Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 22 December 2016
Sec. Cognition

Neurophysiology of Grasping Actions: Evidence from ERPs

\r\nDirk Koester,*Dirk Koester1,2*Thomas Schack,,Thomas Schack1,2,3Jan Westerholz,\r\nJan Westerholz1,2
  • 1Center of Excellence – Cognitive Interaction Technology (CITEC), Bielefeld, Germany
  • 2Neurocognition and Action – Biomechanics Research Group, Faculty of Psychology and Sport Science, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
  • 3Research Institute for Cognition and Robotics (CoR-Lab), Bielefeld, Germany

We use our hands very frequently to interact with our environment. Neuropsychology together with lesion models and intracranial recordings and imaging work yielded important insights into the functional neuroanatomical correlates of grasping, one important function of our hands, pointing toward a functional parietofrontal brain network. Event-related potentials (ERPs) register directly electrical brain activity and are endowed with high temporal resolution but have long been assumed to be susceptible to movement artifacts. Recent work has shown that reliable ERPs can be obtained during movement execution. Here, we review the available ERP work on (uni) manual grasping actions. We discuss various ERP components and how they may be related to functional components of grasping according to traditional distinctions of manual actions such as planning and control phases. The ERP results are largely in line with the assumption of a parietofrontal network. But other questions remain, in particular regarding the temporal succession of frontal and parietal ERP effects. With the low number of ERP studies on grasping, not all ERP effects appear to be coherent with one another. Understanding the control of our hands may help to develop further neurocognitive theories of grasping and to make progress in prosthetics, rehabilitation or development of technical systems for support of human actions.

Introduction

Our hands are important tools to interact with our environment. Yet, we do not fully understand the biological underpinnings of manual actions, that is, how the central nervous system (CNS) achieves and controls manual dexterity. In motor control, it remains a debated issue whether or not the brain entertains (stored) grasp representations (i.e., grip types). Otherwise, grasping movements might be computed on the fly when needed. Here, we take an empirical perspective on neurophysiological processes underlying grasping actions.

How we grasp an object is determined by external and by internal factors. External factors refer to object properties, such as size, weight, shape, or texture (intrinsic properties) and situation aspects, such as position, distance (i.e., spatial coordinates) or possibly orientation of the object (extrinsic object properties; cf. dual-channel hypothesis; Jeannerod, 1981; Castiello, 2005; Goodale, 2011). In contrast, internal factors are properties of the person or the task; that is, largely cognitive variables, such as action planning, the goal of the action, habits or familiarity with the object or action (Rosenbaum et al., 1992; Meulenbroek et al., 1993; Kunde et al., 2007; van der Wel et al., 2007; Logan, 2009; Herbort and Butz, 2010, 2011; Knudsen et al., 2012; Belardinelli et al., 2016). For example, when grasping a bottle, the hand movement including the shaping of the fingers differs depending on whether the bottle will be thrown or precisely placed (Ansuini et al., 2008). That is, the purpose or goal of the action influences the (preceding) movement (Marteniuk et al., 1987; Armbrüster and Spijkers, 2006; Rizzolatti et al., 2014) which reflects a cognitive influence on grasping movements (see also Glover, 2004; Seegelke et al., 2014). Cognitive processes are of particular interest; they are subserved by cortical activity. Electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG) are very well-suited to examine cortical activity in normal healthy humans, especially when fast (cognitive) processes are of interest.

For the performance of such actions, two processing stages have been distinguished (two-component model; Woodworth, 1899; Elliott et al., 2001). In a planning phase, the movement is prepared and initiated without sensory feedback. In the control phase, the movement execution is monitored including (re-)afferent feedback information (Wolpert and Ghahramani, 2000; Glover et al., 2012). An overarching question is whether or not such grip types are (neuro) cognitively represented (Rizzolatti et al., 2000; Begliomini et al., 2007). Alternatively, grasping movements may be computed (e.g., as contact points for the fingers; Smeets and Brenner, 1999), or movement profiles might result from tissue and mechanical properties of the movement system (e.g., Kelso, 1994, 1997). Here, we are concerned with the neurophysiological correlates of (uni) manual grasping in humans with a focus on event-related potentials (ERPs) as there is little such research available.

Other neurocognitive methodologies have been applied in manual action research of which grasping is one important type and these have their own limitations (see below). So far, there is agreement regarding a parietofrontal brain network as the neuroanatomical correlates of grasping (Castiello, 2005; Filimon, 2010; Grafton, 2010 for review), but many neurophysiological studies focussed on reaching or tool use rather than grasping (e.g., Wheaton et al., 2005a; Archambault et al., 2009; Proverbio et al., 2011; Striemer et al., 2011). Other manual actions also received some attention in neurophysiological research, for example, finger or arm movements (Wessel et al., 1994; Slobounov et al., 1998), tapping (Lang et al., 1990), and throwing (Frömer et al., 2012).

Different parts of the parietofrontal network are assumed to fulfill different functions in manual actions. For reaching, it is assumed that parietal areas, especially the parieto-occipital sulcus represent the goals of the movement (Astafiev et al., 2003; Hamilton and Grafton, 2006). Other authors assume that visuo-spatial information regarding the target objects are processed in parietal areas (Jeannerod et al., 1995; Galletti et al., 2003; Filimon et al., 2009; Tarantino et al., 2014) whereas Goldenberg and Spatt (2009) attribute the processing of mechanical interactions among effectors and the environment to parietal areas.

More consistently, frontal areas are proposed to subserve executive functions, and some specifically ascribe goal representations (van Elk et al., 2012) to frontal areas, the planning of sequential behavior (van Schie and Bekkering, 2007) or the control of action execution (Haggard, 2011; Ridderinkhof et al., 2004). Furthermore, in primate studies which combined reaching and grasping, so-called motor schemas have been assigned to frontal areas whereas perceptual schemas have been ascribed to parietal areas (Rizzolatti et al., 1988; Jeannerod et al., 1995). More recently, two distinct neural processing streams have been discussed; one for the transport and one for the grasping component (Jeannerod, 1984, 1988; Galletti et al., 2003; Hattori et al., 2009; Vesia et al., 2013). However, Fattori et al. (2010) reported grasping (pre-shaping of the hand) specific neurons in the “transport stream” of macaque monkeys, specifically, in the medial posterior aspect of the parietal area (V6A, see also Galletti et al., 1999). That is, while there is some agreement about the functional role of frontal areas for manual actions, there is more of a controversy regarding parietal brain areas.

Because movements themselves can pose recording problems for EEG and MEG, such studies often used simple movements like button presses or have focused on the planning/preparation phase of a movement or imagined movements (Verleger et al., 2000; Zaepffel and Brochier, 2012). However, recently reliable ERPs with their high temporal resolution (msec range) have been obtained during movement execution (van Schie and Bekkering, 2007; Koester and Schiller, 2008; Kirsch and Hennighausen, 2010) and are, thus, suitable for investigating the neurophysiological underpinnings of manual actions; it appears that a sufficient number of artifact free trials could be obtained in these studies (for approaches to correct for movement artifact see, e.g., Hesse and James, 2006; Ting et al., 2006). So far, only very few ERP studies have investigated grasping movements. Examining the temporal dynamics of the underlying neurophysiology can advance our understanding of the biological underpinnings of motor control and their links to cognition.

The available grasping ERP studies investigated the neural correlates of different grip types (precision and power grips), for example, with an emphasis on immediate or final action goals. Others studied the relation between neurophysiological processes and behavioral consequences and co-registered kinematic and ERP data. Also, the relation between symbolic processing and grasping has been addressed by using language materials as imperative cues for grasping responses. Also, the role of intentions and habitual effects were investigated in grasping.

van Schie and Bekkering (2007) reported, as far as we know, for the first time ERP findings on executing precision grips. They had participants grasp and transport an object across or below a barrier instructing either how to grasp the object (immediate goal) or where to place the object (final goal). Both, immediate and final goal instructions required the same movements excluding explanations by kinematic differences. Hence, the ERP effects, namely a parieto-occipital slow wave related to the immediate goal and a slow wave over left frontal regions for the final goal specification have been related to the prehension of the object and the planning/control of sequential behavior, respectively. Consistent with the posterior effects, van Elk et al. (2012) reported enhanced parietal activation for the observation of grip errors and interpreted them as processing hand-object interaction. Westerholz et al. (2013) extended van Schie and Bekkering’s (2007) study to power grips (also comparing the same movements emphasizing the immediate or the final goal) to test for differences in the neurophysiological time course because power grips are known to have a shorter deceleration phase and a later maximum grip aperture than precision grips (Castiello et al., 1992).

Both precision (van Schie and Bekkering, 2007) and power grips (Westerholz et al., 2013) yielded a temporally similar frontal slow wave (-1,100 to 0 ms; bilateral for power grips) time-locked to movement end. If reflecting the planning/control of sequential behavior, these planning/control processes should not differ for the grip types. The planning might relate to the goal state (or posture) because the ERP effects were time-locked to the movement end. In contrast, precision and power grips differed temporally over posterior areas. Power grips elicited a posterior slow wave (from at least -600 to -200 ms time-locked to grasping) that preceded the posterior slow wave for precision grips by about 300 ms (-300 to 0 ms). This posterior effect was related to the prehension, and as it precedes the grasping itself, it may, specifically, reflect the preparation of the prehension. Such a preparation may be easier for power compared with precision grips as fewer parameters have to be controlled with less precision.

Another study combined kinematic (hand position and grip aperture) and ERP measures in grasping (De Sanctis et al., 2013). Participants grasped small or large objects with a precision or a power grip, respectively, while kinematics and ERPs were recorded. An increased negativity (motor-related N400) was found at frontal midline electrodes for grasping small objects compared with larger objects. Importantly, this m-N400 component peaked earlier for power than precision grips in accordance with the time course differences between these grip types reported above (van Schie and Bekkering, 2007; Westerholz et al., 2013). De Sanctis et al. (2013) reported ERP amplitude effects before (planning) and during movement execution. Furthermore, movement time (kinematics) and m-N400 latency (ERPs) were positively correlated for both grip types (r = 0.46 and 0.49) suggesting a functional relation between neural and behavioral components of grasping actions.

Another study that co-registered kinematics and ERPs investigated whether grasping is influenced by word reading (Boulenger et al., 2008). Participants were asked to perform a reach-and-grasp task in response to written (non) words, and for the critical results the ERPs and the kinematics for verbs and nouns were compared. (Verbs but not nouns were expected to pre-activate neural circuits for motor control because only verbs refer to actions.) Even though Boulenger et al. (2008) did not investigate various grip types, the results are partly in line with De Sanctis et al.’s (2013) data in that the ERPs showed a reduced ERP amplitude (taken to be the readiness potential, a motor planning ERP component; Kornhuber and Deecke, 1965) for verbs compared with nouns before movement initiation, that is, during planning; the ERP was also affected during movement execution. That is, the ERPs were affected in both phases, planning and execution, as in De Sanctis et al.’s study. Boulenger et al. (2008) reported also a kinematic effect; the wrist’s maximal acceleration was smaller for the verb compared to the noun condition but the ERPs and the kinematics were not analyzed in a combined manner.

Shared neural mechanisms of word reading and grasp planning (in delayed action execution) were also investigated by van Elk et al. (2008). In a dual-task paradigm, participants prepared grasping one of two objects (magnifying glass or cup). Then a semantic categorisation was performed on a written word (to ensure deep processing; Collins and Loftus, 1975). After a subsequent and delayed go/no-go stimulus (a tone), the prepared action was performed in go trials. Critically, two factors were manipulated. First, the action could be meaningful (bringing the glass toward the eye or the cup toward the mouth) or meaningless (glass to mouth or cup to eye). Second, the words could be congruent (“eye” or “mouth”) or incongruent (e.g., “belly” or “knee”) with the action goal. (All critical words referred to body parts.) Similar to Boulenger et al. (2008), the results suggest that reading and action planning share some neural resources. In particular, words that were incongruent with the action goal elicited an anterior N400 effect compared with congruent words but only if the prepared action was meaningful. If the prepared action was meaningless, no N400 effect was obtained. Even though it remains to be shown whether these effects can be generalized to the execution phase, the results go beyond Boulenger et al.’s findings, as they show an involvement of semantic information in preparing a grasping action whereas Boulenger et al., reported an effect for word class, that is, syntactic information.

Interestingly, the N400 effects had a more frontal focus than is reported in language studies (Kutas and Federmeier, 2011). This distribution is in accordance with the frontal distribution in the study by De Sanctis et al. (2013) and has also been found in spoken language production (Koester and Schiller, 2008). These initial observations raise the possibility that such an anterior distribution is related to frontal, maybe (pre)motor, and cortical generators.

Clearly, influences of symbolic information (e.g., reading words) on grasping are cognitive in nature. Three other studies investigated the influence of intentions on grasping by means of ERPs. Two studies investigated the preparation phase of precision grips, and one the planning and execution of power grips. To investigate temporal organization of action planning, Bozzacchi et al. (2012a) recorded pre-movement ERPs, specifically the readiness potential (RP). Participants had three different tasks; reaching, grasping and impossible grasping (attempting to grasp with constrained fingers) of a teacup. These authors found an earlier RP onset over parietal areas and a later RP onset over frontal areas. Parietal effects have been suggested to reflect the specification of grip-related information (i.e., preparation of prehension; van Schie and Bekkering, 2007) that is further processed in frontal areas (Bozzacchi et al., 2012a; see Wheaton et al., 2005a,b, 2009 for manual pantomime movements). The activity over parietal areas began well before action onset for grasping, but not for reaching or impossible grasping. More interestingly, premotor activity seems to precede parietal activity in some instances (Filimon, 2010), but Bozzacchi et al. (2012a) reported parietal activity to precede frontal activity which could be related to the temporal order of parietal before frontal effects in van Schie and Bekkering (2007) and Westerholz et al. (2013; see also Wheaton et al., 2005a). Thus, the temporal organization of the neural mechanisms underlying grasping and its preparation remains controversial.

Similarly, Bozzacchi et al. (2012b) compared ERPs for real grasps, “virtual” grasps (key presses that released a video showing a grasping action) and key presses (without a subsequent effect). Similar motor preparation processes (RP) were found for real and virtual grasps over posterior parietal areas which differed from the key press condition. Again, the parietal activity for virtual and real grasps preceded activity over (pre)motor areas. As the ERP for virtual grasping resembled the real grasping and not the key press condition, Bozzacchi et al. (2012b) conclude that the goal of the action and not the movement kinematics influenced the preparation phase. Furthermore, these authors suggested that action preparation is affected by the meaning of that action, similar to the overall conclusion by van Elk et al. (2008). That is, Bozzachi et al.’s works point toward an influence of the anticipated goal state on grasping preparation. Zaepffel and Brochier (2012) used the contingent negative variation (CNV) to investigate planning processes for a grasping task (reach-grasp-and-pull). Although these authors did not report ERP effects for execution, their findings are taken to support goal anticipations (force and hand shape, i.e., grip type) in accordance with Bozzacchi et al.’s works.

Similar to anticipations, Westerholz et al. (2014a) investigated the role of intentional processes in grasping, that is, the decision what action to perform. These authors investigated the neurophysiological correlates of planning and execution of goal-related grasping and the dominance of an immediate “goal” (grip configuration) and a final goal (goal state). They used a bar transport task; participants grasped a horizontally suspended bar and placed it on one of two target positions. Depending on the experimental condition either the grip (underhand vs. overhand grip) or the goal state (target position and bar orientation) were specified. Alternatively, grip or goal state could be free (i.e., unspecified) and participants had to decide for themselves (2 × 2 design). Interestingly, the specification (i.e., instruction) of goal states yielded a parietal slow wave in the ERP that was extended toward frontocentral electrodes time-locked to grasping. Also, a right frontal slow wave was found time-locked to movement end. These effects were tied to (prepared) prehension and the planning or anticipation of an action sequence, respectively (see van Schie and Bekkering, 2007; Westerholz et al., 2013). Critically, these ERP effects were elicited by the specification of the goal. The specification of the grip itself did not yield any ERP effect suggesting that the goal state which needs to be anticipated is more important than the immediately required movement for grasping, underlining the importance of anticipation in grasping in line with Bozzacchi et al. (2012a,b) and Zaepffel and Brochier (2012).

In another task (grasp-and-rotate), Westerholz et al. (2014b) investigated the role of habitual action control in addition to specified vs. free grip choices. Habitual influences on action have been suggested next to goal state anticipations by Herbort and Butz (2011; see also Künzell et al., 2013). Westerholz et al. (2014b) asked participants to grasp a bar that was marked at one end (as a pointer) and mounted to a dial. When grasping, participants had to rotate the dial immediately. The concomitantly recorded ERPs were analyzed for specified vs. free grips and also for habitual vs. non-habitual grasping (thumb toward the pointer end is considered habitual; Rosenbaum et al., 1992). Interestingly, a bilateral frontal slow wave time-locked to movement end was more negative for non-habitual grasping but there were no posterior ERP effects. Similar to Westerholz et al. (2014a), grip specification yielded also no ERP effects. Whether the absence of posterior effects is related to a “missing” transport phase, to the nature of the task (rotation vs. transport) or the functionality of the rotation (Tucker and Ellis, 1998) remains to be shown. For an overview of these ERP studies on grasping see Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Overview of event-related potential (ERP) studies on grasping (grip execution) including tasks and major outcomes.

As said, other approaches have been applied to manual actions (including grasping). The EEG signal offers further approaches for experimental design and signal analysis not reviewed here. Analyses of various frequency bands of the EEG signal (e.g., Wheaton et al., 2009), of the lateralized readiness potentials (LRP; Verleger et al., 2000; Leuthold and Jentzsch, 2001) and the CNV are fruitful approaches (Zaepffel and Brochier, 2012). Further neurocognitive methodologies that have been used comprise lesion studies, intracranial recording (predominantly animals; e.g., Galletti et al., 2003; Fattori et al., 2010), neuropsychological studies (predominantly human patients; e.g., Wessel et al., 1994; Verleger et al., 1999, 2003; Goldenberg and Spatt, 2009) and functional brain imaging (blood flow changes; e.g., Burnod et al., 1999; Majdandzić et al., 2007; Piefke et al., 2009; Andric et al., 2013). As all others, these methodologies have also different limitations. Integration of these methodologies would provide valuable insights but has to await future research.

Future work may also investigate the role of other modalities (e.g., tactile or haptics). The relation between manual control and other cognitive domains (e.g., language or memory) is not fully understood (Pulvermüller, 2005; Spiegel et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2014). Neurocognitive models of grasping would benefit from relating ERPs to other imaging methods, for example, by localization analyses to establish a functional link between functional anatomical correlates and ERP effects. Finally, future work may focus on applications, for example, in sports (e.g., Pezzulo et al., 2010; Bläsing et al., 2014), in the medical realm (e.g., rehabilitation or for prosthetics) or for improving technical support systems (i.e., human machine interactions; Schack and Ritter, 2013).

Taken together, this review suggests that ERPs can be used reliably to investigate the neurophysiological correlates of grasping movements. The reviewed ERP studies consistently report anterior and posterior (slow) ERP amplitude modulations in line with the evidence for a parietofrontal brain network obtained with other methodologies (Grafton, 2010). As the interpretations of the ERP effects diverge and the number of ERP studies is still relatively low, more research is needed to resolve apparent inconsistencies, for example, regarding the temporal succession of parietal and frontal processes. The combination and extension of methodologies promises to be a fruitful line of action to better apprehend prehension.

Author Contributions

DK, TS, and JW conceptualized and wrote the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

This research was supported by the Cluster of Excellence: Cognitive Interaction Technology “CITEC” (EXC 277) at Bielefeld University, which is funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG).

References

Andric, M., Solodkin, A., Buccino, G., Goldin-Meadow, S., Rizzolatti, G., and Small, S. L. (2013). Brain function overlaps when people observe emblems, speech, and grasping. Neuropsychologia 51, 1619–1629. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ansuini, C., Glosa, L., Turella, L., Altoé, G., and Castiello, U. (2008). An object for an action, the same object for other actions: effects on hand shaping. Exp. Brain Res. 185, 111–119. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1136-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Archambault, P. S., Caminiti, R., and Battaglia-Mayer, A. (2009). Cortical mechanisms for online control of hand movement trajectory: the role of the posterior parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2848–2864. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armbrüster, C., and Spijkers, W. (2006). Movement planning in prehension: Do intended actions influence the initial reach of grasp movement? Motor Control 10, 311–329. doi: 10.1123/mcj.10.4.311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Astafiev, S., Shulman, G., Stanley, C., Snyder, A., Van Essen, D., and Corbetta, M. (2003). Functional organization of human intraparietal and frontal cortex for attending, looking, and pointing. J. Neurosci. 23, 4689–4699.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Begliomini, C., Wall, M. B., Smith, A. T., and Castiello, U. (2007). Differential cortical activity for precision and whole-hand visually guided grasping in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 1245–1252. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05365.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belardinelli, A., Stepper, M., and Butz, M. V. (2016). It’s in the eyes: planning precise manual actions before execution. J. Vis. 16, 18. doi: 10.1167/16.1.18

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bläsing, B., Güldenpenning, I., Koester, D., and Schack, T. (2014). Expertise affects representation structure and categorical activation of grasp postures in climbing. Front. Psychol. 5:1008. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boulenger, V., Silber, B. Y., Roy, A. C., Paulignan, Y., Jeannerod, M., and Nazir, T. A. (2008). Subliminal display of action words interferes with motor planning: a combined EEG and kinematic study. J. Physiol. 102, 130–136. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bozzacchi, C., Giusti, M. A., Pitzalis, S., Spinelli, D., and Di Russo, F. (2012a). Awareness affects motor planning for goal-oriented actions. Biol. Psychol. 89, 503–514. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.12.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bozzacchi, C., Giusti, M. A., Pitzalis, S., Spinelli, D., and Di Russo, F. (2012b). Similar cerebral motor plans for real and virtual actions. PLoS ONE 7:e47783. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burnod, Y., Baraduc, P., Battaglia-Mayer, A., Guigon, E., Koechlin, E., Ferraina, S., et al. (1999). Parieto-frontal coding of reaching: an integrated framework. Exp. Brain Res. 129, 325–346. doi: 10.1007/s002210050902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castiello, U. (2005). The neuroscience of grasping. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 726–736. doi: 10.1038/nrn1775

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castiello, U., Bennett, K. M. B., and Paulignan, Y. (1992). Does the type of prehension influence the kinematics of reaching? Behav. Brain Res. 50, 7–15. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80283-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, A. M., and Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychol. Rev. 82, 407–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Sanctis, T., Tarantino, V., Straulino, E., Begliomini, C., and Castiello, U. (2013). Co-registering kinematics and evoked related potentials during visually guided reach-to-grasp movements. PLoS ONE 8:e65508. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0065508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliott, D., Helsen, W. F., and Chua, R. (2001). A century later: Woodworth’s (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. Psychol. Bull. 127, 342–357. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.3.342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fattori, P., Raos, V., Breveglieri, R., Bosco, A., Marzocchi, N., and Galletti, C. (2010). The dorsomedial pathway is not just for reaching: grasping neurons in the medial parieto-occipital cortex of the macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. 30, 342–349. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3800-09.2010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filimon, F. (2010). Human cortical control of hand movements: parietofrontal networks for reaching, grasping, and pointing. Neuroscientist 16, 388–407. doi: 10.1177/1073858410375468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filimon, F., Nelson, J., Huang, R.-S., and Sereno, M. (2009). Multiple parietal reach regions in humans: cortical representations for visual and proprioceptive feedback during on-line reaching. J. Neurosci. 29, 2961–2971. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3211-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frömer, R., Hafner, V., and Sommer, W. (2012). Aiming for the bull’s eye: preparing for throwing investigated with event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology 49, 335–344. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01317.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galletti, C., Fattori, P., Kutz, D. F., and Gamberini, M. (1999). Brain location and visual topography of cortical area V6A in the macaque monkey. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 575–582. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00467.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galletti, C., Kutz, D., Gamberini, M., Breveglieri, R., and Fattori, P. (2003). Role of the medial parieto-occipital cortex in the control of reaching and grasping movements. Exp. Brain Res. 153, 158–170. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1589-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glover, S. (2004). Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action. Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 3–78. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04000020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glover, S., Wall, M. B., and Smith, A. T. (2012). Distinct cortical networks support the planning and online control of reaching-to-grap in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 35, 909–915. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08018.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldenberg, G., and Spatt, J. (2009). The neural basis of tool use. Brain 132, 1645–1655. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodale, M. (2011). Transforming vision into action. Vision Res. 51, 1567–1587. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grafton, S. T. (2010). The cognitive neuroscience of prehension: recent developments. Exp. Brain Res. 204, 475–491. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2315-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haggard, P. (2011). Decision time for free will. Neuron 69, 404–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, A., and Grafton, S. (2006). Goal representation in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. J. Neurosci. 26, 1133–1137. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4551-05.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hattori, N., Shibasaki, H., Wheaton, L., Wu, T., Matsuhashi, M., and Hallett, M. (2009). Discrete parieto-frontal functional connectivity related to grasping. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 1267–1282. doi: 10.1152/jn.90249.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbort, O., and Butz, M. (2010). Planning and control of hand orientation in grasping movements. Exp. Brain Res. 202, 867–878. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2191-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbort, O., and Butz, M. (2011). Habitual and goal-directed factors in (everyday) object handling. Exp. Brain Res. 213, 371–382. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2787-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hesse, C. W., and James, C. J. (2006). On semi-blind source separation using spatial constraints with applications in EEG analysis. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 53, 2525–2534. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2006.883796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M. (1981). “Intersegmental coordination during reaching at natural visual objects,” in Attention and Performance, Vol. 9, eds J. Long and A. Baddeley (Hillsdale, MI: Erlbaum), 153–168.

Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M. (1984). The timing of natural prehension movements. J. Mot. Behav. 16, 235–254. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1984.10735333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M. (1988). The Neural and Behavioral Organization of Goal-Directed Movements. Oxford: Clarendon.

Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M., Arbib, M., Rizzolatti, G., and Sakata, H. (1995). Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends Neurosci. 18, 314–320. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(95)93921-J

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelso, J. A. S. (1994). The informational character of self-organized coordination dynamics. Hum. Mov. Sci. 13, 393–413. doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(94)90047-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelso, J. A. S. (1997). Relative timing in brain and behaviour: some observations about the generalized motor program and self-organized coordination dynamics. Hum. Mov. Sci. 16, 453–460. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(96)00044-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirsch, W., and Hennighausen, E. (2010). ERP correlates of linear hand movements: distance dependent changes. Clin. Neurophysiol. 121, 1285–1292. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.02.151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knudsen, B., Henning, A., Wunsch, K., Weigelt, M., and Aschersleben, G. (2012). The end- state comfort effect in 3- to 8-year-old children in two object manipulation tasks. Front. Psychol. 3:445. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koester, D., and Schiller, N. O. (2008). Morphological priming in overt language production: electrophysiological evidence from Dutch. NeuroImage 42, 1622–1630. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kornhuber, H. H., and Deecke, L. (1965). Hirnpotentialänderungen bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale. Pflügers Arch. 284, 1–17. doi: 10.1007/BF00412364

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunde, W., Landgraf, F., Paelecke, M., and Kiesel, A. (2007). Dorsal and ventral processing under dual-task conditions. Psychol. Sci. 18, 100–104. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01855.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Künzell, S., Augste, C., Hering, M., Maier, S., Meinzinger, A., and Sießmeir, D. (2013). Optimal control in the critical phase of movement: a functional approach to motor planning processes. Acta Psychol. 143, 310–316. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutas, M., and Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event related brain potential (ERP). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 621–647. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, W., Obrig, H., Lindinger, G., Cheyne, D., and Deecke, L. (1990). Supplementary motor area activation while tapping bimanually different rhythms in musicians. Exp. Brain Res. 79, 504–514. doi: 10.1007/BF00229320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leuthold, H., and Jentzsch, I. (2001). Neural correlates of advance movement preparation and execution: a dipole source analysis approach. Cogn. Brain Res. 12, 207–224. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00052-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, G. D. (2009). “The role of memory in the control of action,” in Oxford Handbook of Human Action, eds E. Morsella, J. Bargh, and P. Gollwitzer (Oxford: Oxford University. Press), 427–441.

Google Scholar

Majdandzić, J., Grol, M. J., van Schie, H. T., Verhagen, L., Toni, I., and Bekkering, H. (2007). The role of immediate and final goals in action planning: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 37, 589–598. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marteniuk, R. G., MacKenzie, C. L., Jeannerod, M., Athenes, S., and Dugas, C. (1987). Constraints on human arm movements. Can. J. Psychol. 41, 365–378. doi: 10.1037/h0084157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meulenbroek, R. G. J., Rosenbaum, D. A., Thomassen, A. J. W. M., Schomaker, L. R. B., and Schomaker, L. (1993). Limb-segment selection in drawing behavior. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 46, 273–299. doi: 10.1080/14640749308401047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pezzulo, G., Barca, L., Bocconi, A. L., and Borghi, A. M. (2010). When affordances climb into your mind: advantages of motor simulation in a memory task performed by novice and expert rock climbers. Brain Cogn. 73, 68–73. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piefke, M., Kramer, K., Korte, M., Schulte-Rüther, M., Korte, J., Wohlschläger, A., et al. (2009). Neurofunctional modulation of brain regions by distinct forms of motor cognition and movement features. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 432–451. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proverbio, A. M., Adorni, R., and D’Aniello, G. E. (2011). 250 ms to code for action affordance during observation of manipulable objects. Neuropsychologia 49, 2711–2717. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 576–582. doi: 10.1038/nrn1706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridderinkhof, K., Van den Wildenberg, W., Segalowitz, S., and Carter, C. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: the role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain Cogn. 56, 129–140. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Camarda, R., Fogassi, L., Gentilucci, M., Luppino, G., and Matelli, M. (1988). Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 71, 491–507. doi: 10.1007/BF00248742

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., and Rozzi, S. (2014). Cortical mechanisms underlying the organization of goal-directed actions and mirror neuron-based action understanding. Physiol. Rev. 94, 655–706. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00009.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (2000). “Cortical mechanisms subserving object grasping and action recognition: a new view on the cortical motor functions,” in The Cognitive Neurosciences, ed. M. S. Gazzaniga (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 539–552.

Google Scholar

Rosenbaum, D. A., Vaughan, J., Jorgensen, M. J., and Barnes, H. J. (1992). Time course of movement planning: selection of handgrips for object manipulation. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 1058–1073.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Schack, T., and Ritter, H. (2013). Representation and learning in motor action - Bridges between experimental research and cognitive robotics. New Ideas Psychol. 31, 258–269. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2013.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seegelke, C., Hughs, C., and Schack, T. (2014). Manual (a)symmetries in grasp posture planning: a short review. Front. Psychol. 5:1480. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slobounov, S. M., Ray, W. J., and Simon, R. F. (1998). Movement-related potentials accompanying unilateral finger movements with special reference to rate of force development. Psychophysiology 35, 537–548. doi: 10.1017/S0048577298970342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smeets, J., and Brenner, E. (1999). A new view on grasping. Motor Control 3, 237–271. doi: 10.1123/mcj.3.3.237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiegel, M., Koester, D., and Schack, T. (2013). The functional role of working memory in the (re-)planning and execution of grasping movements. J. Exp. Psych. 39, 1326–1339.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Striemer, C. L., Chouinard, P. A., and Goodale, M. A. (2011). Programs for action in superior parietal cortex: a triple-pulse TMS investigation. Neuropsychologia 49, 2391–2399. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarantino, V., De Sanctis, T., Straulino, E., Begliomini, C., and Castiello, U. (2014). Object size modulates fronto-parietal activity during reaching movements. Eur. J. Neurosci. 39, 1528–1537. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ting, K. H., Fung, P. C., Chang, C. Q., and Chan, F. H. (2006). Automatic correction of artefact from single-trial event-related potentials by blind source separation using second order statistics only. Med. Eng. Physics 28, 780–794. doi: 10.1016/j.medengphy.2005.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker, M., and Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24, 830–846. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.830

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Wel, R. P., Fleckenstein, R. M., Jax, S. A., and Rosenbaum, D. A. (2007). Hand path priming in manual obstacle avoidance: evidence for abstract spatiotemporal forms in human motor control. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 33, 1117–1126.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

van Elk, M., Van Schie, H. T., and Bekkering, H. (2008). Semantics in action: an electrophysiological study on the use of semantic knowledge for action. J. Physiol. 102, 95–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Elk, M., Viswanathan, S., Van Schie, H. T., Bekkering, H., and Grafton, S. T. (2012). Pouring or chilling a bottle of wine: an fMRI study on the prospective planning of object-directed actions. Exp. Brain Res. 218, 189–200. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3016-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Schie, H. T., and Bekkering, H. (2007). Neural mechanisms underlying immediate and final action goals in object use reflected by slow wave brain potentials. Brain Res. 1148, 183–197. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verleger, R., Adam, S., Rose, M., Vollmer, C., Wauschkuhn, B., and Kömpf, D. (2003). Control of hand movements after striatocapsular stroke: high-resolution temporal analysis of the function of ipsilateral activation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 1468–1476. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00125-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verleger, R., Vollmer, C., Wauschkuhn, B., Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., and Wascher, E. (2000). Dimensional overlap between arrows as cueing stimuli and responses?: evidence from contra-ipsilateral differences in EEG potentials. Cogn. Brain Res. 10, 99–109. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00032-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verleger, R., Wascher, E., Wauschkuhn, B., Jaśkowski, P., Allouni, B., Trillenberg, P., et al. (1999). Consequences of altered cerebellar input for the cortical regulation of motor coordination, as reflected in EEG potentials. Exp. Brain Res. 127, 409–422. doi: 10.1007/s002210050809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vesia, M., Bolton, D. A., Mochizuki, G., and Staines, W. R. (2013). Human parietal and primary motor cortical interactions are selectively modulated during the transport and grip formation of goal-directed hand actions. Neuropsychologia 51, 410–417. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wessel, K., Verleger, R., Nazarenus, D., Vieregge, P., and Kömpf, D. (1994). Movement-related cortical potentials preceding sequential and goal-directed finger and arm movements in patients with cerebellar atrophy. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 92, 331–341. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(94)90101-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westerholz, J., Schack, T., and Koester, D. (2013). Event-related brain potentials for goal-related power grips. PLoS ONE 8:e68501. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westerholz, J., Schack, T., and Koester, D. (2014a). The what-decision in manual action: ERPs for free choice versus specified overt goal-related grasping. Neurosci. Lett. 575, 85–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westerholz, J., Schack, T., Schütz, C., and Koester, D. (2014b). Habitual vs non-habitual manual actions: an ERP study on overt movement execution. PLoS ONE 9:e93116. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0093116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheaton, L., Fridman, E., Bohlalter, S., Vorbach, S., and Hallett, M. (2009). Left parietal activation related to planning, executing and suppressing praxis hand movements. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 980–986. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.02.161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheaton, L., Shibasaki, H., and Hallett, M. (2005a). Temporal activation pattern of parietal and premotor areas related to praxis movements. Clin. Neurophysiol. 116, 1201–1212. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheaton, L., Yakota, S., and Hallett, M. (2005b). Posterior parietal negativity preceding self-paced praxis movements. Exp. Brain Res. 163, 535–539. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2314-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpert, D. M., and Ghahramani, Z. (2000). Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1212–1217. doi: 10.1038/81497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodworth, R. S. (1899). The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychol. Rev. 3, 1–119.

Google Scholar

Zaepffel, M., and Brochier, T. (2012). Planning of visually guided reach-to-grasp movements: inference from reaction time and contingent negative variation (CNV). Psychophysiologiy 49, 17–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01277.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Wininger, M., and Rosenbaum, D. A. (2014). Word generation affects continuous hand movements. J. Mot. Behav. 46, 115–123. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2013.872079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: EEG, event-related potentials, manual action, cognition

Citation: Koester D, Schack T and Westerholz J (2016) Neurophysiology of Grasping Actions: Evidence from ERPs. Front. Psychol. 7:1996. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01996

Received: 27 July 2016; Accepted: 08 December 2016;
Published: 22 December 2016.

Edited by:

Scott Glover, Royal Holloway, University of London, UK

Reviewed by:

Rolf Verleger, University of Lübeck, Germany
Umberto Castiello, University of Padua, Italy

Copyright © 2016 Koester, Schack and Westerholz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Dirk Koester, dkoester@cit-ec.uni-bielefeld.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.