Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 May 2018
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

Long-Term Efficacy of Psychosocial Treatments for Adults With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Meta-Analytic Review

  • 1Department of Psychobiology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
  • 2Department of Basic Psychology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Loyola, Andalucía, Spain
  • 4Department of Basic Psychology and Methodology, University of Murcia, Murcia, Spain

Background: Recent evidence suggests that psychosocial treatments, particularly cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT), are effective interventions for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The objective of this review was to determine the long-term efficacy of psychosocial interventions in improving clinically relevant variables, including ADHD core symptoms, clinical global impression (CGI), and global functioning.

Methods: In total, nine randomized controlled trials and three uncontrolled single-group pretest-posttest studies were included. The data from these studies were combined using the inverse variance method. Heterogeneity and risk of bias were assessed. Subgroup analyses and meta-regressions were performed, to determine the influence of different potential moderator variables (risk of bias, medication status, follow-up length, therapy type and setting, and control group type) on effect size (ES) estimates.

Results: Up to 680 of a total of 1,073 participants assessed pre-treatment were retained at follow-up. Treatment groups showed greater improvement than control groups in self-reported total ADHD symptoms, inattention, and hyperactivity/impulsivity, in addition to CGI and global functioning. Blind assessors also reported a large ES in within-subject outcomes. Studies using dialectical behavioral therapy (DBT) in a group setting, with active control matching, and that were rated as having an unclear risk of bias, achieved significantly lower ES estimates for most outcomes. Treatment effectiveness, according to the CGI measure, and global functioning were significantly increased when the percentage of medicated participants was greater.

Conclusions: Our results indicate that the post-treatment gains reported in previous reviews are sustained for at least 12 months. Nevertheless, these results must be interpreted with caution, because of a high level of heterogeneity among studies and the risk of bias observed in the majority of outcomes. Thus, these findings indicate that psychological interventions are a highly valuable and stable clinical tool for the treatment of core symptoms and global functioning in adults with ADHD.

Introduction

Rationale

ADHD is a childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by developmentally inappropriate levels of inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity (American Psychiatric Association., 2013). The disorder affects 2.5–2.8% of the adult population (Simon et al., 2009; Fayyad et al., 2016), and is associated with significant impairment in academic (Kuriyan et al., 2013; Voigt et al., 2017), health (Nigg, 2013; Brevik et al., 2017), occupational (Kirino et al., 2015; Hechtman et al., 2016), and social (Das et al., 2012) domains. It is also related to the development of other comorbid conditions such as learning disorders (Knouse et al., 2012; Duda et al., 2015); oppositional defiant disorder (Reimherr et al., 2013); anxiety disorders (Cadman et al., 2016); substance use disorder (Capusan et al., 2016); and borderline personality disorder (Matthies and Philipsen, 2014), among others. In addition, the risk of suicide is significantly higher in adults with ADHD (Barbaresi et al., 2013), even after controlling for other comorbid disorders (Stickley et al., 2016). Thus, the need to treat ADHD is evidenced by the significant impact that the disorder has on the different areas of adult life. Pharmacotherapy is the first-line treatment for adults with ADHD with either moderate or severe level of impairment (National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2008). Stimulant drugs exhibit a moderate-to-large effect size (ES) (standardized mean difference [SMD] = 0.72) on ADHD symptoms (Castells et al., 2011; Epstein et al., 2014), while for non-stimulant drugs (e.g., atomoxetine), ES is low-to-moderate (SMD = 0.39) (Faraone and Glatt, 2010). Additionally, pharmacotherapy has long-term beneficial effects (Fredriksen and Peleikis, 2016). Nevertheless, drug therapy has significant limitations, since it is often associated with adverse effects and a high dropout rate (Cunill et al., 2016), and many individuals only exhibit partial responses (Wilens et al., 2002). Moreover, a combination of both psychosocial and medication treatment has proven more effective than drugs alone (Safren et al., 2005; Emilsson et al., 2011; Young et al., 2015), and is also associated with improved treatment adherence (Cunill et al., 2016).

Although they share therapeutic components, different psychosocial approaches have been designed or adapted for the treatment of adult ADHD, including: (1) Cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT), which aims to develop behavioral strategies to compensate for core neuropsychiatric deficits and to change dysfunctional thinking styles (Safren et al., 2004); (2) Dialectical behavioral therapy (DBT), which is a CBT-based approach initially developed by Linehan (1993) for the treatment of borderline personality disorder, and was later modified to address the specific needs of adult ADHD. DBT aims to both promote the acceptance and validation of ADHD-related symptoms, and teach the skills required for change and self-management (Hesslinger et al., 2002); (3) Mindfulness meditation training, which is a type of meditative technique that emphasizes a compassionate and non-reactive attitude toward one's thoughts, emotions, and body state (Zylowska et al., 2008), and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT), which is a combination of CBT and mindfulness; (4) Cognitive training (CT), which is premised on the notion that the key brain networks involved in ADHD can be strengthened, and the cognitive processes they subserve improved, through controlled exposure to information processing tasks (Vinogradov et al., 2012); and (5) Neurofeedback (NFB), which is a variant of EEG biofeedback that aims to promote self-regulation of specific brain activity patterns in an operant conditioning paradigm (Hammond, 2007).

Previously meta-analytic reviews have found that psychosocial interventions are effective at the end of the treatment, with moderate-to-large ES estimates on inattention and total ADHD symptoms and also on clinical global impression (CGI) and global functioning, which were reduced to small-to-moderate ES estimates for comparisons with active control groups, and only increased to large when within-subject data were analyzed, with small-to-moderate effects on hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms (Linderkamp and Lauth, 2011; Cairncross and Miller, 2016; Jensen et al., 2016; Young et al., 2016; Knouse et al., 2017) (Table 1). Their results also varied depending on the source of information, since some reviews suggested that no significant effect was achieved (Jensen et al., 2016), while others found a moderate ES, according to blind evaluators (Knouse et al., 2017). In conclusion, psychosocial interventions have been found to improve ADHD symptoms and other clinically relevant variables in adults with ADHD at the end of treatment. However, none of these reviews examined whether the gains were maintained months after the end of treatment. In a disorder that tends to be chronic such as ADHD, the stability of improvements is one of the key features of an intervention, since the psychotherapy is aimed at the long-term modification of certain pathological behaviors and thoughts that cause nuclear symptoms to generate a greater impairment. Likewise, the efficacy of different therapy options (e.g., CBT, DBT, MBCT, etc.) have not been compared and several recently published significant studies have not been included in some of these reviews.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of characteristics of previous meta-analytic reviews.

A number of studies have evaluated the efficacy of such interventions in the long-term, and demonstrated that it is maintained from 3 to 12 months after the end of the treatment (Safren et al., 2010; Salakari et al., 2010; Emilsson et al., 2011; Pettersson et al., 2014; Fleming et al., 2015; Salomone et al., 2015; Young et al., 2015; Cherkasova et al., 2016; Gu et al., 2017; Nasri et al., 2017), although the magnitudes of the reported effects are heterogeneous. The largest clinical trial so far published in the field found that groups receiving psychosocial therapy had superior outcomes to active control groups at follow-up only in the CGI measure, but not in ADHD symptoms (Philipsen et al., 2015). Other studies have also reported the maintenance of therapeutic achievements, according to the CGI measure (Safren et al., 2010; Young et al., 2015). Moreover, improvements in global functioning are also maintained, according to some studies (Emilsson et al., 2011; Fleming et al., 2015; Young et al., 2015; Morgensterns et al., 2016), but not others (Pettersson et al., 2014). To date, no meta-analytical review of the long-term efficacy of psychosocial treatment in adults with ADHD has been performed.

Objectives

The main purpose of this review was to investigate if the observed post-treatment efficacy of psychological interventions on the nuclear symptoms of adults with ADHD was maintained from 3 months onwards after their termination. Similarly, the study aimed to ascertain if the post-treatment gains in CGI and global functioning were also sustained. Finally, we sought to explore how different variables (e.g., outcome measure source, within-study risk of bias, therapy type and setting, control group type, medication status, and follow-up length) moderate ES estimates for each outcome.

Research Questions

This study aims to answer the following research questions:

A) Are therapeutic gains from the psychosocial treatments maintained at follow-up in adults with ADHD?

B) To what extent do variables, such as therapeutic approach, medication status, or type of control group (among others), influence the maintenance of achievements?

Methods

This review adheres to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Liberati et al., 2009). A detailed checklist can be seen in the Supplementary Table 1.

Study Design

Randomized controlled trials (RCTs) and uncontrolled single-group pretest-posttest studies were included in the analysis. Although uncontrolled single-group pretest-posttest studies raise issues of internal validity, some authors in the meta-analytic area advocate their use in systematic reviews when there are few RCTs in a given field. For example, Hunter and Schmidt (2004) and Petticrew and Roberts (2005) supported the use of within-group designs in a meta-analysis when there are ethical or other reasons that hinder or prevent the use of control groups.

Participants, Interventions, Comparators, and Outcomes Included

The following eligibility criteria were used in this review, detailed using the PICO approach (O'Connor et al., 2011).

Participants

Participants were required to meet the DSM-IV (4th ed., text. rev., American Psychiatric Association., 2000) or DSM-5 (5th ed., American Psychiatric Association., 2013) criteria for ADHD in adults. The studies should specify a detailed protocol of the diagnostic assessment. All participants had to be over 18 years old. Studies using a sample in which participants presented psychotic disorders, bipolar disorder, severe active addictions at the time of treatment, or clinically significant personality disorders (Axis II) were excluded; however, the existence of other Axis I disorders (mood disorders, anxiety disorders, etc.) was not a reason for exclusion.

Intervention

Studies in which at least one of the experimental groups received a psychosocial treatment specifically designed for ADHD, either supported by pharmacotherapy or not, were included. Such treatment had to be adequately described and detailed.

Comparisons

For the between-groups outcomes, studies in which at least one of the groups (control group) did not receive a specific psychotherapy for ADHD were included. The control group could be: (1) An active control group, in which participants received support from a therapist, in a group or individual non-directive sessions in which no specific strategy was discussed, or even non-specific interventions for ADHD such as relaxation training; (2) A treatment as usual (TAU) group, in which all participants received the usual treatment, which included both pharmacological and potentially also some non-pharmacological treatments; (3) A waiting-list group, in which participants were waiting without receiving any psychosocial treatment, although some, but not all, participants received pharmacotherapy.

Outcomes

Studies were required to perform a follow-up assessment at least 3 months after the end of the treatment, which had to include an ADHD symptoms severity scale, since this was used as the primary outcome measure for our review. CGI and global functioning were secondary outcomes. CGI (National Institute of Mental Health, 1985) is a three-item observer-rated scale that measures illness severity, global improvement or change, and therapeutic response. It has been proven as a robust measure of efficacy in clinical trials; therefore, it has been widely used in many treatment evaluation studies. The global functioning outcome provides a measure of the impact that symptoms have on daily functioning in the vital domains (e.g., social, familial, work, personal, and academic, among others). Instruments, such as the Sheenan Disability Scale (Sheenan et al., 1996) or the RATE-S (Young and Ross, 2007) were included in that outcome category.

Systematic Review Protocol

In accomplishing this systematic review, no previous protocol was carried out.

Search Strategy

A systematic literature search was performed in MEDLINE (via PubMed) and Scopus. No date limit was established. The last search was conducted on September 4, 2017. The search terms used are detailed in Supplementary Table 2. In addition, reference lists of retrieved relevant articles were screened.

Data Sources, Study Selection, and Data Extraction

The selection process is illustrated in Figure 1. After removing duplicates, the first author conducted a screening search of relevant articles, based on reading the title and abstract. Then, the two first authors independently applied the eligibility criteria to the methods section of the articles, which were previously masked by a third researcher. Agreement between the two researchers was acceptable (κ = 0.81). Inconsistencies between the assessors were solved by consensus. The final selection of the studies was based on reading the full-text articles.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Selection process diagram.

Data were extracted and coded in a spreadsheet so that all study characteristic items described in the Cochrane Handbook were covered (Higgins and Green, 2011). This sheet was pilot-tested with five studies, to improve its fit to the sample characteristics. Data extraction and coding were performed independently by the two first authors. The codebook can be requested from the corresponding author. Kappa coefficients of inter-rater agreement were excellent for qualitative moderator variables (average κ = 0.88; range = 0.75–0.93), as well as for continuous moderator variables (average intra-class correlation r = 0.95; range = 0.91–1.0). Disagreements were resolved by consensus. Four authors were contacted to request additional information. All of them responded, and data were provided by three of them. To reduce bias, data in included studies were searched for duplicates.

The following data were extracted from each study (Table 2): (1) methodological characteristics: study design, sample size at every assessment point, and follow-up length; (2) participant characteristics: percentage of participants receiving ADHD medication; (3) intervention characteristics: type of therapy, therapy setting, and number of sessions; (4) comparison group characteristics: control group type; (5) outcomes: total ADHD symptoms, inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms, CGI, and global functioning; and (6) outcome characteristics: measure source (self-rated or blind assessor-rated) for all outcomes.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of characteristics of included studies.

Data Analysis

For data analysis, Review Manager software (version 5.3) from the Cochrane Collaboration and Comprehensive Meta Analysis (version 3.3.070) were used. Long-term reduction in the severity of inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, and total ADHD symptoms were considered the primary outcomes, while CGI and global functioning were secondary outcomes. For the between-group (psychosocial treatments vs. control groups) outcomes, the effect size index was defined as the difference between the average pretest-follow-up change of the experimental and control groups, divided by a pooled estimate of the pretest standard deviations of the two groups. In addition, a correction factor for small samples sizes was also applied (Morris, 2008; see Supplementary Figure 1). For within-subject (pretest to follow-up) single-group studies, the effect size was defined as the average pretest-follow-up change, divided by the pretest standard deviation, and with a correction factor for small sample sizes (Morris, 2000; see Supplementary Figure 2). For estimating the variances of both effect size indices, the Pearson correlation coefficient between the pretest and follow-up measures must be available. As this figure was not reported in the studies, a value of 0.70 was assumed for r, as recommended by Rosenthal (1991). Following the rule of thumb suggested by Cohen (1988), ES values of 0.20, 0.50, and 0.80 were considered small, moderate, and large, respectively.

The results of individual studies, weighted by their inverse variance, were combined for each outcome. A random-effects model was chosen because of a suspicion of a high heterogeneity between the studies. A 95% confidence interval (CI) was calculated for each outcome. The consistency of effect sizes was assessed using the I2 index (Higgins et al., 2003), which describes the percentage of total variation across studies that is due to heterogeneity, rather than chance. I2 values of 25, 50, and 75% can be interpreted as reflecting low, moderate, and high heterogeneity, respectively (Higgins et al., 2003).

To determine the internal validity of each study, the risk of bias was assessed by the first author, covering the items described in The Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias (Higgins et al., 2011): (a) the adequacy of randomization and concealment of allocation (selection bias), where a comparison group was available; (b) the blinding of the outcome assessors (blinding of the therapists could not be assessed in studies that evaluated psychosocial treatments); (c) the incomplete outcome data (attrition bias); (d) the selective reporting of the outcomes (reporting bias); and (e) the medication stabilization (other sources of bias). This assessment was supervised by the second author and discrepancies were resolved by consensus. Studies were not excluded based on the result of the evaluation of the risk of bias, but they were divided into subgroups (high, unclear, or low risk of bias) and sensitivity analyses were performed to determine the influence of this variable on ES estimates.

Publication bias was assessed by visually examining the asymmetry in the funnel plots of each outcome and conducting the trim-and-fill method (Duval and Tweedie, 2000). This test trims the asymmetric studies from the right-hand side to locate the unbiased effect (in an iterative procedure), and then fills the plot by re-inserting the trimmed studies on the right, as well as including their imputed counterparts to the left of the mean effect. In addition, the Egger test (Egger et al., 1997) for testing the asymmetry of funnel plots was applied. This test assesses bias using the precision of each ES (the inverse of the standard error) to predict the standardized effect (ES divided by the standard error). Finally, the Fail-safe N (Rosenthal, 1979) was also calculated, which is the number of additional “negative” studies (with a null effect) that would be needed to increase the P-value for the meta-analysis to above 0.05.

Sub-group analyses (chi-square tests) were performed for each outcome, to assess the impact of the following categorical variables on ES estimates: (i) risk of bias in individual studies; (ii) therapy type; (iii) therapy setting; (iv) outcome measure source; (v) and control group type. Additionally, a meta-regression was carried out for each outcome with continuous variables, such as the percentage of participants in the experimental group under pharmacological treatment and the follow-up length, to ascertain the extent to which they predicted the ES.

Results

Study Selection

From 236 records, nine RCTs and three uncontrolled single-group pretest-posttest studies were identified and included in the quantitative review, based on the reading of full-text reports (Figure 1). Two RCTs were excluded from between-groups, but included in within-subject meta-analyses, because the control group was not assessed at follow-up (Pettersson et al., 2014) and all groups received specific ADHD psychotherapy (Cherkasova et al., 2016). Thus, finally, seven and 12 studies were included in the between-groups and within-subject meta-analyses, respectively. The characteristics of the included studies are presented in Table 2, and a list of excluded studies and the reasons for exclusion are detailed in Supplementary Table 3. For the study by Philipsen et al. (2015), each treatment group was compared with the equivalent control group, based on medication status.

Participants

Up to 680 of 1,073 participants assessed pre-treatment were retained at follow-up. On average, 50.72% of participants were male, age was 34.41 years, and 63.70% of participants were taking medication for ADHD during the treatment.

Intervention

Half of the treatment groups underwent CBT (50%) and 25% DBT; while MBCT, Biofeedback (BFB), or a combination of CBT and DBT were applied for 8.33%. On average, 11.42 sessions were conducted. Group and individual treatments were delivered in 41.66 and 25% of studies, respectively, while both types of treatment were combined in 33.33%.

Comparison

An active control group was used in 42.92% of the studies, while 28.64% compared the intervention to a TAU or waitlist group.

Outcomes

Blinded assessors of the primary outcome measure were used in 33.33% of studies, while the remainder used only self-reported measures.

Synthesized Findings

All Treatment vs. All Control Groups

Combining all treatment groups, significant differences were found between the subjects that received a psychosocial intervention and those in the combined control groups in self-reported total ADHD symptoms [χ(1)2 = 7.95, p < 0.01], inattention [χ(1)2 = 6.34, p = 0.01], and hyperactivity/impulsivity [χ(1)2 = 10.90, p < 0.01], as well as in CGI [χ(1)2 = 5.66, p = 0.02], and global functioning [χ(1)2 = 5.60, p = 0.02] outcomes, in favor of the treated groups (Figure 2). Although the treated groups obtained a higher ES, differences in blind assessor rated total ADHD symptoms [χ(1)2 = 2.05, p = 0.15], inattention [χ(1)2 = 0.01, p = 0.91], and hyperactivity/impulsivity [χ(1)2 = 2.37, p = 0.12] outcomes were not significant (Figure 3).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Forest plot for all treatment and control groups on self-reported total ADHD symptoms outcome.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Forest plot for all treatment and control groups on blind assessor-reported total ADHD symptoms outcome.

Between-Group Outcomes

Taking into account only data from the RCTs, treatment groups showed greater improvement than control groups in self-reported total ADHD symptoms (SMD = 0.71; 95% CI [0.22–1.21]), inattention (SMD = 0.64; 95% CI [0.23–1.01]), and hyperactivity/impulsivity (SMD = 0.66; 95% CI [0.18–1.14]) outcomes, for which ES estimates were medium-to-large (Table 3; Figures 46). In contrast, blind assessors reported small-to-moderate ES on total ADHD symptoms (SMD = 0.40; 95% CI [−0.06 to 0.85]), and hyperactivity/impulsivity (SMD = 0.28; 95% CI [−0.13 to 0.70]), and a small ES on inattention (SMD = 0.14; 95% CI [−0.29 to 0.58]) outcomes, but with confidence intervals including zero. Treatment efficacy measured by CGI was small-to-moderate (SMD = 0.44; 95% [0.14–0.74]) (Figure 7). Finally, a moderate-to-large ES (SMD = 0.76; 95% [0.23–1.28]) was achieved for self-reported global functioning (Figure 8). High heterogeneity was observed for all outcomes (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Standardized mean differences (SMD), 95% confidence intervals, heterogeneity analyses, and risk of bias for between-groups and within-subject outcomes.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Forest and funnel plots for between-groups total ADHD symptoms outcome.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Forest and funnel plots for between-groups inattention symptoms outcome.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Forest and funnel plots for between-groups hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms outcome.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Forest plot for between-groups CGI outcome.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Forest plot for between-groups global functioning outcome.

Within-Subject Outcomes

The within-subject treatment ES estimates on all ADHD symptom outcomes were large, both for self-reported (SMD = 1.09; 95% CI [0.85–1.32] for total ADHD symptoms) and blind assessed (SMD = 1.18; 95% CI [0.90–1.46] for total ADHD symptoms) measures, except for hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms as reported by blind assessors (SMD = 0.67; 95% CI [0.49–0.85]) (Table 3, Figures 911). The ES on CGI outcome was also large (SMD = 1.20; 95% CI [0.93–1.48]) (Figure 12), while that on global functioning was moderate-to-large (SMD = 0.58; 95% CI [0.25–0.92]) (Figure 13). I2 indices indicated greater homogeneity than for the between-groups outcomes (Table 3). Heterogeneity was zero and low for the blind assessor-rated inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity outcomes, respectively, and high for self-rated total ADHD symptoms and global functioning, whereas it was moderate for the remaining outcomes.

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Forest and funnel plots for within-subject total ADHD symptoms outcome.

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. Forest and funnel plots for within-subject inattention symptoms outcome.

FIGURE 11
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 11. Forest and funnel plots for within-subject hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms outcome.

FIGURE 12
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 12. Forest plot for within-subject CGI outcome.

FIGURE 13
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 13. Forest plot for within-subject global functioning outcome.

Risk of Bias

All self-rated outcomes were rated with a high risk of bias, while the risk of bias summary for the blind assessor-rated outcomes was classified as unclear (Supplementary Tables 4–8). Only one study was assessed to have a low risk of bias for all outcomes, except for global functioning.

Publication bias indicators were identified for the between-groups outcomes. The results of the Egger's test were significant or marginally significant for total ADHD symptoms, inattention, and hyperactivity/impulsivity outcomes (Supplementary Table 9). Use of the Trim and Fill method decreased the confidence interval to below zero after trimming some studies in the self-reported and blind assessor-rated total ADHD symptoms and self-reported inattention outcomes. In contrast, between-groups CGI and global functioning outcomes are likely robust to publication bias. Similarly, fail-safe N results for self-reported total ADHD symptoms, inattention, and hyperactivity/impulsivity outcomes indicated that a number of studies between six and eight times higher than those included would be necessary for the estimated effect to be null.

Regarding within-subject outcomes, although the results of the Egger's test were significant for blind assessor-rated total ADHD symptoms, inattention, and hyperactivity/impulsivity outcomes, the confidence interval of the Trim and Fill method adjusted estimates remained above zero (Supplementary Table 10). In addition, the fail-safe N for those outcomes ranged from 42 to 157. No indicators of publication bias were apparent for self-reported total ADHD symptoms, inattention, or hyperactivity/impulsivity, or for CGI or global functioning outcomes.

Moderator Analyses

Risk of Bias

Significant differences were found for between-groups total ADHD symptoms [χ(2)2 = 11.74, p < 0.01], inattention [χ(2)2 = 31.22, p < 0.01], and hyperactivity/impulsivity [χ(2)2 = 31.40, p = 0.01] (Supplementary Table 11), as well as for within-subject inattention [χ(2)2 = 8.23, p = 0.02] (Supplementary Table 12). ES estimates were significantly lower for studies rated as having an unclear risk of bias for all of these outcomes.

Therapy

DBT studies achieved significantly, or marginally significantly, lower ES estimates on between-groups total ADHD symptoms [χ2(3) = 16.47, p < 0.01], inattention [χ2(3) = 8, p = 0.05], and hyperactivity/impulsivity [χ2(3) = 32.96, p < 0.01] (Supplementary Table 11), on within-subject total ADHD symptoms [χ2(3) = 7.42, p = 0.06] (see Supplementary Table 12), and on between-groups CGI [χ2(1) = 6.87, p < 0.01] and global functioning [χ2(1) = 5.36, p = 0.02]. Biofeedback studies generated significantly lower ES estimates on within-subject hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms [χ2(3) = 17.10, p < 0.01].

Treatment Setting

ES estimates were significantly, or marginally significantly, lower in studies using a group treatment setting on between-groups and within-subject total ADHD symptoms [χ2(2) = 36.68, p < 0.01 and χ2(2) = 15.83, p < 0.01, respectively], inattention [χ2(2) = 29.21, p < 0.01 and χ2(2) = 5.83, p = 0.05, respectively], and hyperactivity/impulsivity [χ2(2) = 24.83, p < 0.01 and χ2(2) = 5.75, p = 0.06, respectively] (Supplementary Tables 11, 12), as well as on between-groups CGI [χ2(2) = 6.90, p = 0.03] and within-subject global functioning [χ2(1) = 8.92, p < 0.01] outcomes, while the ES on the within-subject CGI outcome was significantly higher than that for the individual setting [χ2(2) = 6.25, p = 0.04].

Measure Source

A significantly lower ES on the within-subject inattention outcome was found for blind assessor measurement [χ2(1) = 3.88, p = 0.05], while no significant differences were found for the remaining outcomes (Supplementary Tables 11, 12).

Control Group

Active control-matched studies generated a significantly, or marginally significantly, lower ES on total ADHD symptoms [χ2(2) = 5.71, p = 0.06], inattention [χ2(2) = 11.61, p < 0.01], and hyperactivity/impulsivity [χ2(2) = 16.46, p < 0.01], as well as for global functioning [χ2(1) = 5.36, p = 0.02] (Supplementary Table 11).

Follow-Up Length

None of the meta-regressions were significant with respect to this variable, with a confidence level of 95% (Supplementary Tables 13, 14).

Percentage of Participants on Medication

A higher percentage of participants on medication predicted higher ES on between-groups and within-subject CGI (Coefficient = 0.01, 95% CI [0.00–0.01], p = 0.02; Coefficient = 0.01, 95% CI [0.00–0.01], p = 0.01) and on within-subject global functioning and total ADHD symptoms (Coefficient = 0.02, 95% CI [0.00–0.04], p = 0.01; Coefficient = 0.01, 95% CI [0.00 to 0.001]), while no significant meta-regressions were found for the remaining outcomes (Supplementary Tables 13, 14).

Discussion

Summary of Main Findings

Psychosocial treatments have exhibited post-treatment efficacy for both core and other symptoms in adults with ADHD (Cairncross and Miller, 2016; Jensen et al., 2016; Young et al., 2016; Knouse et al., 2017); however, no previous studies have systematically explored whether therapeutic improvements are maintained at follow-up assessment. In this study, a meta-analytic review was conducted to determine if treatment gains were sustained between 3 and 12 months after the end of treatment, for both core ADHD symptoms and other clinically relevant measures, and to what extent different moderator variables influence this maintenance.

Our results indicate that self-reported post-treatment gains were effectively sustained for at least 12 months. Inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, and total ADHD symptoms, as well as global functioning, were significant improved in treated compared with control groups, as reported by participants, with medium-to-large ES estimates. Improvements in CGI measure were also maintained. These results support the validity of those obtained in previous post-treatment meta-analyses (Cairncross and Miller, 2016; Jensen et al., 2016; Young et al., 2016; Knouse et al., 2017). Nevertheless, according to blind assessors, between-group improvements in ADHD symptoms reported by previous meta-analytic reviews did not persevere over time.

On the other hand, the results provide further empirical support for the within-subject improvement. This study found that the post-treatment ES estimates on within-subject CGI, total ADHD symptoms, and inattention reported by Knouse et al. (2017) remained large at follow-up, for both blinded and self-reported measures, while the ES on global functioning continued to be moderate-to-large. One interesting finding is that the self-reported within-subject treatment effects on hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms increased from moderate-to-large to large at follow-up.

Concerning the comparison between the different therapeutic options, DBT and Biofeedback are not as effective as CBT on the key outcome measures. Although MBCT reached a large ES on all ADHD symptom outcomes, evidence came only from one study. In addition, when CBT studies were isolated, the ES estimates for between-groups blind assessor-rated total ADHD symptoms became significant (SMD = 0.76; 95% CI [0.45–1.06]) and those for CGI increased to moderate-to-large (SMD = 0.72; 95% CI [0.44–0.99]). Furthermore, the majority of studies for which data were included for most outcomes used CBT. All these findings suggest that CBT could be the psychosocial intervention with the most long-term empirical support for the treatment of ADHD in adults. CBT is based on the premise that underlying neurobiological impairments hinder adults with ADHD from acquiring and using adaptive compensatory strategies (i.e., use of higher-level organization and planning strategies), which maintains and exacerbates the core symptoms and further contributes to a chronic functional impairment persisting since childhood (Knouse and Safren, 2010). That impairment, together with a negative social feedback, can lead to the development of maladaptive negative cognitions and beliefs that decrease motivation and increase avoidance behavior and mood disturbance, thus reinforcing the cycle. This CBT model of ADHD was supported by recent research findings, which found that adult ADHD is significantly related to dysfunctional cognitions, cognitive distortions and maladaptive coping strategies of escape-avoidance (Mitchell et al., 2013; Torrente et al., 2014; Strohmeier et al., 2016). Thus, CBT is aimed at the acquisition and especially the maintenance of compensatory skills, and also at the development of cognitive strategies to challenge the cognitive distortions, so that core neurobiological deficits do not translate as frequently into functional impairments (Safren et al., 2004). Therefore, a possible explanation for the stability of the improvements found on the CBT subset of studies might be that individuals with ADHD were able to learn and integrate the compensatory behavioral skills and the cognitive strategies into daily life, so that the changes were sustained throughout the time, despite the fact that treatment had ended.

Another notable finding from the moderator analyses was that, when compared, the individual setting was more effective than group treatment on the main outcome measures. This result could be explained by the fact that individual treatment is better suited to the specific needs of each individual, and that probably each participant received more attention from the therapist than in a group setting, which might increase the effectiveness of the treatment.

With respect to the source of the measures, on the one hand, only self-reported ADHD core symptom outcomes showed improvements, while blinded assessment did not, when treatment groups were compared to control groups. This finding could be an indicator of the presence of a significant placebo effect on control groups, which obtained a significant ES on the main outcome measures. On the other hand, some other significant results supporting blind-reported efficacy were obtained. First, CGI is a blinded measure, and between-groups treatment effect was small-to-moderate as measured by this instrument, while within-subject change was large. Second, a large effect was reached on within-subject blinded ADHD symptom outcomes. Third, moderator analyses only detected significant differences on the basis of the measure source (blinded vs. self-reported) on the within-subject inattention outcome, while the other comparisons were not significant. Forth, several individual studies found a significant long-term effect as reported by blind assessors (Safren et al., 2010; Emilsson et al., 2011; Young et al., 2015).

Meta-regression results indicate that follow-up length (from 3 to 12 months) does not predict treatment efficacy, which further supports the stability of the gains. In addition, the results indicate that medication is a factor that influences treatment effectiveness, according to the CGI measure, which increased when the percentage of medicated participants was greater, supporting the conclusions of several previous RCTs (Philipsen et al., 2015; Cherkasova et al., 2016). This finding offers empirical support for the combination of psychotherapy, particularly CBT, and pharmacotherapy as the most effective treatment option for adults with ADHD.

Studies using DBT, in a group setting, with active control-matching, and that were rated with an unclear risk of bias, achieved significantly lower ES in the majority of outcomes. This finding could be caused mainly by the study by Philipsen et al. (2015), which had considerable weight in the meta-analyses due to its large sample size since it was present in all these subgroups.

Thus, our findings indicate that there is self-reported evidence that the psychosocial interventions, particularly CBT in an individual setting, specifically improve ADHD core symptoms and global functioning until at least a year after the end of treatment, in comparison with control groups. Additionally, within-subject improvements are also maintained, even according to blind evaluators. These long-term gains further support the usefulness of psychosocial treatments for addressing adult ADHD. Nevertheless, our results must be interpreted with caution because of the high heterogeneity observed in the majority of the outcomes.

Limitations and Future Research

The limitations of the present review require consideration to ensure appropriate interpretation of the findings. First, the statistical power and moderator analyses are limited by the small number of available studies. Second, a high risk of bias was determined for many of the outcomes. Self-reported outcome measures, the use of unblinded outcome assessors, lack of accuracy in the description of dropouts (and the reasons for these), and allocation/randomization processes were the main sources of bias. Third, we identified indicators of publication bias for some outcomes. The results of the Egger test were significant for all between-group and blind assessor-rated within-subject outcomes, and the trim and fill method reduced the confidence intervals of ES estimates for self-reported between-groups outcomes to below zero. Nevertheless, the capacity to detect bias is limited when meta-analyses are based on a limited number of small trials; therefore, the results from these analyses should be treated with considerable caution (Egger et al., 1997). For example, when there are fewer than 10 studies the power of Egger's test is insufficient to distinguish chance from genuine asymmetry (Higgins and Green, 2011). Fourth, the generalization of the findings is limited by the high levels of heterogeneity among the studies included. Fifth, the small number of MBCT and biofeedback studies included in this review (only one of each type of study) limits the interpretation of the results concerning these therapies. Sixth, the high level of attrition observed (36.6%) is a concern, since greater than 20% loss potentially threatens study validity (Schulz and Grimes, 2002). Seventh, it should be noted that the results of the moderator analyses must be interpreted with extreme caution because of the small sample size.

The use of larger samples, multi-center studies, specification of the pharmacological condition, inclusion of a proper comparison group (if possible an active control group), use of independent blinded outcome sources, accurate detailing of attrition (with intent-to-treat analyses) and the allocation/randomization process, and evaluation of maintenance of long-term gains should be addressed by future research to improve the internal validity of findings.

Furthermore, some relevant questions remain unresolved. To improve the efficiency of treatment, further research should be carried out to establish the specific weight of each therapy component in overall efficacy (e.g., organizing and planning skills, skills to reduce distractibility, cognitive restructuring, behavior analysis, and impulse control, among others). Given the heterogeneous nature of ADHD, it will be important to determine whether different psychosocial interventions (or therapy ingredients) may be specifically effective for each of its symptom dimensions (e.g., inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity). In addition, future research could explore the characteristics of adults with ADHD who are more likely to respond to CBT therapies. Previous studies have found that adding a psychosocial intervention improves the effects of pharmacological treatment (Safren et al., 2005; Emilsson et al., 2011; Young et al., 2015); however, more research is needed to determine whether the opposite is true. While some studies have found that adding medication to psychological therapy did not significantly improve outcomes (Weiss et al., 2012), others concluded that a combination of methylphenidate and psychotherapy is significantly more effective than psychotherapy alone (Philipsen et al., 2015; Cherkasova et al., 2016). Although it requires further investigation, some studies have demonstrated the preliminary efficacy and feasibility of the use of CBT alone (without medication) (Ramsay and Rostain, 2011; Ramsay, 2012), which would be especially useful in those, not uncommon, cases in which medication is not tolerated or is ineffective. Treatment guidelines state that psychological interventions without medication may be effective for some adults with moderate impairment; however, there are insufficient data to support this recommendation (National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2008).

Conclusions

Psychosocial treatments are effective for treatment of ADHD at the end of the intervention and the gains are sustained for up to 12 months. In the long term, psychosocial interventions, particularly CBT, are effective in improving self-rated inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, and total ADHD symptoms, together with CGI and global functioning, in comparisons with control groups. Within-subject improvements were also significant when rated by blind assessors. However, a careful interpretation of these data is necessary because of the high level of heterogeneity and high risk of bias determined for many of the outcomes. There remain many important questions to be addressed by future studies; however, the evidence from this review suggests that psychological interventions are highly valuable and stable clinical tools for the treatment of adults with ADHD.

Author Contributions

PhD Student CL-P co-designed the study, conducted the literature searches, the study selection, the data extraction and the statistical analyses, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. SM-S co-designed the study and conducted the study selection. JF-C co-designed study and co-wrote the first draft of the manuscript. JS-M supervised and improved the statistical analyses, and co-wrote the methods section. All authors reviewed and edited the manuscript for accuracy, and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge Virginia Soldino for reviewing the manuscript for accuracy, and to Rosa García-Martínez for masking the articles to carry out the independent blinded selection.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00638/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Figure 1. Effect size index formula for between-group outcomes.

Supplementary Figure 2. Effect size index formula for within-subject outcomes.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. text rev. Washington, DC.

Barbaresi, W. J., Colligan, R. C., Weaver, A. L., Voigt, R. G., Killiam, J. M., and Katusic, S. K. (2013). Mortality, ADHD, and psychosocial adversity in adults with childhood ADHD: a prospective study. Pediatrics 131, 631–637. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-2354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brevik, E. J., Lundervold, A. J., Halmøy, A., Posserud, M.-B., Instanes, J. T., Bjorvatn, B., et al. (2017). Prevalence and clinical correlates of insomnia in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 36, 220–227. doi: 10.1111/acps.12756

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cadman, T., Findon, J., Eklund, H., Hayward, H., Howley, D., Cheung, C., et al. (2016). Six-year follow-up study of combined type ADHD from childhood to young adulthood: predictors of functional impairment and comorbid symptoms. Eur. Psychiatry 35, 47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cairncross, M., and Miller, C. J. (2016). The effectiveness of mindfulness-based therapies for ADHD: A meta-analytic review. J. Atten. Disord. doi: 10.1177/1087054715625301. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capusan, A., Bendtsen, P., Marteinsdottir, I., and Larsson, H. (2016). Comorbidity of adult ADHD and its subtypes with substance use disorder in a large population-based epidemiological tudy. J. Atten. Disord. doi: 10.1177/1087054715626511. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castells, X., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., Rigau, D., Bosch, R., Nogueira, M., Vidal, X., et al. (2011). Efficacy of methylphenidate for adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-regression analysis. CNS Drugs 25, 157–169. doi: 10.2165/11539440-000000000-00000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cherkasova, M. V., French, L. R., Syer, C. A., Cousins, L., Galina, H., Ahmadi-Kashani, Y., et al. (2016). Efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy with and without medication for adults with ADHD: a randomized clinical trial. J. Atten. Disord. doi: 10.1177/1087054716671197. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd. Edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Cunill, R., Castells, X., Tobias, A., and Capellà, D. (2016). Efficacy, safety and variability in pharmacotherapy for adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis and meta-regression in over 9000 patients. Psychopharmacology 233, 187–197. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4099-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Das, D., Cherbuin, N., Butterworth, P., Anstey, K. J., and Easteal, S. (2012). A population-based study of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and associated impairment in middle-aged adults. PLoS ONE 7:e31500. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duda, T. A., Casey, J. E., and McNevin, N. (2015). Development of graphomotor fluency in adults with ADHD: Evidence of attenuated procedural learning. Hum. Mov. Sci. 44, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2015.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duval, S., and Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 56, 455–463. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., and Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 315, 629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emilsson, B., Gudjonsson, G., Sigurdsson, J. F., Baldursson, G., Einarsson, E., Olafsdottir, H., et al. (2011). Cognitive behaviour therapy in medication-treated adults with ADHD and persistent symptoms: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry 11:116. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-11-116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstein, T., Patsopoulos, N. A., and Weiser, M. (2014). Immediate-release methylphenidate for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 9:Cd005041. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD005041.pub2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V., and Glatt, S. J. (2010). A comparison of the efficacy of medications for adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using meta-analysis of effect sizes. J. Clin. Psychiatry 71, 754–763. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04902pur

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fayyad, J., Sampson, N. A., Hwang, I., Adamowski, T., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Al-Hamzawi, A., et al. (2016). The descriptive epidemiology of DSM-IV Adult ADHD in the world health organization world mental health surveys. Atten. Defic. Hyperact. Disord. 9, 47–65. doi: 10.1007/s12402-016-0208-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleming, A. P., McMahon, R. J., Moran, L. R., Peterson, A. P., and Dreessen, A. (2015). Pilot randomized controlled trial of dialectical behavior therapy group skills training for ADHD among college students. J. Atten. Disord. 19, 260–271. doi: 10.1177/1087054714535951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredriksen, M., and Peleikis, D. E. (2016). Long-term pharmacotherapy of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a literature review and clinical study. Basic Clin. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 118, 23–31. doi: 10.1111/bcpt.12477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, Y., Xu, G., and Zhu, Y. (2017). A randomized controlled trial of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for college students with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 22, 388–399. doi: 10.1177/1087054716686183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammond, D. C. (2007). What is neurofeedback? J. Neurother. 10, 25–36. doi: 10.1300/J184v10n04_04

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hechtman, L., Swanson, J. M., Sibley, M. H., Stehli, A., Lakes, K. D., Owens, E. B., et al. (2016). Functional adult outcomes 16 years after childhood diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: MTA results. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 55, 945.e2–952.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.07.774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hesslinger, B., Tebartz Van Elst, L., Nyberg, E., Dykierek, P., Richter, H., Berner, M., et al. (2002). Psychotherapy of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 252, 177–184. doi: 10.1007/s00406-002-0379-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P., Altman, D. G., Gøtzsche, P. C., Jüni, P., Moher, D., Oxman, A. D., et al. (2011). The cochrane collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 343:d5928. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d5928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P. T., and Green, S. (2011). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. The Cochrane Collaboration.

Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., and Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327, 557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunter, J. E., and Schmidt, F. L. (2004). “Technical questions in meta-analysis of d values,” in Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings, 2nd Edn, eds J. E. Hunter and F. L. Schmidt (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications), 335–390.

Jensen, C. M., Amdisen, B. L., Jørgensen, K. J., and Arnfred, S. M. (2016). Cognitive behavioural therapy for ADHD in adults: systematic review and meta-analyses. Atten. Defic. Hyperact. Disord. 8, 3–11. doi: 10.1007/s12402-016-0188-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirino, E., Imagawa, H., Goto, T., and Montgomery, W. (2015). Sociodemographics, comorbidities, healthcare utilization and work productivity in Japanese patients with adult ADHD. PLoS ONE 10:e0132233. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knouse, L. E., Anastopoulos, A. D., and Dunlosky, J. (2012). Isolating metamemory deficits in the self-regulated learning of adults with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 16, 650–660. doi: 10.1177/1087054711417231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knouse, L. E., and Safren, S. A. (2010). Current status of cognitive behavioral therapy for adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 33, 497–509. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2010.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knouse, L. E., Teller, J., and Brooks, M. A. (2017). Meta-analysis of cognitive behavioral treatments for adult ADHD. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 85, 737–750. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuriyan, A. B., Pelham, W. E., Molina, B. S., Waschbusch, D. A., Gnagy, E. M., Sibley, M. H., et al. (2013). Young adult educational and vocational outcomes of children diagnosed with ADHD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 41, 27–41. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9658-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liberati, A., Altman, D. G., Tetzlaff, J., Mulrow, C., Gøtzsche, P. C., Ioannidis, J. P., et al. (2009). The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate healthcare interventions: explanation and elaboration. BMJ 339:b2700. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b2700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linderkamp, F., and Lauth, G. (2011). Zur Wirksamkeit pharmakologischer und psychotherapeutischer Therapien bei Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-/ Hyperaktivitätsstörung (ADHS) im Erwachsenenalter: eine empirische Metaanalyse. Verhaltenstherapie 21, 229–238. doi: 10.1159/000332429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linehan, M. (1993). Cognitive-Behavioral Treatment of Borderline Personality Disorder. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Matthies, S. D., and Philipsen, A. (2014). Common ground in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD)–review of recent findings. Borderline Personal. Disord. Emot. Dysreg. 1:3. doi: 10.1186/2051-6673-1-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, J. T., Benson, J. W., Knouse, L. E., Kimbrel, N. A., and Anastopoulos, A. D. (2013). Are negative automatic thoughts associated with adhd in adulthood? Cogn. Ther. Res. 37, 851–859. doi: 10.1007/s10608-013-9525-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgensterns, E., Alfredsson, J., and Hirvikoski, T. (2016). Structured skills training for adults with ADHD in an outpatient psychiatric context: an open feasibility trial. Atten. Defic. Hyperact. Disord. 8, 101–111. doi: 10.1007/s12402-015-0182-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, S. B. (2000). Distribution of the standardized mean change effect size for meta-analysis on repeated measures. Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol. 53, 17–29. doi: 10.1348/000711000159150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, S. B. (2008). Estimating effect sizes from pretest-posttest-control group designs. Organ. Res. Methods 11, 364–386. doi: 10.1177/1094428106291059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasri, B., Castenfors, M., Fredlund, P., Ginsberg, Y., Lindefors, N., and Kaldo, V. (2017). Group treatment for adults with ADHD based on a novel combination of cognitive and dialectical behavior interventions. J. Atten. Disord. doi: 10.1177/1087054717690231. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (2008). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: the NICE Guidelines on Diagnosis and Management of ADHD in Children, Young People and Adults [CG72]. London: National Institute for Health and Care Excellence.

National Institute of Mental Health (1985). Clinical Global Impressions (CGI) Scale. Psychopharmacol. Bull. 17, 839–843.

Nigg, J. (2013). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and adverse health outcomes. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33, 215–228. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Connor, D., Green, S., and Higgins, J. P. T. (2011). “Chapter 5: Defining the review question and developing criteria for including studies,” in Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions, 4th Edn, eds J. P. T. Higgins and S. Green (The Cochrane Collaboration).

Pettersson, R., Söderström, S., Edlund-Söderström, K., and Nilsson, K. W. (2014). Internet-based cognitive behavioral therapy for adults with ADHD in outpatient psychiatric care: A randomized trial. J. Atten. Disord. 21, 508–521. doi: 10.1177/1087054714539998

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petticrew, M., and Roberts, H. (2005). Systematic Reviews in the Social Sciences: A Practical Guide. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

Google Scholar

Philipsen, A., Jans, T., Graf, E., Matthies, S., Borel, P., Colla, M., et al. (2015). Effects of group psychotherapy, individual counseling, methylphenidate, and placebo in the treatment of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 1199–1210. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramsay, J. R. (2012). “Without a Net”: CBT without medications for an adult with ADHD. Clin. Case Stud. 11, 48–65. doi: 10.1177/1534650112440741

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramsay, J. R., and Rostain, A. L. (2011). CBT without medications for adult ADHD: an open pilot study of five patients. J. Cog. Psychother. 25, 277–286. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.25.4.277

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimherr, F. W., Marchant, B. K., Olsen, J. L., Wender, P. H., and Robison, R. J. (2013). Oppositional defiant disorder in adults with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 17, 102–113. doi: 10.1177/1087054711425774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychol. Bull. 86, 638–641. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-Analytic Procedures for Social Research, ed. rev. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Safren, S. A., Otto, M. W., Sprich, S., Winett, C. L., Wilens, T. E., and Biederman, J. (2005). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for ADHD in medication-treated adults with continued symptoms. Behav. Res. Ther. 43, 831–842. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Safren, S. A., Sprich, S., Chulvick, S., and Otto, M. W. (2004). Psychosocial treatments for adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 27, 349–360. doi: 10.1016/S0193-953X(03)00089-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Safren, S. A., Sprich, S., Mimiaga, M. J., Surman, C., Knouse, L., Groves, M., et al. (2010). Cognitive behavioral therapy vs relaxation for medication-treated adults with ADHD and persistent symptoms. JAMA 304, 875–880. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.1192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salakari, A., Virta, M., Grönroos, N., Chydenius, E., Partinen, M., Vataja, R., et al. (2010). Cognitive-behaviorally-oriented group rehabilitation of adults with ADHD: results of a 6-month follow-up. J. Atten. Disord. 13, 516–523. doi: 10.1177/1087054709332069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salomone, S., Fleming, G. R., Shanahan, J. M., Castorina, M., Bramham, J., O'Connell, R. G., et al. (2015). The effects of a Self-Alert Training (SAT) program in adults with ADHD. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:45. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, K. F., and Grimes, D. A. (2002). Sample size slippages in randomised trials: exclusions and the lost and wayward. Lancet 359, 781–785. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)07882-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheenan, D. V., Harnett-Sheenan, K., and Raj, B. A. (1996). The measurement of disability. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 11, 89–95. doi: 10.1097/00004850-199606003-00015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, V., Czobor, P., Bálint, S., Mészáros, A., and Bitter, I. (2009). Prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis. Br. J. Psychiatry 194, 204–211. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.048827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stickley, A., Koyanagi, A., Ruchkin, V., and Kamio, Y. (2016). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and suicide ideation and attempts: findings from the adult psychiatric morbidity survey 2007. J. Affect. Disord. 189, 321–328. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strohmeier, C. W., Rosenfield, B., DiTomasso, R. A., and Ramsay, J. R. (2016). Assessment of the relationship between self-reported cognitive distortions and adult ADHD, anxiety, depression, and hopelessness. Psychiatry Res. 238, 153–158. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.02.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrente, F., López, P., Alvarez Prado, D., Kichic, R., Cetkovich-Bakmas, M., Lischinsky, A., et al. (2014). Dysfunctional cognitions and their emotional, behavioral, and functional correlates in adults with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): is the cognitive-behavioral model valid? J. Atten. Disord. 18, 412–424. doi: 10.1177/1087054712443153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vinogradov, S., Fisher, M., and de Villers-Sidani, E. (2012). Cognitive training for impaired neural systems in neuropsychiatric illness. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 43–76. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voigt, R. G., Katusic, S. K., Colligan, R. C., Killian, J. M., Weaver, A. L., and Barbaresi, W. J. (2017). Academic achievement in adults with a history of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a population-based prospective study. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 38, 1–11. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, M., Murray, C., Wasdell, M., Greenfield, B., Giles, L., and Hechtman, L. (2012). A randomized controlled trial of CBT therapy for adults with ADHD with and without medication. BMC Psychiatry 12:30. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-30

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilens, T. E., Spencer, T. J., and Biederman, J. (2002). A review of the pharmacotherapy of adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J. Atten. Disord. 5, 189–202. doi: 10.1177/108705470100500401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, S., Khondoker, M., Emilsson, B., Sigurdsson, J. F., Philipp-Wiegmann, F., Baldursson, G., et al. (2015). Cognitive-behavioural therapy in medication-treated adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and co-morbid psychopathology: a randomized controlled trial using multi-level analysis. Psychol. Med. 45, 2793–2804. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715000756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, S., and Ross, R. R. (2007). R&R2 for ADHD Youths and Adults: A Prosocial Competence Training Program. Ottawa, ON: Cognitive Centre of Canada.

Google Scholar

Young, Z., Moghaddam, N., and Tickle, A. (2016). The efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy for adults with ADHD: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Atten. Disord. doi: 10.1177/1087054716664413. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zylowska, L., Ackerman, D. L., Yang, M. H., Futrell, J. L., Horton, N. L., Hale, T. S., et al. (2008). Mindfulness meditation training in adults and adolescents with ADHD: A feasibility study. J. Atten. Disord. 11, 737–746. doi: 10.1177/1087054707308502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: meta-analysis, adult ADHD treatment, psychosocial treatment, long-term efficacy, cognitive-behavioral therapy, dialectical-behavior therapy, mindfulness-based cognitive therapy

Citation: López-Pinar C, Martínez-Sanchís S, Carbonell-Vayá E, Fenollar-Cortés J and Sánchez-Meca J (2018) Long-Term Efficacy of Psychosocial Treatments for Adults With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Meta-Analytic Review. Front. Psychol. 9:638. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00638

Received: 11 January 2018; Accepted: 16 April 2018;
Published: 04 May 2018.

Edited by:

Gianluca Castelnuovo, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Italy

Reviewed by:

Dianna Theadora Kenny, University of Sydney, Australia
Małgorzata Lipowska, University of Gdansk, Poland

Copyright © 2018 López-Pinar, Martínez-Sanchís, Carbonell-Vayá, Fenollar-Cortés and Sánchez-Meca. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carlos López-Pinar, carlopi@alumni.uv.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.