Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 03 August 2018
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Single-Cases in Psychology: From Practice to Theory View all 10 articles

Progressive Compromise of Nouns and Action Verbs in Posterior Cortical Atrophy

\r\nBrenda SteebBrenda Steeb1Indira García-Cordero,Indira García-Cordero2,3Marjolein C. HuizingMarjolein C. Huizing1Lucas CollazoLucas Collazo1Geraldine BorovinskyGeraldine Borovinsky1Jesica FerrariJesica Ferrari4Macarena M. Cuitio,,,Macarena M. Cuitiño1,3,5,6Agustín Ibez,,,,Agustín Ibáñez2,3,7,8,9Lucas Sedeo,Lucas Sedeño2,3Adolfo M. García,,*Adolfo M. García2,3,10*
  • 1Laboratory of Language Research (LILEN), Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 2Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 3National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 4Department of Language Speech, Institute of Cognitive Neurology, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 5Faculty of Psychology, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 6Faculty of Psychology, University of Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 7Universidad Autónoma del Caribe, Barranquilla, Colombia
  • 8Center for Social and Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology, Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez, Santiago de Chile, Chile
  • 9Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Australian Research Council, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 10Faculty of Education, National University of Cuyo, Mendoza, Argentina

Processing of nouns and action verbs can be differentially compromised following lesions to posterior and anterior/motor brain regions, respectively. However, little is known about how these deficits progress in the course of neurodegeneration. To address this issue, we assessed productive lexical skills in a patient with posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) at two different stages of his pathology. On both occasions, he underwent a structural brain imaging protocol and completed semantic fluency tasks requiring retrieval of animals (nouns) and actions (verbs). Imaging results were compared with those of controls via voxel-based morphometry (VBM), whereas fluency performance was compared to age-matched norms through Crawford’s t-tests. In the first assessment, the patient exhibited atrophy of more posterior regions supporting multimodal semantics (medial temporal and lingual gyri), together with a selective deficit in noun fluency. Then, by the second assessment, the patient’s atrophy had progressed mainly toward fronto-motor regions (rolandic operculum, inferior and superior frontal gyri) and subcortical motor hubs (cerebellum, thalamus), and his fluency impairments had extended to action verbs. These results offer unprecedented evidence of the specificity of the pathways related to noun and action-verb impairments in the course of neurodegeneration, highlighting the latter’s critical dependence on damage to regions supporting motor functions, as opposed to multimodal semantic processes.

Introduction

Abundant neuropsychological evidence supports a dissociation between nouns and verbs – particularly, action verbs (Damasio and Tranel, 1993; Bookheimer, 2002; Chiarello et al., 2002; Cappa and Perani, 2003; Vigliocco et al., 2011). Whereas the former are typically compromised upon damage to posterior (mainly temporo-parietal) areas, the latter tend to become differentially impaired following lesions to frontal and subcortical motor regions (Pulvermüller et al., 1999; Shapiro and Caramazza, 2003; Luzzatti et al., 2006; Bocanegra et al., 2017). Far from being a random anatomo-clinical coincidence, this pattern has been sensibly interpreted from an embodied cognition framework (García and Ibáñez, 2016a, 2018; Birba et al., 2017).

This account highlights that nouns denote individuated, atemporal entities (such as objects or animals), typically identified by the collection of their sensory (e.g., visual, spatial, color, tactile) properties (Black and Chiat, 2003). Such a multimodal integration would be critically afforded by anterior and superior temporal as well as infero-parietal regions, given their well-established role as convergence hubs (Bookheimer, 2002; Patterson et al., 2007; Moseley and Pulvermüller, 2014; Ralph et al., 2017). Distinct noun-processing deficits can thus be logically predicted following temporo-parietal damage.

Instead, action verbs explicitly foreground bodily movements, sometimes involving the same effectors, and their identification hinges on recognizing the motion patterns they imply (García and Ibáñez, 2016a,b). Processing of these units would critically hinge on reactivations of the circuits which subserve the programming and execution of those movements, as systematically detected in frontal and subcortical motor regions (Moseley and Pulvermüller, 2014; García and Ibáñez, 2016a; Horoufchin et al., 2018; Pulvermüller, 2018). Indeed, lesions or atrophy of these areas entail selective or differential action-verb deficits (Arévalo et al., 2012; Cardona et al., 2013, 2014; Birba et al., 2017), arguably reflecting the disruption of embodied mechanisms (Birba et al., 2017; García and Ibáñez, 2018). Beyond its theoretical implications, this systematic link represents a promising avenue to develop early biomarkers across a range of brain diseases.

However, clean-cut dissociations between verbs and nouns are not always observed. For instance, difficulties in processing action verbs and other verb classes have been observed upon damage to the left ventral supramarginal gyrus (Meteyard et al., 2013) and other parietal and occipito-temporal regions (Taylor et al., 2017). On the other hand, noun impairments have been reported alongside verb disturbances following frontostriatal damage in Parkinson’s disease (Bocanegra et al., 2017) and frontal damage in non-fluent primary progressive aphasia (Marcotte et al., 2014). While this might seem to jeopardize de validity of the double dissociation originally postulated, a notably underexploited methodological strategy might shed light on the issue.

As it happens, besides the high variability of brain damage across patients (Crutch et al., 2012, 2017), these inconsistencies partly reflect the almost exclusive use of synchronic, one-time assessments, which preclude within-subject comparisons of performance as brain damage extends from one region to the next. A potentially useful way to overcome such limitations is to observe the progression of noun and action-verb processing deficits in the course of neurodegeneration. More particularly, given that nouns are easier to process than verbs (Goodglass and Geschwind, 1976; Zingeser and Berndt, 1990; Berndt et al., 1997; Thompson, 2003; Tyler et al., 2004; Mätzig et al., 2009; Shebani et al., 2017), a physiopathological model characterized by initial posterior damage progressively extending to frontal and subcortical motor hubs would offer a relevant testing ground. Such a pattern can be found in posterior cortical atrophy (PCA).

PCA is a syndrome characterized by posterior brain atrophy, a progressive decline in visual processing, and other higher-order impairments (Crutch et al., 2017), such as visual agnosia, disorientation, ocular fixation deficits, ataxia, anomia, agraphia, acalculia, and transcortical sensory aphasia (Tang-Wai et al., 2004; McMonagle et al., 2006). However, memory and language are relatively spared in early stages. The majority of PCA cases are attributable to an underlying disease, most commonly Alzheimer’s (Seguin et al., 2011).

Imaging studies have associated the syndrome with preferential atrophy of the occipital lobes (Kas et al., 2011; Panegyres et al., 2017), including the primary visual cortex and visual association areas (Schmidtke et al., 2005; Whitwell et al., 2007a; Migliaccio et al., 2009; Lehmann et al., 2011), posterior temporal regions (Schmidtke et al., 2005; Whitwell et al., 2007a; Migliaccio et al., 2009; Lehmann et al., 2011), and posterior parietal regions (Whitwell et al., 2007a) –especially the superior parietal cortex (Migliaccio et al., 2009; Lehmann et al., 2011). Predominantly right hippocampal atrophy has also been reported in PCA patients, with comparatively minor gray matter loss in striatal, insular, and inferior frontal regions (Whitwell et al., 2007a; Migliaccio et al., 2009). Importantly, as degeneration progresses, extended temporo-parietal damage is accompanied by atrophy of frontal and motor hubs (Lehmann et al., 2012; Ryan et al., 2014).

Against this background, we assessed the retrieval of animals (nouns) and actions (verbs) in a patient with PCA on two successive occasions: first, in an initial evaluation (Eval-1) at age 71, when atrophy was mostly confined to posterior regions supporting multimodal semantics; and then, in a follow-up evaluation (Eval-2) at age 73, when atrophy had progressed mainly toward fronto-motor regions. Imaging results were compared with those of controls via voxel-based morphometry (VBM), whereas fluency performance was compared to education- and age-matched norms through single-case statistics (see section “Fluency Performance Analysis”). We predicted that, if action verbs are critically grounded in motor regions, they should be selectively spared when atrophy is mainly posterior, despite their higher processing demands. Then, as neurodegeneration reaches frontal and subcortical motor hubs, deficits in this category should become evident and comparable to those in noun processing. This way, we aimed to assess the action-verb/noun dissociation in an overlooked though critical model of progressive neurodegeneration.

Background

We present the case of a right-handed, 73-year-old man with PCA who reported no previous history of psychiatric or neurological disorders. He completed high school education, entered law school, and dropped out a few years later. He then started a gypsum company with a partner, in which he worked until retirement.

In 2006, at the age of 64, the patient consulted the Center for Memory Studies at the Institute of Cognitive Neurology, reporting non-progressive memory and attention deficits, episodes of spatial disorientation, and difficulty with spatial location of objects, recognition of known places, and reading –although ophthalmological examination indicated normal eye function.

An initial neuropsychological evaluation revealed impairments in tasks taxing visuoperceptual and visuospatial functions, such as localization of stimuli in space, visual memory, and visual attention. Auditory attention was normal and verbal memory tasks revealed difficulties in immediate paragraph recall with preserved word recall. In terms of linguistic abilities, the patient scored within normal ranges in tasks assessing naming, comprehension of complex grammatical structures, and phonological verbal fluency. However, the patient showed letter-by-letter reading. The assessment of executive functions indicated normal performance in tasks requiring motor series programming, responses to conflicting instructions, and verbal inhibitory control. Conversely, motor inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility were slightly impaired. Finally, an initial brain MRI showed no evidence of neoplastic, vascular or inflammatory disease, but it revealed atrophy in the posterior parietal cortex, the post-central gyrus, and the superior parietal lobule.

The patient received a diagnosis of PCA and started cognitive rehabilitation. The treatment initially led to a slight improvement of all impaired functions except for language skills –in fact, previously unobserved signs of anomia appeared at the first control evaluation, 8 months after diagnosis. However, since 19 months after diagnosis, a gradual decline became evident in visuospatial and visuoperceptual functions and visual attention, accompanied by worsening anomia and semantic dysfluency. Memory skills, though initially unimpaired, also became compromised 21 months after diagnosis. Disturbances of language, memory, and, most notably, visuospatial abilities progressed over time. By the age of 71, the patient presented severe cognitive deterioration, with very low performance in general screening tasks. For a more detailed description of the patient’s cognitive profiles at different stages, see Esteves et al. (2018).

Materials and Methods

MRI Analysis

Control MRI Sample

We obtained MRI scans from 16 healthy male controls, with a mean age of 70.19 (SD = 5.28) and an average of 17.25 years of education (SD = 2.11). This sample size was established by reference to previous single-case studies yielding robust results with control groups of 13–16 subjects (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004; Migliaccio et al., 2012; Spinelli et al., 2015). Control participants had no history of neurological or psychiatric conditions. As shown by Crawford’s modified t-tests (Crawford and Garthwaite, 2002, 2012; Crawford et al., 2011), this sample was matched with the patient for age (Eval-1: t = 0.15, p = 0.88, two-tailed probability; Eval-2: t = 0.52, p = 0.61, two-tailed probability) and education (t = -0.11, p = 0.91, two-tailed probability). Note that Crawford’s t-test is robust for non-normal distributions, presents low values of type I error, and has yielded replicable results in previous single-case studies –even when individual scores are compared with data from small samples (Baez et al., 2013; Couto et al., 2013; García et al., 2016).

The study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Ethics Committee of the Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO, now a host institution of the Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience). All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and written informed consent was obtained from the patient for the publication of this case report. The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of INECO. Demographical and MRI data from the patient and controls are available upon request.

Image Acquisition

MRI acquisition and pre-processing steps are reported following the practical guide from the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (Nichols et al., 2017; Poldrack et al., 2017). The patient and the controls were scanned in a 1.5-T Philips Intera scanner with a standard head coil (eight channels). We used a T1-weighted anatomical 3D spin echo sequence that covered the whole brain. Structural T1 scans were acquired parallel to the plane connecting the anterior and posterior commissures with the following parameters: matrix size = 256 × 224 × 256 (for Eval-1) and 175 × 256 × 256 (for Eval-2), 1 mm isotropic, repetition time (TR) = 7489 ms, echo time (TE) = 3420 ms, flip angle = 8°, and sequence duration = 7 min.

Voxel-Based Morphometry Pre-processing

VBM was performed to account for the patient’s global atrophy pattern in Eval-1 and Eval-2. Structural images from the patient and the control group were pre-processed with the DARTEL Toolbox from Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM12),1 following previous procedures (Ashburner and Friston, 2000; García-Cordero et al., 2016; Melloni et al., 2016; Sedeño et al., 2017). T1-weighted images in native space were segmented in gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid volumes, using the default parameters of the SPM12 (bias regularization was set to 0.001 and bias FWHM was set to 60 mm cut-off). The following step consisted in applying the DARTEL (create templates) module, with the default parameters of SPM12, to create a template of gray matter that is generated from the complete data set –thus increasing the accuracy of inter-subject alignment (Ashburner, 2007). Then, we ran the “Normalize to MNI Space” module to affine register the last template and the individual gray matter segmented maps, from the previous step, into the MNI space. Subsequently, all images were modulated to correct volume changes by Jacobian determinants, and to avoid bias in the intensity of an area due to its expansion during warping. Then, a 12 mm full-width half-maximum kernel was applied to all images, based on previous recommendations (Ashburner and Friston, 2000; Good et al., 2001). This step leads the data to present a more normal distribution for subsequent parametric analysis. Finally, total gray matter volume was obtained via the VBM8 toolbox for SPM122 and used as a nuisance covariate in the image statistical analysis.

Semantic Fluency Tasks

In both Eval-1 and Eval-2, the patient’s word retrieval skills were assessed via semantic fluency tasks (administered in Spanish, the patient’s native language). In the noun condition, the patient was given 1 min to name as many animals as possible. Then, in the action-verb condition, the patient was allotted another minute to mention as many actions as he could. The patient was instructed not to repeat words and, when he did, these were excluded from the analyses. Words belonging to the same lemma, but with a different derivational suffix [e.g., perro (dog), perra (female dog), perrito (small dog)] or hyperonyms [e.g., paloma (dove), ave (bird)] were also rejected. In the action-verb task, only infinitives were accepted. During the test, the examiner remained quiet and no clues were provided. Answers were given orally, audio-recorded, and then transcribed.

Statistical Analysis

Image Statistical Analysis

To establish the patient’s whole-brain atrophy pattern in Eval-1 and Eval-2, we compared his pre-processed imaged with those of the control group via t-tests in a second-level analysis within general linear models on SPM12, following previous studies (Baez et al., 2013; García et al., 2016). Only voxels with a gray matter intensity value greater than 0.2 (maximum value = 1) were entered to avoid edge effects around the border between gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid (Friederich et al., 2012). We included total gray matter volume as a covariate in the group comparison to exclusively identify regional changes that are not explained by global effects (Friederich et al., 2012). In line with formal recommendations (Poldrack et al., 2008) and previous reports (Whitwell et al., 2007b; Zamboni et al., 2008), the statistical threshold was set at p < 0.001 uncorrected, with an extension of 30 voxels.

Fluency Performance Analysis

The patient’s fluency performance was compared to validated age- and educational-level-matched norms for the Argentine population (animals: n = 63, M = 19.5, SD = 5.5; verbs: n = 25, M = 21, SD = 5.73) –for further data on the sample yielding these normative values, see (Butman et al., 2000; Abraham et al., 2008). The results were compared through Crawford’s t-tests (Crawford and Howell, 1998; Crawford and Garthwaite, 2002, 2012; Crawford et al., 2009, 2011). As stated above, this test provides robust information for non-normal distributions, presents low rates of type-I error, and has yielded robust results in previous single-case studies (Straube et al., 2010; Couto et al., 2013, 2014; García et al., 2016, 2017c).

Results

Eval-1 Results

VBM Results

Relative to controls, the patient exhibited atrophy in left posterior regions, namely, the lingual and middle temporal gyri (for details, see Table 1 and Figure 1A).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Patient’s atrophied areas in the initial evaluation.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Anatomical and behavioral results. (A) Patient’s atrophy pattern at the initial evaluation (Eval-1, age 71, left panel) and the second evaluation (Eval-2, age 73, right panel). The image evidences the progression of neurodegeneration from posterior regions (in Eval-1) to frontal and motor regions (in Eval-2), as compared to healthy sociodemographically matched controls (p < 0.001 uncorrected, with an extension of 30 voxels; coordinates are in MNI space). (B) Patient’s fluency performance compared to age-matched norms. Noun fluency was affected in both assessments, whereas action-verb fluency proved impaired only in Eval-2. The asterisk () indicates significant differences (p < 0.05 and p < 0.001) between the patient’s performance and normative values.

Fluency Results

The patient’s semantic fluency performance on Eval-1 revealed a dissociation between noun and action-verb processing (Figure 1B). On the one hand, he retrieved only two words in the noun condition, thus exhibiting a significant impairment relative to age-matched norms (Z = -3.2; t = -3.2; p = 0.002). On the other hand, he produced 13 action verbs, which indicated low but normal performance in verb fluency compared to controls (Z = -1.4; t = -1.4; p = 0.18).

Eval-2 Results

VBM Results

In Eval-2, the patient’s atrophy progressed from posterior to central and frontal regions, including the thalamus, the cerebellum, the rolandic operculum, the inferior and superior frontal gyrus, and the supramarginal gyrus (for detailed areas of atrophy, see Table 2 and Figure 1A).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Patient’s atrophied areas in the second evaluation.

Fluency Results

Fluency assessment at Eval-2 revealed significant deficits in both categories (Figure 1B). Specifically, the patient was capable of naming only one animal (Z = -3.4; t = -3.3; p = 0.001) and eight action verbs (Z = -2.3; t = -2.2; p = 0.036).

Discussion

The present study aimed to assess the dissociation between noun and action-verb production in a longitudinal lesion model showing earlier atrophy of posterior regions (Eval-1) and later compromise of fronto-motor regions (Eval-2). Results showed that, in Eval-1, the patient evinced a selective deficit in noun fluency, whereas action-verb difficulties became significant only in Eval-2, supporting the critical reliance of action verbs on (embodied) motor mechanisms. These findings are discussed below.

In Eval-1 (age 71), the patient’s selective atrophy of left posterior regions (middle temporal and lingual gyri) was accompanied by impaired noun retrieval but spared action-verb production. Compatibly, previous research has repeatedly shown marked noun impairments in aphasic patients with predominant left temporal and occipital lesions (Luzzatti et al., 2006), even when verb processing was spared (Damasio and Tranel, 1993). In line with these findings, our Eval-1 results suggest that when neural damage spares anterior/motor regions, processing of action verbs is not significantly compromised.

In Eval-2 (age 73), when atrophy had extended to the supramarginal gyrus and, more crucially, to various regions subserving motor function –namely, the thalamus (Sommer, 2003), the cerebellum (Manto et al., 2012), the rolandic operculum (Brown et al., 2007, 2009), and the inferior and superior frontal gyri (Dassonville et al., 2001; Gardini et al., 2016)–, the patient’s noun fluency deficits were accompanied by action-verb difficulties. This aligns with evidence that left posterior frontal lesions typically involve greater impairments for verbs than nouns in naming and repetition tasks –even with ambiguous items and pseudo-words– (Shapiro and Caramazza, 2003), and that action verbs may become selectively compromised in patients with frontostriatal motor network atrophy (Birba et al., 2017; Bocanegra et al., 2017; García et al., 2017a). The patient’s anatomo-clinical profile in Eval-2 extends such findings, further suggesting that action-verb deficits are distinctly related to damage of anterior/motor regions, as opposed to other portions of the vast language network.

Despite the converging evidence described above, other approaches have yielded contradictory results. For example, difficulties in regular (but not irregular) verb production have been reported in a subset of stroke patients with left ventral supramarginal damage (Meteyard et al., 2013). Also, left inferior frontal degeneration in non-fluent primary progressive aphasia has been associated with deficits in both verb and noun production (Marcotte et al., 2014). Although, prima facie, these results would seem to jeopardize the dissociation postulated above, such conflicting findings may reflect methodological limitations, such as the heterogeneous nature (Luzzatti et al., 2002; Mätzig et al., 2009) and anatomical inconsistency (Berndt et al., 1997; Thompson et al., 2012) of regions compromised across patients with focal damage, as well as the diffuse nature of neurodegenerative disorders (Bird et al., 2000; Cousins and Grossman, 2017; Shebani et al., 2017). Moreover, many of the studies reporting trans-categorical deficits lacked anatomical mappings of lesion extent (De Renzi and Di Pellegrino, 1995; Berndt et al., 1997; Luzzatti et al., 2002; Mätzig et al., 2009; Bocanegra et al., 2017; García et al., 2017c), so they cannot firmly rule out the dissociation postulated herein; indeed, exclusively clinical confirmation of a disorder is blind to the actual advance of physiopathology.

Importantly, too, noun and action-verb deficits are differentially related to overall cognitive status: when domain-general functions are compromised, nouns are impaired as much as verbs, even in disorders not characterized by posterior damage (Birba et al., 2017; Bocanegra et al., 2017; García et al., 2017a). Given that most studies failing to replicate this double dissociation did not assess the role of domain-general functions in the patients’ performance, noun deficits in fronto-motor disorders may have emerged as an epiphenomenon to overall (non-language-specific) alterations. Moreover, note that several studies have looked at verbs in general (Miceli et al., 1988; Zingeser and Berndt, 1990; Cappa et al., 1998; Kim and Thompson, 2000; Luzzatti et al., 2006; Tyler et al., 2008; Silveri et al., 2017), which are widely varied in their psycholinguistic properties and depend on widely distributed networks covering multiple brain regions (Marshall et al., 1998; Bird et al., 2000; Shapiro et al., 2006). Thus, at least in some of these studies, verb deficits in patients with posterior lesions may reflect difficulties in lexical subsets whose putative bases are more widely distributed across posterior lobes.

Considering the above caveats, our study represents a useful complementary approach to assess the neurofunctional organization of action verbs and nouns. First, note that PCA is characterized by posterior-to-anterior atrophy, and that nouns are easier to process than verbs in terms of argumental structure (Thompson, 2003), syntactic complexity (Goodglass and Geschwind, 1976; Zingeser and Berndt, 1990; Berndt et al., 1997), morphology (Tyler et al., 2004), and imageability (Crepaldi et al., 2006). Thus, our longitudinal approach created a stringent testing ground for our hypothesis, since a selective deficit for nouns in Eval-1 (when atrophy was confined to posterior regions) could not be explained by coarse category-related demands. Second, using VBM, we confirmed the location and extent of atrophy in each time point, which enabled us to move beyond the neural uncertainty of neuropsychological studies based solely on clinical evidence. Third, by employing a longitudinal design focused on a progressive, single-subject atrophy model, we circumvented confounds related to within-sample heterogeneity in multi-participant studies on language disorders (Nespoulous, 2000; Krishnan et al., 2012).

More generally, our study meets the requisite of incorporating single-subject studies in cognitive neuroscience, as a crucial complement of group-based neuroimaging research (Dubois and Adolphs, 2016). As shown by previous works, establishing particular patterns of compromised and spared functions given circumscribed MRI/fMRI abnormalities in theoretically relevant single cases is highly useful for tracking particular forms of functional organization, impairment, and re-organization (Baez et al., 2013; Sedeño et al., 2014; Couto et al., 2015; García et al., 2017a,c; Ibáñez et al., 2018). A dialog between individual-level findings and group-based outcomes is critical to foster progress in both theoretical and applied areas of the field.

In this sense, the dissociation evinced through our design is also supported by research on healthy subjects. Evidence from fMRI studies in multiple languages (e.g., English, German, Spanish, Italian) has repeatedly shown increased activation in frontal/pre-frontal regions for verbs and middle/posterior temporal regions for nouns (Martin et al., 1995; Koenig and Lehmann, 1996; Perani et al., 1999; Shapiro et al., 2006). Moreover, EEG research has revealed increased early frontal negativity modulations for verbs compared to nouns (Brown et al., 1999) and enhanced N400 amplitudes –a component with sources in temporal regions (Lau et al., 2008; Kutas and Federmeier, 2011)– for nouns compared to verbs (Lee and Federmeier, 2006). In addition, transcranial magnetic stimulation of the primary motor cortex systematically yields selective effects for verbs, in general (Shapiro et al., 2001; Cappelletti et al., 2008), and action verbs, in particular (Gerfo et al., 2008; Tomasino et al., 2008; Liuzzi et al., 2010; Innocenti et al., 2014). Our study extends these single-time-point studies with compatible longitudinal evidence.

From a theoretical stance, the present findings can constrain models of lexico-semantic organization in the brain. Nouns (in this case, names of animals) jointly evoke visual, auditory, somatosensory, and higher-level properties which are crucially subserved by convergence hubs in temporal regions (Patterson et al., 2007; Binder and Desai, 2011; Seghier, 2013; Ralph et al., 2017). On the other hand, action verbs are characterized by denoting patterns of bodily movement, whose learning, execution, and control rely on motor regions, including those affected in the patient (Bak, 2013; Bocanegra et al., 2015; García et al., 2016; Abrevaya et al., 2017; Birba et al., 2017). Rather than mere anatomo-functional coincidences, these associations likely reflect the embodied foundations of language mechanisms. From this perspective, the neural regions engaged for the processing of specific word categories are largely driven by the sensorimotor experiences they allude to (Pulvermüller, 2005; García and Ibáñez, 2014, 2016a,b; Moseley and Pulvermüller, 2014; Zwaan, 2014; Horoufchin et al., 2018).

In this sense, a key theoretical contribution of our work is that the successive compromise of experientially specialized networks is mirrored by the temporal ordering of lexico-semantic alterations. Whereas the embodied framework has incorporated a diachronic dimension thanks to relevant studies on child development (Glenberg et al., 2011; Wellsby and Pexman, 2014; Marshall, 2016), such a perspective is missing from models of adult embodied cognition. Given that sustained motor activity is causally related to action-language comprehension (e.g., Trevisan et al., 2017), reduced motility in the elderly could be expected to decrease the reliance of action-verb processing on putative motor networks. Our findings suggest that this is not the case, even when less psycholinguistically accessible word classes are significantly affected. Insights along these lines, supported by longitudinal lesion models, could offer important constraints to consolidate age-sensitive accounts of other embodied language processes, such as categorization (Dalla Volta et al., 2018) and syntactic processing (Casado et al., 2018).

Moreover, although lesion-based embodied models of action-verb processing have mainly drawn on movement disorders (Cardona et al., 2013; Birba et al., 2017) and asymptomatic samples at risk for such pathologies (Kargieman et al., 2014; García et al., 2017b) our study indicates that important insights can be gained by examining non-motor diseases involving specific patterns of motor network sparing and damage. In this sense, the neurofunctional segregation of circuits subserving action-verb and noun association has been informed by evidence from frontal variant frontotemporal dementia (Bak and Hodges, 2003), a non-motor disease secondarily compromising motor circuitry. Further explorations in relevant patient samples, even if not primarily characterized by movement alterations, could expand the basis for empirico-theoretical developments in the field.

In addition, our findings bear clinical implications. A recent translational proposal, known as the “disrupted motor grounding hypothesis” (Birba et al., 2017), underscores the relevance of action-verb deficits as potential sensitive biomarkers of motor-network atrophy, including anterior/motor and frontostriatal regions. Importantly, validation of this proposal requires accruing evidence that such deficits do not emerge when movement-related regions are spared, particularly if other word categories are affected. Our study shows precisely that: in Eval-1, action-verb production was preserved, despite significant posterior atrophy and accompanying noun fluency deficits. By corroborating this progressive anatomo-clinical link at the single-patient level (namely, the level at which clinical interventions operate), our study refines the group-based rationale supporting the disrupted motor grounding hypothesis, addressing recent calls for personalized investigations of brain function (Dubois and Adolphs, 2016).

Limitations and Avenues for Further Research

This work has some limitations. First, the MRI control sample had a moderate size. Note, however, that robust and replicable results have been obtained in previous reports employing similar or smaller control sample sizes (Crepaldi et al., 2006; García et al., 2016; Shebani et al., 2017), or even no control group at all (Damasio and Tranel, 1993; Shapiro and Caramazza, 2003). Also, our use of the same control sample for both time points may have partially amplified VBM differences in Eval-2. Although the same strategy has been successfully employed in previous research (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004; Chan et al., 2015), future studies should examine whether our results are replicated when using a control sample specifically matched on a one-to-one basis to the patient’s age in each time point. Third, the independence of neuroanatomical and behavioral results in the control sample may have impacted our results. It would thus be useful to replicate our study with behavioral outcomes from the control sample as opposed to normative data. Fourth, despite their relevance and wide use, fluency tasks allow for no control over the psycholinguistic properties of words in each condition. Future studies should aim to extend this work with additional tasks, including not only productive but also receptive paradigms. It might also be argued that the animal category has a lesser productivity than the action category, which may have influenced the differential pattern observed in Eval-1. Yet, even if this were the case, this could hardly account for the specific pattern of deficits we detected, as such a difference was held constant for both the patient and controls. Finally, it would be interesting for future studies to replicate our single-case approach including other lexical categories, more particularly, different classes of action and non-action verbs.

Conclusion

This longitudinal single-case study shows that while action verbs, as opposed to nouns, can be spared in the context of temporal brain atrophy, they become significantly compromised when neurodegeneration reaches frontal and subcortical motor hubs. Our progressive lesion-model approach constitutes an informative complement to more traditional one-time group- and patient-level research on the neural organization of lexical categories. Future studies along these lines could further our understanding of the functional organization of language, while opening new windows for translational extensions of the embodied cognition framework (García and Ibáñez, 2018).

Author Contributions

JF, MC, AI, and AMG conceived and designed the study. GB evaluated the patient and administered the fluency tests. IG-C, LS, and LC analyzed the images. MC performed the statistical analysis of the fluency tests. IG-C, LS, and AMG designed Figure 1. BS, MCH, IG-C, and AMG wrote the manuscript. AI and LS provided critical revisions on successive drafts. All authors approved the manuscript in its final form.

Funding

We wish to thank the patient and control participants for volunteering their time. This work was partially supported by grants from CONICET, CONICYT/FONDECYT Regular (1170010), FONDAP 15150012, and the INECO Foundation.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/
  2. ^ http://www.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm/

References

Abraham, M., Della Valentina, R., Gauchat, S., and Marino, J. (2008). Valores normativos de la prueba de Fluidez de acción (nombramiento de verbos). Rev. Neuropsicol. Neuropsiquiatr. Neurosci. 8, 11–19.

Google Scholar

Abrevaya, S., Sedeño, L., Fittipaldi, S., Pineada, D., Lopera, F., Buriticá, O., et al. (2017). The road less traveled: alternative pathways for action-verb processing in Parkinson’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 55, 1429–1435. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arévalo, A. L., Baldo, J. V., and Dronkers, N. F. (2012). What do brain lesions tell us about theories of embodied semantics and the human mirror neuron system? Cortex 48, 242–254. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashburner, J., and Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-based morphometry–the methods. Neuroimage 11(6 Pt 1), 805–821. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baez, S., Couto, B., Herrera, E., Bocanegra, Y., Trujillo-Orrego, N., Madrigal-Zapata, L., et al. (2013). Tracking the cognitive, social, and neuroanatomical profile in early neurodegeneration: type III Cockayne Syndrome. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5:80. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bak, T. H. (2013). The neuroscience of action semantics in neurodegenerative brain diseases. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 26, 671–677. doi: 10.1097/wco.0000000000000039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bak, T. H., and Hodges, J. R. (2003). Kissing and dancing - a test to distinguish the lexical and conceptual contributions to noun/verb and action/object dissociation. Preliminary results in patients with frontotemporal dementia. J. Neurolinguistics 16, 169–181. doi: 10.1016/S0911-6044(02)00011-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berndt, R. S., Haendiges, A. N., Mitchum, C. C., and Sandson, J. (1997). Verb retrieval in aphasia. 2. Relationship to sentence processing. Brain Lang. 56, 107–137. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, J. R., and Desai, R. H. (2011). The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 527–536. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birba, A., García-Cordero, I., Kozono, G., Legaz, A., Ibáñez, A., Sedeño, L., et al. (2017). Losing ground: frontostriatal atrophy disrupts language embodiment in Parkinson’s and Huntington’s disease. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 80, 673–687. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bird, H., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., and Hodges, J. R. (2000). The rise and fall of frequency and imageability: noun and verb production in semantic dementia. Brain Lang. 73, 17–49. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Black, M., and Chiat, S. (2003). Noun-verb dissociations: a multi-faceted phenomenon. J. Neurolinguistics 16, 231–250. doi: 10.1016/S0911-6044(02)00017-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bocanegra, Y., García, A. M., Lopera, F., Pineda, D., Baena, A., Ospina, P., et al. (2017). Unspeakable motion: selective action-verb impairments in Parkinson’s disease patients without mild cognitive impairment. Brain Lang. 168, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2017.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bocanegra, Y., García, A. M., Pineda, D., Buriticá, O., Villegas, A., Lopera, F., et al. (2015). Syntax, action verbs, action semantics, and object semantics in Parkinson’s disease: dissociability, progression, and executive influences. Cortex 69, 237–254. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bookheimer, S. (2002). Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 151–188. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142946

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, C. M., Hagoort, P., and Keurs, M. T. (1999). Electrophysiological signatures of visual lexical processing: open-and closed-class words. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 261–281. doi: 10.1162/089892999563382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, S., Laird, A. R., Pfordresher, P. Q., Thelen, S. M., Turkeltaub, P., and Liotti, M. (2009). The somatotopy of speech: phonation and articulation in the human motor cortex. Brain Cogn. 70, 31–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, S., Ngan, E., and Liotti, M. (2007). A larynx area in the human motor cortex. Cereb. Cortex 18, 837–845. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butman, J., Allegri, R. F., Harris, P., and Drake, M. (2000). Fluencia verbal en español. Medicina 60, 561–564.

Google Scholar

Cappa, S. F., Binetti, G., Pezzini, A., Padovani, A., Rozzini, L., and Trabucchi, M. (1998). Object and action naming in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 50, 351–355. doi: 10.1212/WNL.50.2.351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cappa, S. F., and Perani, D. (2003). The neural correlates of noun and verb processing. J. Neurolinguistics 16, 183–189. doi: 10.1016/S0911-6044(02)00013-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cappelletti, M., Fregni, F., Shapiro, K. A., Pascual-Leone, A., and Caramazza, A. (2008). Processing nouns and verbs in the left frontal cortex: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. J. Cogn. Neurol. 20, 707–720. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardona, J. F., Gershanik, O., Gelormini-Lezama, C., Houck, A. L., Cardona, S., Kargieman, L., et al. (2013). Action-verb processing in Parkinson’s disease: new pathways for motor–language coupling. Brain Struct. Funct. 218, 1355–1373. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0510-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardona, J. F., Kargieman, L., Sinay, V., Gershanik, O., Gelormini, C., Amoruso, L., et al. (2014). How embodied is action language? Neurological evidence from motor diseases. Cognition 131, 311–322. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casado, P., Martín-Loeches, M., León, I., Hernández-Gutiérrez, D., Espuny, J., Muñoz, F., et al. (2018). When syntax meets action: brain potential evidence of overlapping between language and motor sequencing. Cortex 100, 40–51. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, L. T. A., Lynch, W., May, M., Horton, J. C., Miller, B. L., and Rabinovici, G. D. (2015). Prodromal posterior cortical atrophy: clinical, neuropsychological and radiological correlation. Neurocase 21, 44–55. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2013.860176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiarello, C., Liu, S., Shears, C., and Kacinik, N. (2002). Differential asymmetries for recognizing nouns and verbs: Where are they? Neuropsychology 16:35. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.16.1.35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cousins, K. A., and Grossman, M. (2017). Evidence of semantic processing impairments in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Parkinson’s disease. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 30, 617–622. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Couto, B., Adolfi, F., Sedeño, L., Salles, A., Canales-Johnson, A., Alvarez-Abut, P., et al. (2015). Disentangling interoception: insights from focal strokes affecting the perception of external and internal milieus. Front. Psychol. 6:503. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Couto, B., Salles, A., Sedeño, L., Peradejordi, M., Barttfeld, P., Canales-Johnson, A., et al. (2014). The man who feels two hearts: the different pathways of interoception. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1253–1260. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Couto, B., Sedeño, L., Sposato, L. A., Sigman, M., Riccio, P. M., Salles, A., et al. (2013). Insular networks for emotional processing and social cognition: comparison of two case reports with either cortical or subcortical involvement. Cortex 49, 1420–1434. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., and Garthwaite, P. H. (2002). Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences. Neuropsychologia 40, 1196–1208. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00224-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., and Garthwaite, P. H. (2012). Single-case research in neuropsychology: a comparison of five forms of t-test for comparing a case to controls. Cortex 48, 1009–1016. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., Garthwaite, P. H., and Howell, D. C. (2009). On comparing a single case with a control sample: an alternative perspective. Neuropsychologia 47, 2690–2695. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., Garthwaite, P. H., and Ryan, K. (2011). Comparing a single case to a control sample: testing for neuropsychological deficits and dissociations in the presence of covariates. Cortex 47, 1166–1178. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, J. R., and Howell, D. C. (1998). Regression equations in clinical neuropsychology: an evaluation of statistical methods for comparing predicted and obtained scores. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 20, 755–762. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.5.755.1132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crepaldi, D., Aggujaro, S., Arduino, L. S., Zonca, G., Ghirardi, G., Inzaghi, M. G., et al. (2006). Noun–verb dissociation in aphasia: the role of imageability and functional locus of the lesion. Neuropsychologia 44, 73–89. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crutch, S. J., Lehmann, M., Schott, J. M., Rabinovici, G. D., Rossor, M. N., and Fox, N. C. (2012). Posterior cortical atrophy. Lancet Neurol. 11, 170–178. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70289-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crutch, S. J., Schott, J. M., Rabinovici, G. D., Murray, M., Snowden, J., van der Flier, W. M., et al. (2017). Consensus classification of posterior cortical atrophy. Alzheimers Dement. 13, 870–884. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla Volta, R., Avanzini, P., De Marco, D., Gentilucci, M., and Fabbri-Destro, M. (2018). From meaning to categorization: the hierarchical recruitment of brain circuits selective for action verbs. Cortex 100, 95–110. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damasio, A. R., and Tranel, D. (1993). Nouns and verbs are retrieved with differently distributed neural systems. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 90, 4957–4960. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.11.4957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dassonville, P., Lewis, S. M., Zhu, X. H., Ugurbil, K., Kim, S. G., and Ashe, J. (2001). The effect of stimulus–response compatibility on cortical motor activation. Neuroimage 13, 1–14. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Renzi, E., and Di Pellegrino, G. (1995). Sparing of verbs and preserved, but ineffectual reading in a patient with impaired word production. Cortex 31, 619–636. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80016-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubois, J., and Adolphs, R. (2016). Building a science of individual differences from fMRI. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 425–443. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esteves, S., Ramirez Romero, D. A., Torralva, T., Martínez Cuitiño, M., Herndon, S., Couto, B., et al. (2018). Posterior cortical atrophy: a single case cognitive and radiological follow-up. Neurocase 24, 16–30. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2017.1421667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friederich, H. C., Walther, S., Bendszus, M., Biller, A., Thomann, P., Zeigermann, S., et al. (2012). Grey matter abnormalities within cortico-limbic-striatal circuits in acute and weight-restored anorexia nervosa patients. Neuroimage 59, 1106–1113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., Abrevaya, S., Kozono, G., Cordero, I. G., Córdoba, M., Kauffman, M. A., et al. (2016). The cerebellum and embodied semantics: evidence from a case of genetic ataxia due to STUB1 mutations. J. Med. Genet. 54, 114–124. doi: 10.1136/jmedgenet-2016-104148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., Bocanegra, Y., Herrera, E., Moreno, L., Carmona, J., Baena, A., et al. (2017a). Parkinson’s disease compromises the appraisal of action meanings evoked by naturalistic texts. Cortex 100, 111–126. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., Bocanegra, Y., Herrera, E., Pino, M., Muñoz, E., Sedeño, L., et al. (2017b). Action semantic and syntactic deficits in subjects at risk for Huntington’s disease. J. Neuropsychol. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12120 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., and Ibáñez, A. (2014). Words in motion: motor-language coupling in Parkinson’s disease. Transl. Neurosci. 5, 152–159. doi: 10.2478/s13380-014-0218-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., and Ibáñez, A. (2016a). A touch with words: dynamic synergies between manual actions and language. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 68, 59–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., and Ibáñez, A. (2016b). Processes and verbs of doing, in the brain: theoretical implications for systemic functional linguistics. Funct. Lang. 23, 305–335. doi: 10.1075/fol.23.3.02gar

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., and Ibáñez, A. (2018). When embodiment breaks down: language deficits as novel avenues into movement disorders. Cortex 100, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., Sedeño, L., Herrera Murcia, E., Couto, B., and Ibáñez, A. (2017c). A lesion-proof brain? Multidimensional sensorimotor, cognitive, and socio-affective preservation despite extensive damage in a stroke patient. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:335. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Cordero, I., Sedeño, L., de la Fuente, L., Slachevsky, A., Forno, G., Klein, F., et al. (2016). Feeling, learning from and being aware of inner states: interoceptive dimensions in neurodegeneration and stroke. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371:20160006. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardini, S., Venneri, A., McGeown, W. J., Toraci, C., Nocetti, L., Porro, C. A., et al. (2016). Brain activation patterns characterizing different phases of motor action: execution, choice and ideation. Brain Topogr. 29, 679–692. doi: 10.1007/s10548-016-0491-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerfo, E. L., Oliveri, M., Torriero, S., Salerno, S., Koch, G., and Caltagirone, C. (2008). The influence of rTMS over prefrontal and motor areas in a morphological task: grammatical vs. semantic effects. Neuropsychologia 46, 764–770. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glenberg, A. M., Goldberg, A. B., and Zhu, X. (2011). Improving early reading comprehension using embodied CAI. Instr. Sci. 39, 27–39. doi: 10.1007/s11251-009-9096-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Good, C. D., Johnsrude, I. S., Ashburner, J., Henson, R. N., Friston, K. J., and Frackowiak, R. S. (2001). A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 14(1 Pt 1), 21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodglass, H., and Geschwind, N. (1976). “Language disorders (aphasia),” in Handbook of Perception, eds E. Carterette and M. Friedman (New York, NY: Academic Press), 389–428.

Google Scholar

Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Murray, R. C., Rankin, K. P., Weiner, M. W., and Miller, B. L. (2004). Clinical, cognitive and anatomical evolution from nonfluent progressive aphasia to corticobasal syndrome: a case report. Neurocase 10, 426–436. doi: 10.1080/13554790490894011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horoufchin, H., Bzdok, D., Buccino, G., Borghi, A. M., and Binkofski, F. (2018). Action and object words are differentially anchored in the sensory motor system-A perspective on cognitive embodiment. Sci. Rep. 8:6583. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24475-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ibáñez, A., Zimerman, M., Sedeño, L., Lori, N., Rapaccioli, M., Cardon, J. F., et al. (2018). Early bilateral and massive compromise of the frontal lobes. Neuroimage 18, 543–552. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.02.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Innocenti, A., De Stefani, E., Sestito, M., and Gentilucci, M. (2014). Understanding of action-related and abstract verbs in comparison: a behavioural and TMS study. Cogn. Process. 15, 85–92. doi: 10.1007/s10339-013-0583-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kargieman, L., Herrera, E., Baez, S., García, A. M., Dottori, M., Gelormini, C., et al. (2014). Motor–language coupling in Huntington’s disease families. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:122. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kas, A., de Souza, L. C., Samri, D., Bartolomeo, P., Lacomblez, L., Kalafat, M., et al. (2011). Neural correlates of cognitive impairment in posterior cortical atrophy. Brain 134, 1464–1478. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, M., and Thompson, C. K. (2000). Patterns of comprehension and production of nouns and verbs in agrammatism: implications for lexical organization. Brain Lang. 74, 1–25. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenig, T., and Lehmann, D. (1996). Microstates in language-related brain potential maps show noun–verb differences. Brain Lang. 53, 169–182. doi: 10.1006/brln.1996.0043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krishnan, G., Tiwari, S., Pai, A. R., and Rao, S. N. (2012). Variability in aphasia following subcortical hemorrhagic lesion. Ann. Neurosci. 19, 158–160. doi: 10.5214/ans.0972.7531.190404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutas, M., and Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 621–647. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lau, E. F., Phillips, C., and Poeppel, D. (2008). A cortical network for semantics: (de)constructing the N400. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 920–933. doi: 10.1038/nrn2532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, C., and Federmeier, K. D. (2006). To mind the mind: an event-related potential study of word class and semantic ambiguity. Brain Res. 1081, 191–202. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmann, M., Barnes, J., Ridgway, G. R., Ryan, N. S., Warrington, E. K., Crutch, S. J., et al. (2012). Global gray matter changes in posterior cortical atrophy: a serial imaging study. Alzheimers Dement. 8, 502–512. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.09.225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmann, M., Crutch, S. J., Ridgway, G. R., Ridha, B. H., Barnes, J., Warrington, E. K., et al. (2011). Cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry in posterior cortical atrophy and typical Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 1466–1476. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liuzzi, G., Freundlieb, N., Ridder, V., Hoppe, J., Heise, K., Zimerman, M., et al. (2010). The involvement of the left motor cortex in learning of a novel action word lexicon. Curr. Biol. 20, 1745–1751. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.08.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luzzatti, C., Aggujaro, S., and Crepaldi, D. (2006). Verb-noun double dissociation in aphasia: theoretical and neuroanatomical foundations. Cortex 42, 875–883. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70431-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luzzatti, C., Raggi, R., Zonca, G., Pistarini, C., Contardi, A., and Pinna, G. D. (2002). Verb-noun double dissociation in aphasic lexical impairments: the role of word frequency and imageability. Brain Lang. 81, 432–444. doi: 10.1006/brln.2001.2536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manto, M., Bower, J. M., Conforto, A. B., Delgado-García, J. M., da Guarda, S. N. F., Gerwig, M., et al. (2012). Consensus paper: roles of the cerebellum in motor control—The diversity of ideas on cerebellar involvement in movement. Cerebellum 11, 457–487. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0331-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcotte, K., Graham, N., Black, S., and Leonard, C. (2014). Verb production in the nonfluent and semantic variants of primary progressive aphasia: the influence of lexical and semantic factors. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 31, 565–583. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2014.970154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, J., Pring, T., and Chiat, S. (1998). Verb retrieval and sentence production in aphasia. Brain Lang. 63, 159–183. doi: 10.1006/brln.1998.31949

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, P. J. (2016). Embodiment and human development. Child Dev. Perspect. 10, 245–250. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, A., Haxby, J. V., Lalonde, F. M., Wiggs, C. L., and Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of color and knowledge of action. Science 270, 102–105. doi: 10.1126/science.270.5233.102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mätzig, S., Druk, J., Masterson, J., and Vigliocco, G. (2009). Noun and verb differences in picture naming: past studies and new evidence. Cortex 45, 738–758. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMonagle, P., Deering, F., Berliner, Y., and Kertesz, A. (2006). The cognitive profile of posterior cortical atrophy. Neurology 66, 331–338. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000196477.78548.db

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melloni, M., Billeke, P., Baez, S., Hesse, E., de la Fuente, L., Forno, G., et al. (2016). Your perspective and my benefit: multiple lesion models of self-other integration strategies during social bargaining. Brain 139, 3022–3040. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meteyard, L., Price, C. J., Woollams, A. M., and Aydelott, J. (2013). Lesions impairing regular versus irregular past tense production. Neuroimage 3, 438–449. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miceli, G., Silveri, M. C., Nocentini, U., and Caramazza, A. (1988). Patterns of dissociation in comprehension and production of nouns and verbs. Aphasiology 2, 351–358. doi: 10.1080/02687038808248937

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Migliaccio, R., Agosta, F., Rascovsky, K., Karydas, A., Bonasera, S., Rabinovici, G. D., et al. (2009). Clinical syndromes associated with posterior atrophy early age at onset AD spectrum. Neurology 73, 1571–1578. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181c0d427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Migliaccio, R., Agosta, F., Toba, M. N., Samri, D., Corlier, F., De Souza, L. C., et al. (2012). Brain networks in posterior cortical atrophy: a single case tractography study and literature review. Cortex 48, 1298–1309. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moseley, R. L., and Pulvermüller, F. (2014). Noun, verbs, objects, actions and abstractions: local fMRI activity indexes semantics, not lexical categories. Brain Lang. 132, 28–42. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nespoulous, J.-L. (2000). Invariance vs variability in aphasic performance. An example: agrammatism. Brain Lang. 71, 167–171. doi: 10.1006/brln.1999.2242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nichols, T. E., Das, S., Eickhoff, S. B., Evans, A. C., Glatard, T., Hanke, M., et al. (2017). Best practices in data analysis and sharing in neuroimaging using MRI. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 299–303. doi: 10.1038/nn.4500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panegyres, P. K., Goh, J., McCarthy, M., and Campbell, A. I. (2017). The nature and natural history of posterior cortical atrophy syndrome: a variant of early-onset Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 31, 295–306. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., and Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 976–987. doi: 10.1038/nrn2277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perani, D., Cappa, S. F., Schnur, T., Tettamanti, M., Collina, S., Rosa, M. M., et al. (1999). The neural correlates of verb and noun processing. A PET study. Brain 122(Pt 12), 2337–2344. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.12.2337

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poldrack, R. A., Baker, C. I., Durnez, J., Gorgolewski, K. J., Matthews, P. M., Munafo, M. R., et al. (2017). Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 115–126. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poldrack, R. A., Fletcher, P. C., Henson, R. N., Worsley, K. J., Brett, M., and Nichols, T. E. (2008). Guidelines for reporting an fMRI study. Neuroimage 40, 409–414. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 576–582. doi: 10.1038/nrn1706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F. (2018). Neural reuse of action perception circuits for language, concepts and communication. Prog. Neurobiol. 160, 1–44. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2017.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F., Lutzenberger, W., and Preissl, H. (1999). Nouns and verbs in the intact brain: evidence from event-related potentials and high-frequency cortical responses. Cereb. Cortex 9, 497–506. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.5.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ralph, M. A., Jefferies, E., Patterson, K., and Rogers, T. T. (2017). The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 42–55. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, N. S., Shakespeare, T. J., Lehmann, M., Keihaninejad, S., Nicholas, J. M., Leung, K. K., et al. (2014). Motor features in posterior cortical atrophy and their imaging correlates. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 2845–2857. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.05.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidtke, K., Hüll, M., and Talazko, J. (2005). Posterior cortical atrophy: variant of Alzheimer’s disease? J. Neurol. 252, 27–35. doi: 10.1007/s00415-005-0594-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedeño, L., Couto, B., Melloni, M., Canales-Johnson, A., Yoris, A., Baez, S., et al. (2014). How do you feel when you can’t feel your body? Interoception, functional connectivity and emotional processing in depersonalization-derealization disorder. PLoS One 9:e98769. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedeño, L., Piguet, O., Abrevaya, S., Desmaras, H., García-Cordero, I., Baez, S., et al. (2017). Tackling variability: a multicenter study to provide a gold-standard network approach for frontotemporal dementia. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 3804–3822. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seghier, M. L. (2013). The angular gyrus: multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. Neuroscientist 19, 43–61. doi: 10.1177/1073858412440596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seguin, J., Formaglio, M., Perret-Liaudet, A., Quadrio, I., Tholance, Y., Rouaud, O., et al. (2011). CSF biomarkers in posterior cortical atrophy. Neurology 76, 1782–1788. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821ccc98

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, K. A., and Caramazza, A. (2003). Grammatical processing of nouns and verbs in left frontal cortex? Neuropsychologia 41, 1189–1198. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00037-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, K. A., Moo, L. R., and Caramazza, A. (2006). Cortical signatures of noun and verb production. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 1644–1649. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504142103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, K. A., Pascual-Leone, A., Mottaghy, F. M., Gangitano, M., and Caramazza, A. (2001). Grammatical distinctions in the left frontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 713–720. doi: 10.1162/08989290152541386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shebani, Z., Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., Diaz-de-Grenu, L. Z., Dawson, K., and Pulvermüller, F. (2017). Semantic word category processing in semantic dementia and posterior cortical atrophy. Cortex 93, 92–106. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silveri, M. C., Traficante, D., Monaco, M. R. L., Iori, L., Sarchioni, F., and Burani, C. (2017). Word selection processing in Parkinson’s disease: when nouns are more difficult than verbs. Cortex 100, 8–20. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.05.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sommer, M. A. (2003). The role of the thalamus in motor control. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 663–670. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2003.10.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spinelli, E. G., Caso, F., Agosta, F., Gambina, G., Magnani, G., Canu, E., et al. (2015). A multimodal neuroimaging study of a case of crossed nonfluent/agrammatic primary progressive aphasia. J. Neurol. 262, 2336–2345. doi: 10.1007/s00415-015-7845-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Straube, T., Weisbrod, A., Schmidt, S., Raschdorf, C., Preul, C., Mentzel, H. J., et al. (2010). No impairment of recognition and experience of disgust in a patient with a right-hemispheric lesion of the insula and basal ganglia. Neuropsychologia 48, 1735–1741. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang-Wai, D. F., Graff-Radford, N. R., Boeve, B. F., Dickson, D. W., Parisi, J. E., Crook, R., et al. (2004). Clinical, genetic, and neuropathologic characteristics of posterior cortical atrophy. Neurology 63, 1168–1174. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000140289.18472.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, L. J., Evans, C., Greer, J., Senior, C., Coventry, K. R., and Ietswaart, M. (2017). Dissociation between semantic representations for motion and action verbs: evidence from patients with left hemisphere lesions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:35. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, C. K. (2003). Unaccusative verb production in agrammatic aphasia: the argument structure complexity hypothesis. J. Neurolinguistics 16, 151–167. doi: 10.1016/S0911-6044(02)00014-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, C. K., Lukic, S., King, M. C., Mesulam, M. M., and Weintraub, S. (2012). Verb and noun deficits in stroke-induced and primary progressive aphasia: the Northwestern Naming Battery. Aphasiology 26, 632–655. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2012.676852

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasino, B., Fink, G. R., Sparing, R., Dafotakis, M., and Weiss, P. H. (2008). Action verbs and the primary motor cortex: a comparative TMS study of silent reading, frequency judgments, and motor imagery. Neuropsychologia 46, 1915–1926. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevisan, P., Sedeño, L., Birba, A., Ibáñ, A., and García, A. M. (2017). A moving story: whole-body motor training selectively improves the appraisal of action meanings in naturalistic narratives. Sci. Rep. 7:12538. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12928-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, K. L., Bright, P., Fletcher, P., and Stamatakis, E. A. (2004). Neural processing of nouns and verbs: the role of inflectional morphology. Neuropsychologia 42, 512–523. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, L. K., Randall, B., and Stamatakis, E. A. (2008). Cortical differentiation for nouns and verbs depends on grammatical markers. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1381–1389. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Druks, J., Barber, H., and Cappa, S. F. (2011). Nouns and verbs in the brain: a review of behavioural, electrophysiological, neuropsychological and imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 407–426. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wellsby, M., and Pexman, P. M. (2014). Developing embodied cognition: insights from children’s concepts and language processing. Front. Psychol. 5:506. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitwell, J. L., Jack, C. R., Kantarci, K., Weigand, S. D., Boeve, B. F., Knopman, D. S., et al. (2007a). Imaging correlates of posterior cortical atrophy. Neurobiol. Aging 28, 1051–1061. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.05.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitwell, J. L., Sampson, E. L., Loy, C. T., Warren, J. E., Rossor, M. N., Fox, N. C., et al. (2007b). VBM signatures of abnormal eating behaviours in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neuroimage 35, 207–213. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zamboni, G., Huey, E. D., Krueger, F., Nichelli, P. F., and Grafman, J. (2008). Apathy and disinhibition in frontotemporal dementia: insights into their neural correlates. Neurology 71, 736–742. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000324920.96835.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zingeser, L. B., and Berndt, R. S. (1990). Retrieval of nouns and verbs in agrammatism and anomia. Brain Lang. 39, 14–32. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(90)90002-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwaan, R. A. (2014). Embodiment and language comprehension: reframing the discussion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 229–234. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: noun-verb dissociation, embodied cognition, neurodegeneration, posterior cortical atrophy, verbal fluency, MRI

Citation: Steeb B, García-Cordero I, Huizing MC, Collazo L, Borovinsky G, Ferrari J, Cuitiño MM, Ibáñez A, Sedeño L and García AM (2018) Progressive Compromise of Nouns and Action Verbs in Posterior Cortical Atrophy. Front. Psychol. 9:1345. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01345

Received: 16 March 2018; Accepted: 13 July 2018;
Published: 03 August 2018.

Edited by:

Lorys Castelli, Università degli Studi di Torino, Italy

Reviewed by:

Anna M. Borghi, Sapienza Università di Roma, Italy
Claudia Gianelli, Universität Potsdam, Germany

Copyright © 2018 Steeb, García-Cordero, Huizing, Collazo, Borovinsky, Ferrari, Cuitiño, Ibáñez, Sedeño and García. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Adolfo M. García, adolfomartingarcia@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.