Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 26 November 2012
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Examining the role of memory in social cognition View all 15 articles

Autobiographical memory and mentalizing impairment in personality disorders and schizophrenia: clinical and research implications

  • 1Center for Metacognitive Interpersonal Therapy, Rome, Italy
  • 2School for Cognitive Therapy “Istituto A.T. Beck,” Rome, Italy
  • 3Scuola di Psicoterapia “Studi Cognitivi,” Milan, Italy
  • 4Roudebush VA Medical Center, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA

Clinicians in the fields of mental health, neuroscientists, and social psychologists have been increasingly interested in how persons with psychiatric conditions experience a range of difficulties related to how they think about themselves and others. These difficulties include problems forming and retrieving the specific autobiographical memories (AM) that ground a sense of personal identity (McAdams, 2001). They have also involved diminishments in the capacity to engage in acts of mentalization or metacognition which allow for the understanding of mental states. Both of these forms of dysfunction are of increasing relevance given that each has been commonly observed across different psychiatric conditions, appears relatively independently from symptoms, and is uniquely linked to functional impairment (Dimaggio and Lysaker, 2010; Liu et al., in press).

To date deficits in AM and metacognition have mostly been analyzed separately. Psychopathology research has tended to explore either one or the other, thus neglecting the larger issue of how metacognition and AM are produced by systems which are likely closely related and likely feed and support one another (Markowitsch and Staniloiu, 2011). In development, AM and Theory of Mind (ToM) tend be influenced by common factors (Nelson and Fivush, 2004). Persons who present with overgeneralized AM also experience mentalizing problems including reduced affect awareness and poorer ToM (Palmieri et al., 2012).

Psychotherapy research and general neuroscience have begun to examine how these functions support one another and how disruptions in either AM or metacognition might lead to decrements in the other. To explore this issue we review psychopathology research concerned separately with problems in AM and metacognition in two forms of mental illness: schizophrenia and personality disorders (PD). We then discuss the advances from psychotherapy research and neuroscience for understanding how these two phenomena are related and comment on opportunities offered by these insights for scientific and clinical work.

Autobiographical Memories in Persons Diagnosed with Psychiatric Disorders

AM are understood to be a core aspect of healthy function across the human life span (Conway, 2005; Singer et al., in press). Being able to retrieve personal memories allows persons to form representations of themselves as unique beings who exist with meaningful continuity over time. They also provide a context for making sense of what is happening interpersonally in the moment.

Such processes often seem disrupted in persons suffering with significant forms of psychopathology. Instead of resorting to key past events as a prototype for meaning-making and action predictions, patients might resort to overgeneralized memories or intellectualizations, in order to make sense of their choices, dilemmas and conflicts. For instance, patients with Narcissistic PD might respond to an interpersonal conflict not by recalling specific memories about their live in which the conflict was solved, but rather with a generalization about others as inept or ungrateful. Patients diagnosed with psychosis might respond to the same dilemma without any clear recollection from the past, or if the past was recalled it might be disjointed and not clearly related to the problem in the moment.

To date, qualitative and quantitative work has suggested at least six different though related ways (Dimaggio and Semerari, 2001; Lysaker et al., 2001) in which AM are disturbed in psychosis and PD. First, AM in persons with these conditions often lack clear space and time boundaries. It is difficult to detect where and when an event has taken place. Second, when memories are retrieved they are often made sense of through the use of intellectualization and moral rules, without a nuanced sense of what actually happened between the specific people involved in the memory; for instance it might be recalled that certain people were ungrateful or demanding without there being sufficient detail to determine what might have led up to that event. Third, descriptions of others present in the narrative episode are often sketchy and the sequences of the actions and reactions are difficult to follow and bereft of detail.

Fourth, within the narrative episode the dialogue that takes place among the characters is often repetitive and stereotyped. Exchanges between people may be recalled but they tend to follow more of a formula than to reflect recollections of a unique conversation. Fifth, the narrative theme of the memory may be redundant, often reflective of the most basic or gross set of motives, and the story itself tends to be formulaic; memories may center around threat and fight/flight responses and end without any resolution or an irreversible resolution (e.g., a severing of connection to another person). A sixth problem is that AM recollected lack a pictorial quality which might enable a listener to imagine what happened. Memories may lack details related to visual, auditory, olfactory, or gustatory cues, elements which would lend an opportunity to understand the memory as a unique experience.

Turning to explorations of specific disorders, research has suggested that many patients with avoidant, dependent and obsessive-compulsive PD featured reduced AM specificity (Spinhoven et al., 2009). Similarly, narratives of patients with borderline PD have frequently been found to be overly general (Maurex et al., 2010), less often concerned with prototypical life events and moreover less coherent (Jørgensen et al., 2012). Autobiographical narratives produced by patients with borderline PD portray life events as either imbued with dysregulated affect and lacking any sense of agency (Adler et al., 2012).

Regarding schizophrenia, fundamental disruptions have been observed (Lysaker and Lysaker, 2008). Many with schizophrenia are less able to spontaneously recall memories which involved discrete events that occurred in a specific time and place (Corcoran and Frith, 2003; Riutort et al., 2003; D'Argembeau et al., 2008). There may be lesser recall of specific or well-defined memories, and a tendency to recall more public than private events (Cuervo-Lombard et al., 2007; Raffard et al., 2010). Further, the specific AM of patients with schizophrenia are less consistent with the overall self-image they endorse and the self lacks agency; this suggests a deficit not just in retrieving memories but also in integrating different aspects of identity into a coherent whole (Bennouna-Greene et al., 2012).

Metacognitive/Mentalizing Dysfunctions in Personality Disorders and Schizophrenia

Literature also suggests that patients with PD and schizophrenia suffer from a wide array of dysfunctions in thinking about themselves and others. Referred to as dysfunction in metacognition or mentalizing, this includes difficulties recognizing mental states, correctly naming them and using them in a flexible way and as a reliable source of information.

Research has specifically suggested that many with PD experience difficulties describing their own emotions, seeing links between emotion and thoughts (Semerari et al., 2007; Nicolò et al., 2011), and seeing their own thought processes in a detached and reflective way (Bateman and Fonagy, 2004; Semerari et al., 2005; Dimaggio et al., 2009a,b). Difficulties appraising the mental states of others has also been noted (Fonagy et al., 2002) though relatively less impairment has been observed in more basic laboratory ToM tasks (Domes et al., 2009; Ghiassi et al., 2010). The combination of poor mentalizing and PD have also predicted poorer vocational outcome (Bly et al., 2012); poorer mentalizing skills in patients with avoidant and borderline PD is a negative predictor of psychotherapy outcome (Gullestad et al., in press). In general, persons with PD appear to have limited abilities to use mentalization in order to respond to psychological problems (Carcione et al., 2011). The ability to reading the mind of the others within tasks that call for empathy have been found to be problematic for persons with narcissistic PD, and a deficit of which they are unaware of (Ritter et al., 2011).

More pronounced metacognitive deficits have been found in schizophrenia. Compared with persons without psychosis, deficits have been noted in a broad array of assessments of both cognitive and affective ToM tasks (Brüne, 2005; Penn et al., 2008), and these problems are associated with negative symptoms and poor social functioning (Brüne et al., 2011). The narratives of persons with schizophrenia feature severe inabilities to identify mental states, to see the world from multiple perspectives, and to use that knowledge to respond to psychological challenges (Lysaker et al., 2007; Mitchell et al., 2012) both as a trait marker (Lysaker et al., 2011b) and as a result of trauma (Lysaker et al., 2011c). Empathy has been found a problem for many with schizophrenia (Derntl et al., 2009).

Neuroscience and the Link of Autobiographical Memory with Metacognition

In parallel with these developments, neuroscientists have also offered evidence of a close relationship between AM and metacognition. For instance imaging studies have suggested that AM is supported with brain areas which partially overlap with ones devoted to attributions to mental states (Rabin et al., 2010; Spreng and Grady, 2010; Mar, 2011; Rabin and Rosenbaum, 2012). Spreng and Mar (2012) have noted that even non-overlapping cortical areas linked with AM and mentalistic attributions, share a network of connections. Whitehead et al. (2009) have suggested that the ability to engage in pretend play is coupled with activations in cortical areas implicated in both ToM and narrative processes.

The Interplay of Impairments in AM and Metacognition in Psychotherapy

While much psychopathology research has considered impairment in AM and metacognition separately, recent work in psychotherapy has begun to urge that successful treatment requires a consideration of the relationship of the two. Among those are mentalization-based treatment (Bateman and Fonagy, 2004), metacognitive interpersonal therapy for PD (Dimaggio et al., 2007, 2012), and metacognition-oriented psychotherapy for psychosis (Lysaker et al., 2007, 2011a; Buck and Lysaker, 2009; Salvatore et al., 2009, 2012). These approaches share the idea that a key aspect of therapy is to help patients to narrate specific AM—instead of resorting to overgeneralized memories or intellectualizations—as specific AM are the most fertile soil to think about for mental states (Dimaggio et al., 2010, 2012; Lysaker et al., 2011a). In other words, the enrichment of AM may promote improvements in metacognitive capacity. To address this possibility these procedures train therapists to seek specific memories. For instance, overgeneralized recalls have been suggested as a cue to explore for the specifics of narrative episodes (Dimaggio et al., 2012).

The rationale for seeking and thinking about specific episodes is based on the assumption that metacognitive reflection is at its heart a consideration of real life events, and as such impoverished AM offer little fertile ground for metacognitive activity. Accordingly, for many patients the appearance of more nuanced AM precedes the development of the ability to think in more complex ways about oneself and others; narrative episodes may provide opportunities for patients to recognize actual chains of thoughts, affects and behaviors in interpersonal interactions (Dimaggio et al., 2012). Other patients though may need to reach some basic level of access to mental states, in particular awareness of affects and their links with thoughts and behaviors, before their AM emerge. Formal analysis of case studies has shown that successful therapy involves the progressive growth of narrative capacity and aspects of metacognition, with the ability to narrate one's own story improving first, then followed by awareness of one's own states and awareness of the mental states of the others (Lysaker et al., 2005, 2007).

Implications for Treatment and Research in Psychopathology

While difficulties in AM and metacognition have been treated separately as factors which are present and influence outcome in significant forms of mental illness, psychotherapy, and neuroscience research suggests the need for these phenomena to be studied together. More efforts would seem to have the potential to answer several questions, including whether one form of deficit emerges before the other. Do metacognitive dysfunctions result in impoverished AM or diminished AM leave persons less able to think about mental states (Dimaggio et al., 2008, 2009a,b)? Are AM and metacognition best thought of as two networks operating together or a reflection of a single core psychological process? Are they decoupled in a similar way in non-clinical subjects and in different forms of psychopathology?

There is reason to believe that narrative enrichment (Gonçalves et al., 2012) and promoting mentalization (Bateman and Fonagy, 2009) are features of successful therapies; research addressing their interplay may guide the development of these interventions.

References

Adler, J. M., Chin, E. D., Kolisetty, A. P., and Oltmanss, T. F. (2012). The distinguishing characteristics of narrative identity in adults with features of borderline personality disorder: an empirical investigation. J. Pers. Dis. 26, 498–512.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bateman, A., and Fonagy, P. (2004). Psychotherapy for Borderline Personality Disorder: Mentalization-Based Treatment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bateman, A., and Fonagy, P. (2009). Randomized controlled trial of outpatient mentalization-based treatment versus structured clinical management for borderline personality disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 166, 1355–1364.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bennouna-Greene, M., Berna, F., Conway, M. A., Rathbone, C. J., Vidailhet, P., and Danion, J. M. (2012). Self-images and related autobiographical memories in schizophrenia. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 247–257.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bly, E. M., Wright, A. J., and Tuber, S. B. (2012). Unemployed and poor in New York: the impact of mentalization and axis II psychopathology on job outcome. Bull. Menninger Clin. 76, 101–129.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brüne, M. (2005). “Theory of mind” in schizophrenia: a review of the literature. Schizophr. Bull. 31, 21–42.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brüne, M., Dimaggio, G., and Lysaker, P. H. (2011). Metacognition and social functioning in schizophrenia: evidence, mechanisms of influence and treatment implications. Curr. Psychiatry Rev. 7, 239–247.

Buck, K. D., and Lysaker, P. H. (2009). Addressing metacognitive capacity in the psychotherapy for schizophrenia: a case study. Clin. Case Stud. 8, 463–464.

Carcione, A., Semerari, A., Nicolò, G., Pedone, R., Popolo, R., Conti, L., et al. (2011). Metacognitive mastery dysfunctions in personality disorder psychotherapy. Psychiatry Res. 190, 60–71.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. J. Mem. Lang. 53, 594–628.

Corcoran, R., and Frith, C. D. (2003). Autobiographical memory and theory of mind: evidence of a relationship in schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 33, 897–905.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cuervo-Lombard, C., Jovenin, N., Hedelin, G., Rizzo-Peter, L., Conway, M. A., and Danion, J. M. (2007). Autobiographical memory of adolescence and early adulthood events: an investigation in schizophrenia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 13, 335–343.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

D'Argembeau, A., Raffard, S., and Van der Linden, M. (2008). Remembering the past and imagining the future in schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 117, 247–251.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Derntl, B., Finkelmeyer, A., Toygar, T. K., Hülsmann, A., Schneider, F., Falkenberg, D. I., et al. (2009). Generalized deficit in all core components of empathy in schizophrenia. Schizphor. Res. 108. 197–206.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dimaggio, G., Carcione, A., Conti, M. L., Nicolò, G., Fiore, D., Pedone, R., et al. (2009a). Impaired decentration in personality disorder: an analysis with the metacognition assessment scale. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 16, 450–462.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dimaggio, G., Vanheule, S., Lysaker, P. H., Carcione, A., and Nicolò, G. (2009b). Impaired self-reflection in psychiatric disorders among adults: a proposal for the existence of a network of semi-independent functions. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 653–664.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dimaggio, G., and Lysaker, P. H. (eds.). (2010). Metacognition and Severe Adult Mental Disorders: from Basic Research to Treatment. London: Routledge.

Dimaggio, G., Lysaker, P. H., Carcione, A., Nicolò, G., and Semerari, A. (2008). Know yourself and you shall know the other … to a certain extent: multiple paths of influence of self-reflection on mindreading. Conscious. Cogn. 17, 778–789.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dimaggio, G., Salvatore, G., Fiore, D., Carcione, A., Nicolò, G., and Semerari, A. (2012). General principles for treating the overconstricted personality disorder. toward operationalizing technique. J. Pers. Disord. 26, 63–83.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dimaggio, G., Salvatore, G., Nicolò, G., Fiore, D., and Procacci, M. (2010). “Enhancing mental state understanding in the over-constricted personality disorder with metacognitive interpersonal therapy,” in Metacognition and Severe Adult Mental Disorders: from Basic Research to Treatment, eds G. Dimaggio and P. H. Lysaker (London: Routledge), 247–268.

Dimaggio, G., and Semerari, A. (2001). Psychopathological narrative forms. J. Constr. Psychol. 14, 1–23.

Dimaggio, G., Semerari, A., Carcione, A., Nicolò, G., and Procacci, M. (2007). Psychotherapy of Personality Disorders: Metacognition, States of Mind and Interpersonal Cycles. London: Routledge.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Domes, G., Schulze, L., and Herpertz, S. C. (2009). Emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder-a review of the literature. J. Pers. Disord. 23, 6–19.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., and Target, M. (2002). Affect Regulation, Mentalization and the Development of the Self. New York, NY: Other Press.

Ghiassi, V., Dimaggio, G., and Brüne, M. (2010). Dysfunctions in understanding others's mind in borderline personality disorder: a study using cartoon picture stories. Psychother. Res. 20, 657–667.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gonçalves, M. M., Mendes, I., Cruz, G., Ribeiro, A. P., Sousa, I., Angus, L., et al. (2012). Innovative moments and change in client-centered therapy. Psychother. Res. 22, 389–401.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gullestad, F. S., Johanssen, M. S., Høglend, P., Karterud, S., and Wilberg, T. (in press). Mentalization as a moderator of treatment effects: findings from a randomized clinical trial for personality disorders. Psychother. Res.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jørgensen, C. R., Berntsen, D., Bech, M., Kjølbye, M., Bennedsen, B. E., and Ramsgaard, S. B. (2012). Identity-related autobiographical memories and cultural life scripts in patients with Borderline Personality Disorder. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 788–798.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Liu, X., Li, L., Xiao, J., Yang, J., and Jiang, X. (in press). Abnormalities of autobiographical memory of patients with depressive disorders: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Psychother.

Lysaker, P. H., Buck, K. D., Carcione, A., Procacci, M., Salvatore, G., Nicolò, G., et al. (2011a). Addressing metacognitive capacity in the psychotherapy for schizophrenia: a conceptual model of the key tasks and processes. Psychol. Psychother. 84, 58–69.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lysaker, P. H., Erickson, M. A., Buck, B., Buck, K. D., Olesek, K., Grant, M., et al. (2011b). Metacognition and social function in schizophrenia: associations over a period of five months. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 16, 241–255.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lysaker, P. H., Gumley, A., Brüne, M., Vanheule, S., Buck, K., and Dimaggio, G. (2011c). Deficits in the ability to recognize one's own affects and those of others: associations with neurocognition, symptoms and sexual trauma among persons with schizophrenia. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 1183–1192.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lysaker, P. H., Buck, K. D., and Ringer, J. (2007). The recovery of metacognitive capacity in schizophrenia across thirty two months of individual psychotherapy: a case study. Psychother. Res. 17, 713–720.

Lysaker, P. H., Davis, L. D., Eckert, G. J., Strasburger, A., Hunter, N., and Buck, K. D. (2005). Changes in narrative structure and content in schizophrenia in long term individual psychotherapy: a single case study. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 406–416.

Lysaker, P. H., and Lysaker, J. T. (2008). Schizophrenia and the Fate of the Self. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Lysaker, P. H., Lysaker, J. T., and Lysaker, J. T. (2001). Schizophrenia and the collapse of the dialogical self: recovery, narrative and psychotherapy. Psychol. Psychother. 38, 252–261.

Mar, R. A. (2011). The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 103–134.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Markowitsch, H. J., and Staniloiu, A. (2011). Memory, autonoetic consciousness, and the self. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 16–39.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maurex, L., Lekander, M., Nilssonne, Å., Andersson, E. E., Åsberg, M., and Öhman, A. (2010). Social problem solving, autobiographical memory, trauma, and depression in women with borderline personality disorder and a history of suicide attempts. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 49, 327–342.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 5, 100–122.

Mitchell, L. J., Gumley, A., Reilly, E. S., MacBeth, A., Lysaker, P. H., Carcione, A., et al. (2012). Metacognition in forensic patients with schizophrenia and a past history of interpersonal violence: an exploratory study. Psychosis 4, 42–51.

Nelson, K., and Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: a social cultural developmental theory. Psychol. Rev. 111, 486–511.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nicolò, G., Semerari, A., Lysaker, P. H., Dimaggio, G., Conti, L., d'Angerio, S., et al. (2011). Alexithymia in personality disorders. Correlations with symptoms and interpersonal functioning. Psychiatry Res. 190, 37–42.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Palmieri, R., Dimaggio, G., and Gasparre, A. (2012). Overgeneralized autobiographical memories and metacognitive dysfunctions. Paper presented at the Congress of the Società Italiana di Terapia Comportamentale e Cognitiva (Rome), Oct 4–7.

Penn, D. L., Sanna, L. J., and Roberts, D. L. (2008). Social cognition in schizophrenia: an overview. Schizophr. Bull. 34, 408–411.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rabin, J. S., Gilboa, A., Stuss, D. T., Mar, R. A., and Rosenbaum, R. S. (2010). Common and unique neural correlates of autobiographical memory and theory of mind. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1095–1111.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rabin, J. S., and Rosenbaum, R. S. (2012). Familiarity modulates the functional relationship between theory of mind and autobiographical memory. Neuroimage 62, 520–529.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raffard, S., D'Argembeau, A., Lardi, C., Bayard, S., Boulenger, J. P., and Van der Linden, M. (2010). Narrative identity in schizophrenia. Conscious. Cogn. 19, 328–340.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ritter, K., Dziobek, I., Preißler, S., Rüter, A., Vater, A., Fydrich, T., et al. (2011). Lack of empathy in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 187, 241–247.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Riutort, M., Cuervo, C., Danion, J. M., Peretti, C. S., and Salamé, P. (2003). Reduced levels of specific autobiographical memories in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 117, 35–45.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Salvatore, G., Lysaker, P. H., Gumley, A., Popolo, R., Mari, J., and Dimaggio, G. (2012). Out of illness experience: metacognition-oriented therapy for promoting self-awareness in psychosis sufferers. Am. J. Psychother. 66, 85–106.

Salvatore, G., Popolo, R., Procacci, M., Nicolò, G., Carcione, A., Semerari, A., et al. (2009). Adapted-metacognitive interpersonal therapy for improving adherence to intersubjective contexts in a person with schizophrenia. Clin. Case Stud. 8, 473–488.

Semerari, A., Carcione, A., Dimaggio, G., Nicolò, G., and Procacci, M. (2007). Understanding minds, different functions and different disorders? The contribution of psychotherapeutic research. Psychiatry Res. 17, 106–119.

Semerari, A., Dimaggio, G., Nicolò, G., Pedone, R., Procacci, M., and Carcione, A. (2005). Metarepresentative functions in borderline personality disorders. J. Pers. Dis. 19, 690–710.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Singer, J. A., Blagov, P., Berry, M., and Oost, K. M. (in press). Self-defining memories, scripts, and the life story: narrative identity in personality and psychotherapy. J. Pers.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Spreng, R. N., and Grady, C. (2010). Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospection and theory-of-mind and their relationship to the default mode network. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1112–1123.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Spreng, R. N., and Mar, R. A. (2012). I remember you: a role for memory in social cognition and the functional neuroanatomy of their interaction. Brain Res. 1428, 43–50.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Spinhoven, P., Bamelis, L., Molendijk, M., Haringsma, R., and Arntz, A. (2009). Reduced specificity of AM in cluster C personality disorders and the role of depression, worry and experiential avoidance. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 118, 520–530.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Whitehead, C., Marchant, J. L., Craik, D., and Frith, C. D. (2009). Neural correlates of observing pretend play in which one object is represented as another. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 4, 369–378.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Dimaggio G, Salvatore G, Popolo R and Lysaker PH (2012) Autobiographical memory and mentalizing impairment in personality disorders and schizophrenia: clinical and research implications. Front. Psychology 3:529. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00529

Received: 30 October 2012; Accepted: 08 November 2012;
Published online: 26 November 2012.

Edited by:

R. Nathan Spreng, Cornell University, USA

Reviewed by:

R. Nathan Spreng, Cornell University, USA

Copyright © 2012 Dimaggio, Salvatore, Popolo and Lysaker. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: gdimaje@libero.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.