Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 19 June 2014
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic The long and short of mental time travel-- self-projection over time-scales large and small View all 25 articles

Belief in optimism might be more problematic than actual optimism

  • 1Department of Psychology, Elizabethtown College, Elizabethtown, PA, USA
  • 2Department of Music, North-West University, Potchefstroom, South Africa

People appear to be biased in their predictions of the future; predicting that projects will take less time than they actually will (see Roy et al., 2005a; Buehler et al., 2010; Halkjelsvik and Jørgensen, 2012, for review) and that they will be unlikely to experience future misfortunes (see Dunning et al., 2004, for review). These biases are frequently attributed to people being overly optimistic (Armor and Taylor, 1998, 2002; Dunning et al., 2004). However, research indicates that many of these seemingly motivated biases could actually be due to task characteristics or other non-motivational causes (Juslin et al., 2000; Chambers and Windschitl, 2004; Roy et al., 2005a). Here I review research, focusing on my own work on time estimation and self-assessment, which indicates that these seemingly optimistic biases might often have other non-motivational causes. Further, I discuss why a belief that people are overly optimistic might be problematic.

Optimism in Time Estimation?

The tendency for people to think that they will finish tasks earlier than they actually will is frequently thought of as fitting into a larger category of optimistic biases (Armor and Taylor, 1998, 2002). It has been proposed that underestimation is due to people forming an optimistic scenario of how a task will be completed and ignoring memory for how long similar tasks have taken in the past (Kahneman and Tversky, 1982; Buehler et al., 2002).

However, underestimation of future task duration might not be due to optimism about the future, but instead be due to biased memories of the past (Roy et al., 2005a; see also Griffin and Buehler, 2005; Roy et al., 2005). Similarly, it has been proposed that it is the quality of people's episodic (Szpunar et al., 2013) and semantic (Irish and Piguet, 2013) memories that predict their ability to envision the future (Schacter et al., 2008; Szpunar and McDermott, 2008). In support of the view that biased predictions are caused by biased memories, research indicates that factors that influence memories of past task duration have parallel influences on predictions of future task duration (Roy and Christenfeld, 2007, 2008) with biased memories frequently associated with biased predictions (Thomas et al., 2004, 2007). Furthermore, correcting memory for past task duration is one of the only interventions that has been found to successfully reduce bias and improve accuracy for predictions of future task duration (Roy et al., 2008). In relation, people seem to be accurate in estimates for tasks where frequent feedback on timing is given (Tobin and Grondin, 2012), but not for tasks where feedback is absent (Tobin and Grondin, 2009; Tobin et al., 2010; Bisson et al., 2012). In the field of software development, predictions of for how long a new project will take are more accurate when data on task duration for similar projects is utilized during predictions than when it is not (Jørgensen, 2004a; Furulund and Moløkken-Østvold, 2007).

While bias in predicted duration appears to often be due to bias in memory, memory itself could be biased by people's motivations. However, bias in remembered duration often appears to be due to task characteristics such as novelty (Boltz et al., 1998; Hinds, 1999; Roy and Christenfeld, 2007; Tobin et al., 2010), relative duration (Yarmey, 2000; Roy and Christenfeld, 2008; Tobin and Grondin, 2009), size of potential estimation anchors (Thomas and Handley, 2008), and duration since completion (Roy et al., 2008). These tasks characteristics may alter attention that is paid to the task (Thomas and Weaver, 1975) or memory storage size associated with the task (Ornstein, 1969; Block and Reed, 1978) and bias estimation. Task characteristics cause bias in remembered duration that in turn causes bias in predicted duration. For example, actors and observers appeared to be more influenced in their estimates, for both past and future tasks, by task characteristics, such as overall task duration and number of remembered task components, than they were by being the more motivationally involved person performing the task (Roy et al., 2013a; see also Byram, 1997; Hinds, 1999; Jørgensen, 2004b).

The reason that memory and prediction might be similarly biased is that both might rely on a similar constructive process with estimation based upon a general prototypical representation for task duration that is adjusted up or down depending on the specifics of the situation (Burt and Kemp, 1994; Roy et al., 2005a). Because memory for past task duration is often biased (see Wallace and Rabin, 1960; Fraisse, 1963; Block and Zakay, 1997 for reviews), the prototypical representations that people have for many tasks, which is the average of previous experience, is similarly biased. A result of prototypical memories that underestimate duration is an expectation that tasks in the future, such as travel plans, will take less time than they will in actuality (van de Ven et al., 2011). While people frequently underestimate how long tasks will take in the future, much of this bias appears to be due to task characteristics and not due to optimism or motivation. This is not to say that motivation never plays a role in bias; at times people's motivations influence their memories for past task duration (Meade, 1963; Schwab et al., 2013) and their predictions of future duration (Buehler et al., 1997; Byram, 1997). However, research indicates that bias in estimated duration frequently exists without motivational causes.

Optimism in Other Judgments?

A person's estimate of task duration is often tied to their perceived competency at the task. For many tasks, quicker completion is linked to high ability. Indeed, more experience with a task is often linked to a greater tendency to underestimate task duration (Boltz et al., 1998; Hinds, 1999; Roy and Christenfeld, 2007). Further, people often appear to be overly optimistic in their self-assessments, rating themselves as above average on a number of skills and personality traits (see Taylor and Brown, 1988; Chambers and Windschitl, 2004; Dunning et al., 2004; Sedikides and Gregg, 2008, for reviews). It would seem logically impossible for the majority of people to be above average (Taylor and Brown, 1988). However, similar to time estimation, this apparent bias may be due to people relying on prototypical representations of ability when assessing their own abilities (Krueger, 1998; Gigerenzer, 2002; Moore, 2007; Galesic et al., 2012; Roy et al., 2013b). People understand that various skills have skewed ability distributions and their self-assessments are often related to distribution shape: high when most are believed to be good and low when most are believed to be bad (Galesic et al., 2012; Roy et al., 2013b). When the skill being rated has a skewed distribution, a tendency to rely on prototypical representations can lead to self-assessments that, on face value, appear to indicate a belief in the self as being falsely unique (above or below average), but actually may indicate a belief in the self as being prototypical (near the mode of the distribution; Roy et al., 2013b). Further, a reliance on prototypes can also help explain why people underestimate the likelihood that they will experience rare events, such as getting cancer, and overestimate the likelihood that they will experience common events, such as owning a car (Chambers et al., 2003; Kruger and Burrus, 2004).

It is also possible that other task characteristics, such as how clearly defined and specific the task is (Dunning et al., 2004; Roy and Liersch, 2013) or how much information people have about their own ability and the ability of others (Chambers and Windschitl, 2004), can influence self-assessments. On a related note, people's overconfidence in their responses on quizzes and tests might have more to do with the type of questions being asked than with actual self-confidence (see Juslin et al., 2000 for review). While people appear to overestimate their abilities and performance, these biases appear to often be due to specific aspects of the skill, performance or event and not due to optimism or motivation.

Problems with Perceived Optimism

While people might not be overly optimistic about how their future will unfold, a belief in optimism as the cause of bias may be problematic both for researchers and for people in general. Inasmuch as biased predictions are viewed as being caused by optimism and that being optimistic is viewed positively, people might not seek out the true causes of bias and ways to eliminate bias.

Even though people acknowledge they are often biased in their predictions (Buehler et al., 2002; Armor et al., 2008), it is not clear if they know why they are biased. In general, people are not always able to accurately describe their decision-making processes (e.g., Wilson and Hodges, 1992). It appears that, like researchers (Taylor and Brown, 1988; Armor and Taylor, 1998, 2002; Dunning et al., 2004), people often attribute their errors to being overly optimistic (Armor et al., 2008). The belief in optimism as a cause of bias can be problematic for two reasons: first, people might be missing the real causes of their error. To make accurate and unbiased predictions for how long it will take to complete a task, people would need to realize that factors such as task length (Roy and Christenfeld, 2008), familiarity (Roy and Christenfeld, 2007), and complexity (Roy et al., 2013a) biased their memories for these tasks and correct these memories before estimating future task duration. Simply ascribing their error to optimism would make them miss the actual causes.

Second, while optimism appears to be a popular (Christensen-Szalanski and Beach, 1984), convenient, and more importantly, common excuse for error, there are few negatives to being seen as overly optimistic. While people believe that they are at times overly optimistic, they actually believe that they should try to be more optimistic (Armor et al., 2008). They appear to be willing to deal with what they believe are the negative consequences of their optimism. To a certain degree, they would be correct to do so because a large number of positive outcomes have been associated with having an optimistic outlook (e.g., Rasmussen et al., 2009). Because they do not mind their self-diagnosed cause of their bias, people will not be motivated to seek out the real cause. Interventions aimed at decreasing bias by decreasing optimism are likely to be unpopular as well as misguided (Roy et al., 2005a).

Summary

People often appear to be overly optimistic in their predictions of the future. However, much of this apparent optimism could be due to other processes such as an over reliance on prototypical representations. In the case of time estimation, people's representation for task duration is often too short, causing them to underestimate future completion times. To improve predictions, factors that bias memory need to be taken into account and corrected. If, instead, bias in prediction is attributed to an optimistic outlook, then people will be unlikely to identify and correct the true causes of their bias. Interventions might be better aimed at decreasing the belief in over-optimism, not optimism itself.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Armor, D. A., Massey, C., and Sackett, A. M. (2008). Prescribed optimism: is it right to be wrong about the future? Psychol. Sci. 19, 329–331. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02089.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Armor, D. A., and Taylor, S. E. (1998). “Situated optimism: specific outcome expectancies and self-regulation,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 30, ed M. P. Zanna (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 309–379.

Armor, D. A., and Taylor, S. E. (2002). “When predictions fail: the dilemma of unrealistic optimism,” in Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment, eds T. D. Gilovich, D. W. Griffin, and D. Kahneman (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 334–347. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511808098.021

CrossRef Full Text

Bisson, N., Tobin, S., and Grondin, S. (2012). Prospective and retrospective time estimates of children: a comparison based on ecological tasks. PLoS ONE 7:e33049. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033049

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Block, R. A., and Reed, M. A. (1978). Remembered duration: evidence for a contextual change hypothesis. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem. 4, 656–665. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.4.6.656

CrossRef Full Text

Block, R. A., and Zakay, D. (1997). Prospective and retrospective durations judgments: a meta-analytic review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 4, 184–197. doi: 10.3758/BF03209393

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boltz, M. G., Kupperman, C., and Dunne, J. (1998). The role of learning in remembered duration. Mem. Cogn. 26, 903–921. doi: 10.3758/BF03201172

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buehler, R., Griffin, D., and MacDonald, H. (1997). The role of motivated reasoning in optimistic time predictions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 23, 238–247. doi: 10.1177/0146167297233003

CrossRef Full Text

Buehler, R., Griffin, D., and Peetz, J. (2010). Chapter one-the planning fallacy: cognitive, motivational, and social origins. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 43, 1–62. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(10)43001-4

CrossRef Full Text

Buehler, R., Griffin, D., and Ross, M. (2002). “Inside the planning fallacy: the causes and consequences of optimistic time prediction,” in Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment, eds T. D. Gilovich, D. W. Griffin, and D. Kahneman (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 250–270. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511808098.016

CrossRef Full Text

Burt, C. D. B., and Kemp, S. (1994). Construction of activity duration and time management potential. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 8, 155–168. doi: 10.1002/acp.2350080206

CrossRef Full Text

Byram, S. J. (1997). Cognitive and motivational factors influencing time prediction. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 3, 216–239. doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.3.3.216

CrossRef Full Text

Chambers, J. R., and Windschitl, P. D. (2004). Biases in social comparative judgments: the role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. Psychol. Bull. 130, 813–838. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.813

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chambers, J. R., Windschitl, P. D., and Suls, J. (2003). Egocentrism, event frequency, and comparative optimism: when what happens frequently is more likely to happen to me. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 1343–1356. doi: 10.1177/0146167203256870

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Christensen-Szalanski, J. J. J., and Beach, L. R. (1984). The citation bias: fad and fashion in the judgment and decision literature. Am. Psychol. 39, 75–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.39.1.75

CrossRef Full Text

Dunning, D., Heath, C., and Suls, J. M. (2004). Flawed self-assessment: implications for health, education, and the workplace. Psychol. Sci. Public Int. 5, 69–106. doi: 10.1111/j.1529-1006.2004.00018.x

CrossRef Full Text

Fraisse, P. (1963). The Psychology of Time. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

Furulund, K. M., and Moløkken-Østvold, K. (2007). “Increasing software effort estimation accuracy using experience data, estimation models and checklists,” in Seventh International Conference on Quality Software, 2007, QSIC'07 (Portland, OR), 342–347.

Galesic, M., Olsson, H., and Rieskamp, J. (2012). Social sampling explains apparent biases in judgments of social environments. Psychol. Sci. 23, 1515–1523. doi: 10.1177/0956797612445313

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gigerenzer, G. (2002). Calculated Risks: How to Know When Numbers Deceive You. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.

Griffin, D., and Buehler, R. (2005). Biases and fallacies, memories and predictions: comment on Roy, Christenfeld, and McKenzie 2005. Psychol. Bull. 131, 757–760. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.757

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Halkjelsvik, T., and Jørgensen, M. (2012). From origami to software development: a review of studies on judgment-based predictions of performance time. Psychol. Bull. 138, 238–271. doi: 10.1037/a0025996

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hinds, P. J. (1999). The curse of expertise: the effects of expertise and debiasing methods on predictions of novice performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 5, 205–221. doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.5.2.205

CrossRef Full Text

Irish, M., and Piguet, O. (2013). The pivotal role of semantic memory in remembering the past and imagining the future. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:27. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jørgensen, M. (2004a). Top-down and bottom-up expert estimation of software development effort. Inform. Soft. Technol. 46, 3–16. doi: 10.1016/S0950-5849(03)00093-4

CrossRef Full Text

Jørgensen, M. (2004b). Regression models of software development effort estimation accuracy and bias. Empir. Soft. Eng. 9, 297–314. doi: 10.1023/B:EMSE.0000039881.57613.cb

CrossRef Full Text

Juslin, P., Winman, A., and Olsson, H. (2000). Naive empiricism and dogmatism in confidence research: a critical examination of the hard-easy effect. Psychol. Rev. 107, 384–396. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.384

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1982). “Intuitive prediction: biases and corrective procedures,” in Judgments Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, eds D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, and A. Tversky (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 414–421. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511809477.031

CrossRef Full Text

Krueger, J. (1998). The bet on bias: a foregone conclusion? Psycoloquy 9:46.

Kruger, J., and Burrus, J. (2004). Egocentrism and focalism in unrealistic optimism (and pessimism). J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 40, 332–340. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.06.002

CrossRef Full Text

Meade, R. D. (1963). Effect of motivation and progress on the estimation of longer time intervals. J. Exp. Psychol. 65, 564–567. doi: 10.1037/h0046552

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moore, D. A. (2007). No so above average after all: when people believe they are worse than average and its implications for theories of bias in social comparison. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 102, 42–58. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.09.005

CrossRef Full Text

Ornstein, R. E. (1969). On The Experience of Time. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

Rasmussen, H. N., Scheier, M. F., and Greenhouse, J. B. (2009). Optimism and physical health: a meta-analytic review. Ann. Behav. Med. 37, 239–256. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9111-x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roy, M. M., and Christenfeld, N. J. S. (2007). Bias in memory predicts bias in estimation of future task duration. Mem. Cogn. 35, 557–564. doi: 10.3758/BF03193294

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roy, M. M., and Christenfeld, N. J. S. (2008). Effect of task length on remembered and predicted duration. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 15, 202–208. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.1.202

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roy, M. M., Christenfeld, N. J. S., and Jones, M. (2013a). Actors, observers and the estimation of task duration. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 66, 121–137. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.699973

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roy, M. M., Liersch, M. J., and Broomell, S. (2013b). People believe that they are prototypically good and bad. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 122, 200–213. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2013.07.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roy, M. M., Christenfeld, N. J. S., and McKenzie, C. R. M. (2005a). Underestimation of future duration: memory incorrectly used or memory bias. Psychol. Bull. 131, 738–756. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.738

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roy, M., Christenfeld, N., and McKenzie, C. (2005b). The broad applicability of memory bias and its coexistence with the planning fallacy: reply to Griffin and Buehler 2005. Psychol. Bull. 131, 761–762. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.761

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roy, M. M., and Liersch, M. J. (2013). I am better than you think: self-awareness of the better-than-average effect. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 43, 1648–1659. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12117

CrossRef Full Text

Roy, M. M., Mitten, T. S., and Christenfeld, N. J. S. (2008). Correcting memory improves accuracy of predicted duration. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 14, 266–275. doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.14.3.266

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2008). Episodic simulation of future events: concepts, data, and application. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 39–60. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schwab, S., Memmert, D., and Roy, M. M. (2013). The effect of motivation and attention on bias in memory for duration. Psychology 4, 83–87. doi: 10.4236/psych.2013.42011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sedikides, C., and Gregg, A. P. (2008). Self-enhancement: food for thought. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 3, 102–116. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00068.x

CrossRef Full Text

Szpunar, K. K., Addis, D. R., McLelland, V. C., and Schacter, D. L. (2013). Memories of the future: new insights into the adaptive value of episodic memory. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:47. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00047

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Szpunar, K. K., and McDermott, K. B. (2008). Episodic future thought and its relation to remembering: evidence from ratings of subjective experience. Conscious. Cogn. 17, 330–334. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Taylor, S. E., and Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: a social-psychological perspective on mental health. Psychol. Bull. 103, 193–210. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thomas, E. A. C., and Weaver, W. B. (1975). Cognitive processing and time perception. Percept. Psychophys. 17, 363–367. doi: 10.3758/BF03199347

CrossRef Full Text

Thomas, K. E., and Handley, S. J. (2008). Anchoring in time estimation. Acta Psycol. 127, 24–29. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.12.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thomas, K. E., Handley, S. J., and Newstead, S. E. (2004). The effects of prior experience on estimating the duration of simple tasks. Curr. Psychol. Cogn. 22, 83–100.

Thomas, K. E., Handley, S. J., and Newstead, S. E. (2007). The role of prior task experience in temporal misestimation. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 60, 230–240. doi: 10.1080/17470210600785091

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tobin, S., Bisson, N., and Grondin, S. (2010). An ecological approach to prospective and retrospective timing of long durations: a study involving gamers. PLoS ONE 5:e9271. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009271

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tobin, S., and Grondin, S. (2009). Video games and the perception of very long durations by adolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 25, 554–559. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tobin, S., and Grondin, S. (2012). Time perception is enhanced by task duration knowledge: evidence from experienced swimmers. Mem. Cogn. 40, 1339–1351. doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0231-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van de Ven, N., van Rijswijk, L., and Roy, M. M. (2011). The return trip effect: why the return trip often seems to take less time. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 827–832. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0150-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wallace, M., and Rabin, A. I. (1960). Temporal experience. Psychol. Bull. 57, 213–236. doi: 10.1037/h0041410

CrossRef Full Text

Wilson, T. D., and Hodges, S. D. (1992). “Attitudes as temporary constructions,” in The Construction of Social Judgments, eds L. L. Martin and A. Tesser (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc), 37–65.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Yarmey, A. D. (2000). Retrospective duration estimations for variant and invariant events in field situations. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 14, 45–57. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(200001)14:1<45::AID-ACP623>3.0.CO;2-U

CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: time, optimism, bias, prototype, memory, future

Citation: Roy MM (2014) Belief in optimism might be more problematic than actual optimism. Front. Psychol. 5:624. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00624

Received: 30 April 2014; Paper pending published: 28 May 2014;
Accepted: 02 June 2014; Published online: 19 June 2014.

Edited by:

Simon Grondin, Université Laval, Canada

Reviewed by:

Anne-Claire Rattat, University of Toulouse-CUFR JF Champollion, France

Copyright © 2014 Roy. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: roym@etown.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.