove music boosts self-selected running speed and positive mood in female university students

  • 1Faculty of Health and Sport Sciences, Ryutsu Keizai University, Ibaraki, Japan
  • 2Faculty of Psychology, Kobe Gakuin University, Hyogo, Japan

Introduction: Approximately 27.5% of adults worldwide fail to meet the recommended 150 min per week of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity. Music is a powerful tool that enhances positive affective responses and exercise adherence. However, little is known regarding which types of music are most effective in enhancing these benefits. Groove, which induces a sensation of “wanting to move to the music” and is associated with positive affective responses, may help make self-paced exercise more active and pleasant. Therefore, in this study, we examined the effects of high-groove (HG) music on self-paced exercise and mood responses.

Methods: Thirty-eight university students (18 males) participated in this randomized crossover study under two experimental conditions. The participants ran on a treadmill for 10 min at a self-selected comfortable speed while listening to HG and low-groove (LG) music playlists.

Results: The HG playlist received higher groove ratings than the LG playlist. Self-selected running speed and positive mood responses (vitality and arousal) were greater under the HG condition only in women. Exercise intensity measured using heart rate and the rate of perceived exertion was comparable between the conditions. The groove ratings for the HG playlist positively correlated with speed (HG-LG) and vitality changes under the HG condition (post-pre).

Conclusion: These results suggest that HG music promotes a positive mood response and exercise adherence, particularly in female students. Moreover, the observed sex difference suggests that individual differences in music perception may also influence exercise behavior. Focusing on groove, a musical characteristic distinct from tempo and volume, our study provides a more comprehensive understanding of music that is compatible with exercise, while also examining potential sex differences in its effects on self-paced exercise and mood responses. These findings contribute to health promotion by encouraging active living via physical exercise.

1 Introduction

The WHO guidelines for physical activity and sedentary behavior state that physical activity has a positive effect on mental and cognitive health across the human life span and recommend 150 min of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity per week (1). Nonetheless, approximately 27.5% of adults worldwide do not meet this recommendation (2). Previous studies regarding the promotion of exercise adherence have indicated that affective response to physical exercise has been identified as a key factor for enhancing motivation towards exercise and physical activity (37). A systematic review of actual surveys concluded that positive affective responses during and after exercise reliably correlated with affective judgments on future physical activity (8). In addition to exercise adherence, positive mood responses to exercise can predict the effects of exercise on mental health (9, 10).

For a more sustainable exercise experience, individualized self-paced exercise prescriptions based on emotional state, rather than at a specific prescribed intensity, have been proposed (11). In affect-based, self-paced exercise, exercise intensity is self-regulated to “feel good” based on affective responses (11, 12). Affect-based exercise not only enhances affective responses but also improves exercise adherence— measured by exercise behavior— particularly in individuals with low physical activity levels and cardiorespiratory fitness (1315). In addition to the benefits of exercise adherence, exercise training at an affect-regulated intensity also improves aerobic fitness (16). These exercise prescription strategies are also advantageous from the perspective of self-determination theory (17).

Music is one of the most powerful and compatible environmental factors for improving exercise performance and positive mood responses (18, 19). Few studies in the health-promotion literature have examined the effect of music on self-paced exercise at the submaximal level. Edworthy and Waring (20) reported that fast, loud music enhanced exercise intensity and positive mood during treadmill running (20). Hutchinson et al. (21) showed that exercise with self-selected music promoted a greater exercise intensity during 20 min of affect-regulated running, exercise compared to without music while maintaining a “good” feeling (21). Almeida et al. (22) also indicated that fast tempo (140 beats per minute [bpm) music enhances self-selected walking pace in women (22). Although these studies have shown that listening to music increases exercise intensity and positive mood in self-paced exercise, it is unclear whether any musical characteristics other than tempo and volume are effective in enhancing positive affect and exercise behavior.

The groove sensation may be a promising musical feature that enhances affect and exercise behavior (23, 24). A common feature of groove across cultures is “the pleasurable sensation of wanting to move one's body to music” (2527). Groovy music evokes arousal and pleasant feelings (27, 28). In addition to positive mood responses, groove influences movement. Groove not only affects dance-like movements (29) but also facilitates repetitive and cyclic movements, such as gait and cycling performance. High-groove (HG) music facilitates faster and less variable movements than those performed with low-groove (LG) music (30, 31). Rhythmic entrainment, a fundamental mechanism by which listeners' bodily rhythms adapt to the periodicities of music, may underlie the mechanisms through which groove facilitates movement and elicits a positive mood (32, 33). The coactivation of emotional and motivational brain systems, including the dopaminergic reward pathway, along with the activation of several other areas outside the emotional system, including motor-, attention-, and memory-related regions, is thought to underlie this neural basis (34, 35).

Individual experiences towards groove music are influenced by listener characteristics such as music familiarity (26, 36, 37). Particularly, sex-based differences are known. The tempo at which groove is easily experienced is faster in men than in women (38). In addition, women prefer pop music more (39) and can better identify emotional expressions in music than men (40, 41). These findings suggest sex differences exist in groove sensation, even while listening to the same music.

Taken together, the present study aimed to clarify the effects of groove on self-selected exercise speed and affective response in men and women. We hypothesized that higher groove music would increase self-selected running pace and positive mood compared to lower groove music with a compatible tempo. Furthermore, given that women are generally more sensitive to musical stimuli than men, we anticipated that the observed effects would be more pronounced in women. Our study extends previous research by focusing on groove, a musical characteristic distinct from tempo and volume, while also examining potential sex differences in its effects on self-paced exercise and mood responses.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

A priori power analysis was performed using G-power software (42). An optimal total sample size of N = 20 for each sex was calculated, with a partial η2 effect size of 0.17, power of 0.8, and alpha of 0.05, based on our previous acute exercise intervention studies (43, 44).

Forty healthy university students (aged 18–21 years; 20 male participants) from the Faculty of Health and Sports Sciences of Ryutsu Keizai University participated. Those majoring in sports and health sciences were likely more engaged in exercise and had higher physical activity levels than students in non-sports-related fields. Two male participants were excluded because of unaccomplished experiments or missing data, resulting in data from only 38 students (18 male participants) being collected and analyzed. Participants' demographic characteristics are presented in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant demographic.

Written informed consent was obtained from all participants before starting the experiment. Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the Ethics Committee of Ryutsu Keizai University (approval number: 45). This study complied with the ethical requirements of the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.2 Procedures

The two experimental conditions, HG and LG conditions, were implemented using a within-subject crossover design in a randomized order. An outline of the experimental procedures is shown in Figure 1. At least 10 min after arrival, the participants performed the first exercise session. The second exercise session was conducted after a minimum of 15 min of seating rest, when the heart rate was confirmed to decrease to resting levels.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Outline of the experimental procedures. RPE, rating of perceived exertion; HG, high-groove; LG, low-groove

As part of the exercise session, the participants ran on the treadmill (PH-OST-2705, Life Fitness) for 10 min, including a 5 min warm-up. The participants were instructed to run, but not walk, for a total of 10 min at a speed that felt comfortable; they were allowed to adjust the treadmill speed by themselves at the beginning and 5 min after the start for 30 s each time. The slope of the treadmill was set to zero. The treadmill speed display was masked from the participants' viewpoint to minimize the impact of cognitive biases. Participants who had never run on a treadmill were allowed to run for 1–2 min before the experiment for familiarization. The first exercise session was then performed after confirming that the heart rate had returned to resting levels following a seated rest. The treadmill speed and heart rate (HR) were recorded continuously during the sessions. HR was measured using a wireless chest strap telemetry system (WHS-1, Union Tool Co., Tokyo, Japan). The rate of perceived exertion [RPE; (45)] was measured immediately after exercise.

2.3 Auditory stimuli

We used commercially available music to provide a natural listening experience for the participants (26). The music selection process began with groove ratings for a pool of music tracks, from which a balanced set of high- and low-groove tracks was selected based on these ratings. Although various acoustic features contribute to the perception of groove (27), in this study, we relied on subjective groove evaluations for music selection. First, we selected music that is familiar to Japanese university students since familiarity with a song is known to affect groove (46). Altogether 138 music tracks from a variety of genres (pop, rock, hip-hop, jazz, etc.) were originally listed based on input from lab members from the same faculty as the experimental participants. Nine lab members (six women) rated the groove ratings after listening to music pieces and ranked them. Subsequently, three tracks were selected from the top and bottom of the list to create the HG and LG playlists (Table 2). To minimize the impact of a specific tempo on groove (47) and entrainment (48), music with a variety of tempos (slow, medium, and fast) was included in the playlists. Furthermore, the tempo of the music (bpm) was counterbalanced to be comparable across lists to control the effect of tempo on groove. The bpm were obtained from https://tunebat.com.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Musical playlist.

Music was played through loudspeakers, with the volume adjusted to be consistent across all tracks, ensuring it was neither too loud nor too quiet, so as to avoid interference with the sound of the ergometer. The order of the tracks to be listened to in the experiments was randomized across the participants.

2.4 Groove rating

We used the Japanese Version of the Experience of Groove Questionnaire (EGQ-JA) to measure the groove experience for each HG and LG music playlists (49, 50). The EGQ-JA consists of six items and evaluates the groove experience using two scales: the urge to move and pleasure. The participants were asked to indicate how they felt when they listened to a piece of music based on a seven-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 = “strongly disagree” to 6 = “strongly agree”. The total score for the six items was used as the groove rating.

The participants listened to the music tracks again after the exercise sessions and rated each track. The average rating of the three tracks was used as the groove rating for the HG and LG playlists.

2.5 Mood scale

The psychological mood state was measured using the Two-Dimensional Mood Scale (TDMS) before and after the exercise session (51). The TDMS is a psychometric scale comprising eight mood-expressing words that describe both pleasurable and arousal states (energetic, lively, lethargic, listless, relaxed, calm, irritated, and nervous). The participants were asked to indicate how they were feeling at the time according to a six-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 = 'not at all' to 5 = 'extremely'. Vitality level, which represents low arousal–displeasure to high arousal–pleasure (–10 to +10 points), and stability level, which represents high arousal–displeasure to low arousal–pleasure (–10 to +10 points), were measured. Based on these scores, pleasure (vitality + stability: −20 to +20 points) and arousal (vitality – stability: −20 to +20 points) levels were calculated.

2.6 Statistical analysis

First, we tested the significance of the differences according to sex and groove experience while listening to the HG and LG playlists. We performed a repeated-measures two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) of groove ratings by sex and condition, followed by a Bonferroni's post hoc test.

Second, we examined the effects of groove on running speed and exercise intensity (HR, RPE) with a paired t-test and mood changes with a repeated measures two-way ANOVA. To account for cases wherein groove ratings differed between men and women, an analysis was performed for each sex.

Finally, when there were significant differences in exercise intensity and mood changes between conditions, we examined their relationship with groove experiences.

3 Results

3.1 Groove ratings

The groove ratings measured using the EGQ-JP are presented in Table 3. A repeated-measures two-way ANOVA by sex (male/female) × music condition (HG/LG) revealed a significant interaction [F(1, 36) = 4.75, p = 0.04, partial η2 = 0.12]. A series of post hoc comparisons using a Bonferroni test revealed that groove ratings for the HG playlist were significantly greater than those for the LG playlist in both men [F(1, 17) = 36.25, p < 0.001, d = 1.80] and women [F(1, 19) = 90.60, p < 0.001, d = 2.82]. However, the groove ratings for the HG playlist were significantly higher in women than in men [F(1, 36) = 10.37, p = 0.003, d = 1.01]. The results indicating higher groove ratings for HG than those for LG for both men and women confirmed the validity of each playlist. In addition, the groove ratings for the HG playlist in women indicated that women were more appreciative of HG.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The results of groove ratings, running speed, and exercise intensity.

3.2 Running speed and exercise intensity

The results of running speed and exercise intensity (HR and RPE) are presented in Table 3. A paired t-test revealed that running speed (6–10 min) was faster when listening to HG music than LG music in women [t(19) = 2.09, p = 0.04997, d = 0.21]; however, no significant differences were observed in men [t(17) = 0.64, p = 0.53, d = 0.06]. In contrast, running speed at warm-up, RPE, and HR did not differ between the conditions in either men or women (Table 3).

3.3 Mood changes

The psychological mood states (vitality, stability, arousal, and pleasure) measured using the TDMS are shown in Figure 2. In women, a repeated measures two-way ANOVA with experimental condition (LG/HG) and time (pre/post) revealed a significant interaction for vitality [F(1, 19) = 4.69, p = 0.04, partial η2 = 0.20] and arousal levels [F(1, 19) = 6.02, p = 0.02, partial η2 = 0.24]. A post hoc comparison using a Bonferroni test revealed that the vitality [F(1, 19) = 14.44, p = 0.001, d = 1.67] and arousal levels [F(1, 19) = 11.51, p = 0.003, d = 1.72] in the post-exercise session for the HG condition were greater than those in the LG condition. There were no interactions for stability levels [F(1, 19) = 2.42, p = 0.14, partial η2 = 0.11] and pleasure levels [F(1, 19) = 1.19, p = 0.29, partial η2 = 0.06].

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Psychological mood state (vitality, stability, pleasure, arousal) for high- and low-groove conditions in pre- and post-exercise sessions in men and women. Post-exercise vitality and arousal levels were significantly higher under the high-groove condition than under the low-groove condition. * indicates p < 0.01 versus low-groove condition after Bonferroni correction.

In men, there were no significant interactions for validity [F(1, 17) = 2.85, p = 0.11, partial η2 = 0.11], stability [F(1, 17) = 0.32, p = 0.58, partial η2 = 0.02], pleasure [F(1, 17) = 1.67, p = 0.21, partial η2 = 0.09], and arousal [F(1, 17) = 2.72, p = 0.12, partial η2 = 0.14].

3.4 Relationship of groove rating to running speed and mood changes

The results of the correlation analyses are shown in Figure 3. In women, the Pearson correlation analysis revealed positive correlations between groove rating in the HG and running speed (HG-LG) (r = 0.46, p = 0.04) and vitality (post-pre) (r = 0.47, p = 0.04). There was no significant correlation between the groove rating and arousal (r = 0.24, p = 0.30). In men, there were no significant correlations between groove ratings and running speed or mood changes.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Relationships between groove rating for the high-groove playlist and (A) speed change (HG–LG), (B) vitality change (post–pre), (C) arousal change (post–pre), separately for men and women. Positive correlations were found between groove ratings and both faster running speed and increased vitality in women. r represents Pearson's product-moment correlation coefficient. * indicates p < 0.05.

4 Discussion

This study aimed to clarify the effects of HG music on self-selected comfortable running pace and mood responses in male and female university students. The findings in this study revealed that running while listening to HG music induced a higher running speed and more positive mood than that induced by LG music in female students. Furthermore, among female students, the enhanced running speed and vitality levels in the HG condition positively correlated with individual groove ratings for HG music tracks. These findings support our hypothesis, at least in female students, that HG music can make self-paced exercise more active and pleasant and suggest that individual differences in groove experiences may moderate these effects.

Among female students, HG music accelerated running speed and improved positive mood compared to LG music, and these effects were greater in those with higher groove ratings for HG music. These results indicate that groove is involved in the effects of music on running speed and mood, in addition to the previously identified factors such as tempo and volume of music (20, 22). Listening to HG music elicits a faster gait in short-distance measurements (30, 52). The results of this study show that similar effects can be obtained for continuous running. Furthermore, the increased positive mood with HG music is consistent with findings from previous studies which showed that pedaling while listening to HG drum breaks had positive effects on participants' psychological states, including “excited” and “having fun” (53).

Despite the increased running speed in women, exercise intensity (as measured by HR and RPE) remained unchanged. The potential effect of increased exercise intensity due to a higher running speed may have been cancelled out by the effects of HG music on HR and RPE. A previous study has shown that listening to music during exercise can reduce RPE (18). In addition, some reports have indicated that music reduces HR (54, 55). This suggests that, although increased running speed in women may have led to an increase in exercise intensity, the accompanying effects of music on reducing HR and RPE could have counteracted this increase, resulting in no overall change in the perceived exercise intensity.

In the present study, HG music did not affect running speed or mood responses in male participants. There may be two possible explanations for this finding. First, women may be more sensitive to groove than men, and the HG playlist may not have elicited sufficient groove sensations in men. Previous studies suggest that women are more sensitive to the emotional aspects of music than men (40, 41). Additionally, women's affective responses (happy-sad) to music are more influenced by tempo and mode compared to men (56). Since groove is associated with pleasant feelings, women are thought to be more sensitive to groove sensation. Our results, showing that women rated the HG playlist higher than men, support this idea. The biological and neuroscientific mechanisms underlying sex differences remain largely unexplored, thus many aspects are still unclear (57). Second, the contribution of groove as a determinant of running speed and mood responses was less among men. The results of the correlation analysis confirmed that groove rating did not predict the effect on speed and mood in men. Other determinants such as tempo and volume may have a greater impact on men.

This study has some limitations. First, the effect of the synchronization of music and movement is unclear. Since this study did not focus on whether the running steps were synchronized with the music tempo, the running pitch was not recorded. However, the music used in this study ranged between 81 and 159 bpm, whereas the typical running pitch is approximately within 160–200 bpm, suggesting that the music tempo and movement were not synchronized. Music synchronized with movement patterns may further enhance the effect (58). This is a topic for future study. Second, since the participants comprised university students who belonged to the Faculty of Health and Sports Sciences, many were likely familiar with exercise. Additional verification is needed, targeting those with no or fewer exercise habits to promote physically active lifestyles. Third, this study revealed sex differences in the effects of groove on exercise. A more detailed investigation is necessary to determine the factors leading to these individual differences. Applying these findings in practice, considering sex differences is essential to optimize the use of groove music in exercise programs. Fourth, individual differences, including physiological and musical background, as well as music perception, may have influenced the results. Addressing these potential confounding factors in future research could contribute to the development of personalized exercise interventions that account for individual variability. Fifth, the neural basis underlying the observed effects remains unclear. Future studies utilizing neuroimaging methods could provide new insights into the mechanisms through which groove influences exercise performance, as the dopamine reward system and other pathways are likely involved (34, 35).

In conclusion, the present study demonstrated that HG music increased self-selected running speed and positive mood responses compared to LG music, and these effects were greater in female students with higher groove ratings for HG music. These results suggest that HG music promotes a positive mood and exercise adherence, contributing to health promotion through a physically active lifestyle. Coaches and fitness instructors can leverage these benefits by incorporating HG music into exercise programs to enhance motivation and performance. Additionally, integrating HG music into rehabilitation and health promotion programs may help sustain long-term exercise participation.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by The Ethics Committee of Ryutsu Keizai University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

KS: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SK: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported in part by Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) grants 22K17808 (KS), 25K14846 (KS), and 23K25671 (SK).

Acknowledgments

We thank the members of the Laboratory of Gymnastics and Physical Fitness, Faculty of Health and Sport Sciences, Ryutsu Keizai University, for their assistance in data collection.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that this study was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as potential conflicts of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. World Health Organization. WHO Guidelines on Physical Activity and Sedentary Behaviour. Geneva: World Health Organization (2020).

Google Scholar

2. Guthold R, Stevens GA, Riley LM, Bull FC. Worldwide trends in insufficient physical activity from 2001 to 2016: a pooled analysis of 358 population-based surveys with 1.9 million participants. Lancet Glob Heal. (2018) 6:e1077–86. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(18)30357-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Lee HH, Emerson JA, Williams DM. The exercise-affect-adherence pathway: an evolutionary perspective. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01285

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Murphy SL, Eaves DL. Exercising for the pleasure and for the pain of it: the implications of different forms of hedonistic thinking in theories of physical activity behavior. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:1–3. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00843

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Ekkekakis P. People have feelings! exercise psychology in paradigmatic transition. Curr Opin Psychol. (2017) 16:84–8. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Stevens CJ, Baldwin AS, Bryan AD, Conner M, Rhodes RE, Williams DM. Affective determinants of physical activity: a conceptual framework and narrative review. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:568331. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.568331

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Stults-Kolehmainen MA, Blacutt M, Bartholomew JB, Gilson TA, Ash GI, McKee PC, et al. Motivation states for physical activity and sedentary behavior: desire, urge, wanting, and craving. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:1–17. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.568390

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Rhodes RE, Kates A. Can the affective response to exercise predict future motives and physical activity behavior? A systematic review of published evidence. Ann Behav Med. (2015) 49:715–31. doi: 10.1007/s12160-015-9704-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Nouchi R, Nouchi H, Kawashima R. A single 30 minutes bout of combination physical exercises improved inhibition and vigor-mood in middle-aged and older females: evidence from a randomized controlled trial. Front Aging Neurosci. (2020) 12:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00179

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Suwabe K, Hyodo K, Fukuie T, Ochi G, Inagaki K, Sakairi Y, et al. Positive mood while exercising influences beneficial effects of exercise with music on prefrontal executive function: a functional NIRS study. Neuroscience. (2021) 454:61–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.06.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Williams DM. Exercise, affect, and adherence: an integrated model and a case for self-paced exercise. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2008) 30:471–96. doi: 10.1038/jid.2014.371

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Ladwig MA, Hartman ME, Ekkekakis P. Affect-based exercise prescription. ACSMS Health Fit J. (2017) 21:10–5. doi: 10.1249/FIT.0000000000000332

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Baldwin AS, Kangas JL, Denman DC, Smits JAJ, Yamada T, Otto MW. Cardiorespiratory fitness moderates the effect of an affect-guided physical activity prescription: a pilot randomized controlled trial. Cogn Behav Ther. (2016) 45:445–57. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2016.1194454

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Williams DM, Dunsiger S, Emerson JA, Gwaltney CJ, Monti PM, Miranda R. Self-paced exercise, affective response, and exercise adherence: a preliminary investigation using ecological momentary assessment. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2016) 38:282–91. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2015-0232

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Teixeira DS, Bastos V, Andrade AJ, Palmeira AL, Ekkekakis P. Individualized pleasure-oriented exercise sessions, exercise frequency, and affective outcomes: a pragmatic randomized controlled trial. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2024) 21:1–19. doi: 10.1186/s12966-024-01636-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Parfitt G, Alrumh A, Rowlands AV. Affect-regulated exercise intensity: does training at an intensity that feels “good” improve physical health? J Sci Med Sport. (2012) 15:548–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2012.01.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am Psychol. (2000) 55:68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Terry PC, Karageorghis CI, Curran ML, Martin OV, Parsons-Smith RL. Effects of music in exercise and sport: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. (2020) 146:91–117. doi: 10.1037/bul0000216

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Jones L, Zenko Z. A systematic narrative review of extrinsic strategies to improve affective responses to exercise. Front Sport Act Living. (2023) 5:1186986. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2023.1186986

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Edworthy J, Waring H. The effects of music tempo and loudness level on treadmill exercise. Ergonomics. (2006) 49:1597–610. doi: 10.1080/00140130600899104

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hutchinson JC, Jones L, Vitti SN, Moore A, Dalton PC, O’Neil BJ. The influence of self-selected music on affect-regulated exercise intensity and remembered pleasure during treadmill running. Sport Exerc Perform Psychol. (2018) 7:80–92. doi: 10.1037/spy0000115

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Almeida FAM, Nunes RFH, Ferreira SS, Krinski K, Elsangedy HM, Buzzachera CF, et al. Effects of musical tempo on physiological, affective, and perceptual variables and performance of self-selected walking pace. J Phys Ther Sci. (2015) 27:1709–12. doi: 10.1589/jpts.27.1709

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Witek M, Clarke EF, Wallentin M, Kringelbach ML, Vuust P. Syncopation, body-movement and pleasure in groove music. PLoS One. (2014) 9:e94446. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094446

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kawase S. Is happier music groovier? The influence of emotional characteristics of musical chord progressions on groove. Psychol Res. (2024) 88:438–48. doi: 10.1007/s00426-023-01869-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Madison G. Experiencing groove induced by music: consistency and phenomenology. Music Percept. (2006) 24:201–8. doi: 10.1525/mp.2006.24.2.201

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Janata P, Tomic ST, Haberman JM. Sensorimotor coupling in music and the psychology of the groove. J Exp Psychol Gen. (2012) 141:54–75. doi: 10.1037/a0024208

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Etani T, Miura A, Kawase S, Fujii S, Keller PE, Vuust P, et al. A review of psychological and neuroscientific research on musical groove. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2024) 158:105522. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105522

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Bowling DL, Ancochea PG, Hove MJ, Tecumseh Fitch W. Pupillometry of groove: evidence for noradrenergic arousal in the link between music and movement. Front Neurosci. (2019) 13:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.01039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Van Dyck E, Moelants D, Demey M, Deweppe A, Coussement P, Leman M. The impact of the bass drum on human dance movement. Music Percept. (2013) 30:349–59. doi: 10.1525/mp.2013.30.4.349

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Leow L-A, Parrott T, Grahn JA. Individual differences in beat perception affect gait responses to low- and high-groove music. Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8:811. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00811

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Chen J, Han J, Su P, Wang M, Shi W, Tang D. Effects of perceived groove in music on cycling performance and intermuscular coherence between trunk and lower limb muscles. J Sci Med Sport. (in press). doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2025.01.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Vuilleumier P, Trost W. Music and emotions: from enchantment to entrainment. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2015) 1337:212–22. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12676

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Trost WJ, Labbé C, Grandjean D. Rhythmic entrainment as a musical affect induction mechanism. Neuropsychologia. (2017) 96:96–110. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Gebauer L, Kringelbach ML, Vuust P. Ever-changing cycles of musical pleasure: the role of dopamine and anticipation. Psychomusicol Music Mind Brain. (2012) 22:152–67. doi: 10.1037/a0031126

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Matthews TE, Witek MAG, Lund T, Vuust P, Penhune VB. The sensation of groove engages motor and reward networks. Neuroimage. (2020) 214:116768. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116768

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Senn O, Kilchenmann L, Bechtold T, Hoesl F. Groove in drum patterns as a function of both rhythmic properties and listeners’ attitudes. PLoS One. (2018) 13(6):e0199604. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199604

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Senn O, Rose D, Bechtold T, Kilchenmann L, Hoesl F, Jerjen R, et al. Preliminaries to a psychological model of musical groove. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:1–5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01228

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Kawase S, Kashio I, Morizumi K, Matsumoto S. Individual differences in tempo of groovy music. Proceedings of 17th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition and the 7th Conference of the Asia-Pacific Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music: 2023 August 26; Tokyo, Japan. Tokyo, Japan: e-Proceedings (2023). p 484.

Google Scholar

39. Colley A. Young people’s musical taste: relationship with gender and gender-related traits. J Appl Soc Psychol. (2008) 38:2039–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00379.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Gabrielsson A, Juslin PN. Emotional expression in music performance: between the performer’s intention and the listener’s experience. Psychol Music. (1996) 24:68–91. doi: 10.1177/0305735696241007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Hunter PG, Glenn Schellenberg E, Stalinski SM. Liking and identifying emotionally expressive music: age and gender differences. J Exp Child Psychol. (2011) 110:80–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2007) 39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Suwabe K, Byun K, Hyodo K, Reagh ZM, Roberts JM, Matsushita A, et al. Rapid stimulation of human dentate gyrus function with acute mild exercise. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2018) 115:10487–92. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1805668115

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Suwabe K, Kuwamizu R, Hyodo K, Yoshikawa T, Otsuki T, Zempo-Miyaki A, et al. Improvement of mnemonic discrimination with acute light exercise is mediated by pupil-linked arousal in healthy older adults. Neurobiol Aging. (2024) 133:107–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2023.09.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Borg G. Borg’s Perceived Exertion and Pain Scales. Champaign: Human Kinetics (1998).

Google Scholar

46. Senn O, Bechtold TA, Hoesl F, Kilchenmann L. Taste and familiarity affect the experience of groove in popular music. Music Sci. (2021) 25:45–66. doi: 10.1177/1029864919839172

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Etani T, Marui A, Kawase S, Keller PE. Optimal tempo for groove: its relation to directions of body movement and Japanese nori. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:462. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00462

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Khalfa S, Roy M, Rainville P, Dalla Bella S, Peretz I. Role of tempo entrainment in psychophysiological differentiation of happy and sad music? Int J Psychophysiol. (2008) 68:17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.12.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Senn O, Bechtold T, Rose D, Câmara GS, Düvel N, Jerjen R, et al. Experience of groove questionnaire. Music Percept. (2020) 38:46–65. doi: 10.1525/mp.2020.38.1.46

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Kawase S, Okano M, Bechtold TA, Senn O. Japanese Version of the experience of groove questionnaire (EGQ-JA): translation and validation. Music Percept. (in press).

Google Scholar

51. Sakairi Y, Nakatsuka K, Shimizu T. Development of the two-dimensional mood scale for self-monitoring and self-regulation of momentary mood states. Jpn Psychol Res. (2013) 55:338–49. doi: 10.1111/jpr.12021

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Ready EA, Holmes JD, Grahn JA. Gait in younger and older adults during rhythmic auditory stimulation is influenced by groove, familiarity, beat perception, and synchronization demands. Hum Mov Sci. (2022) 84:102972. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2022.102972

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Fukuie T, Suwabe K, Kawase S, Shimizu T, Ochi G, Kuwamizu R, et al. Groove rhythm enhances exercise impact on prefrontal Cortex function in groove enjoyers. Neuroscience. (2023) 531:117–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2023.08.039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Copeland BL, Franks BD. Effects of types and intensities of background music on treadmill endurance. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. (1991) 31:100–3. Available online at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18614741861474

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

55. Moore AR, Hutchinson JC, Winter C, Dalton PC, Bolgla LA, Paolone VJ. Music alters heart rate and psychological responses but not muscle activation during light-intensity isometric exercise. Sport Med Health Sci. (2024) 6:394–401. doi: 10.1016/j.smhs.2024.01.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Webster GD, Weir CG. Emotional responses to music: interactive effects of mode, texture, and tempo. Motiv Emot. (2005) 29:19–39. doi: 10.1007/s11031-005-4414-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Koelsch S, Maess B, Grossmann T, Friederici AD. Electric brain responses reveal gender differences in music processing. Neuroreport. (2003) 14:709–13. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200304150-00010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Lim HBT, Karageorghis CI, Romer LM, Bishop DT. Psychophysiological effects of synchronous versus asynchronous music during cycling. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2014) 46:407–13. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3182a6378c

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: groove, physical exercise, exercise adherence, heart rate, affect, self-paced exercise, tempo, sex differences

Citation: Suwabe K and Kawase S (2025) High-groove music boosts self-selected running speed and positive mood in female university students. Front. Sports Act. Living 7:1586484. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2025.1586484

Received: 3 March 2025; Accepted: 25 March 2025;
Published: 14 April 2025.

Edited by:

Aleksandra Maria Rogowska, University of Opole, Poland

Reviewed by:

Lucas Murrins Marques, Santa Casa de Sao Paulo School of Medical Sciences, Brazil
Jebabli Nidhal, University of Jendouba, Tunisia

Copyright: © 2025 Suwabe and Kawase. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kazuya Suwabe, a3N1d2FiZUBya3UuYWMuanA=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.