Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 20 December 2018
Sec. Cognition

Adjusting Sample Sizes for Different Categories of Embodied Cognition Research

\r\nAlexander Skulmowski*Alexander Skulmowski*Günter Daniel ReyGünter Daniel Rey
  • Psychology of Learning with Digital Media, Chemnitz University of Technology, Chemnitz, Germany

Introduction

Research in the field of embodied cognition is occupied with a variety of research questions stemming from the idea that cognition is deeply connected with bodily aspects such as perception and action (Barsalou, 1999, 2008). However, some embodiment studies have been identified to exhibit problems such as non-replicable results (Lakens, 2014). With this article, we wish to accomplish three aims: exemplifying ways of categorizing embodied cognition research in an informative manner; providing guidelines on how to identify problematic study designs; suggesting solutions for potentially problematic designs.

Within the field of embodied cognition, several aspects are investigated as outlined by Wilson (2002). One example for embodiment mentioned by Wilson (2002) is gesturing (for an overview on gesturing, see Hostetter and Alibali, 2008). Embodied cognition theory can be used to analyze the relation between gestures and mental processes (e.g., Hostetter and Alibali, 2008). Furthermore, there is a debate around the question whether language and meaning are grounded in perceptual contents experienced through the body (e.g., Borghi et al., 2004; for an overview on grounded cognition, see Barsalou, 2010). Besides research on cognition, principles of embodied cognition have been applied to fields such as social psychology (see Meier et al., 2012, for an overview) and educational psychology (see Paas and Sweller, 2012, for an overview). For instance, research on embodiment in the context of social cognition has provided evidence for the claim that bodily sensations such as weight can alter judgments on importance (e.g., Ackerman et al., 2010). In educational psychology, one application of embodiment theory is the design of interactive learning environments (e.g., Johnson-Glenberg et al., 2014).

In response to the current replication crisis in psychology (for discussions, see Pashler and Wagenmakers, 2012; Maxwell et al., 2015), several solutions have been proposed to improve the quality of psychological research (e.g., Chambers, 2013; Simons, 2014; LeBel, 2015; for overviews, see Ferguson, 2015; Zwaan et al., 2017). Benjamin et al. (2018) argue for a change of the standard 0.05 alpha level and instead support to lower the default alpha value for novel findings in the field of psychology to 0.005. Importantly, the sample size and power of studies have been described as pivotal contributors to replicable results (Fraley and Vazire, 2014).

Multiple types of embodied cognition research are facing the problem of delivering non-replicable results as discussed in the literature (e.g., Rabelo et al., 2015). Perugini et al. (2014) present a method for the calculation of sample sizes for replication studies and confirmatory research that takes into account that observed effect sizes may be inaccurate estimates. They suggest to conduct sample size calculations using an effect size that is based on the lower bounds of the confidence interval computed for an observed effect size (Perugini et al., 2014). Another method is presented by Simonsohn (2015), who makes the argument that sample size calculations for replication studies should not merely use the effect sizes reported in the original research that is to be replicated. He explains that by increasing the sample size by the factor of 2.5, a replication study can be used to assess whether an effect is too small to have been appropriately captured in the original study (Simonsohn, 2015). This method has already been used in a recent replication study on embodied cognition effects (Ronay et al., 2017). We suggest to use one of the aforementioned methods of sample size calculation for studies involving embodiment-based manipulation types that are known for potential problems. In the following, we will present three important aspects that can be used to check whether an embodied cognition study design will need amendments such as an increased sample size.

Identifying Problematic Embodiment Research

We wish to present three ways of assessing embodied cognition research: (1) categorization in reference to the dimensions of bodily engagement and task integration (based on Skulmowski and Rey, 2018); (2) categorization using the directness of an embodiment manipulation (based on ideas presented by Lee and Schwarz, 2014); (3) considering moderators and boundary conditions (based on Fay and Maner, 2015; see also, e.g., Maglio and Trope, 2012; Kaspar et al., 2016; Skulmowski and Rey, 2017a).

Bodily Engagement and Task Integration

Skulmowski and Rey (2018) presented a taxonomy of embodied learning research that hinges upon the two dimensions bodily engagement and task integration. They define low levels of bodily engagement as tasks with only minor bodily movement that occurs while sitting (Skulmowski and Rey, 2018). High levels of bodily engagement are considered to be tasks in which locomotion or other extensive forms of bodily movement are required (Skulmowski and Rey, 2018). Skulmowski and Rey (2018) ground the notion of task integration in Wilson and Golonka's (2013) characterization of embodied cognition as a task-bound utilization of bodily resources. In the taxonomy of Skulmowski and Rey (2018), incidental manipulations are said to be reliant on cues that alter cognitive variables while Skulmowski and Rey (2018) state integrated manipulations to be strongly intertwined with tasks.

Although the taxonomy of Skulmowski and Rey (2018) is mainly focused on embodied learning, in this paper we highlight applications of this taxonomy to decisions concerning virtually all types of embodied cognition research. Most importantly, Skulmowski and Rey (2018) discuss how the power of a study can be affected by the location of the study on the matrix of their taxonomy. By comparing the results of two study series utilizing similar types of incidental weight manipulations in a learning task (Alban and Kelley, 2013; Skulmowski and Rey, 2017a), Skulmowski and Rey (2018) conclude that a higher degree of bodily engagement can increase effect sizes. Turning to the factor of task integration, based on studies such as Mavilidi et al. (2015), Skulmowski and Rey (2018) state that integrated embodied learning manipulations have led to better learning results compared to non-integrated forms in which an embodiment manipulation does not have a connection to the learning contents. Based on these two conclusions regarding the two dimensions of the taxonomy, we derive the assumption that embodied cognition research may in general thought to be most robust when both the level of bodily engagement and the degree of task integration are high. Conversely, as stated by Skulmowski and Rey (2018), incidental manipulations with minor bodily engagement can lead to smaller effects. In addition, such designs may even result in non-reproducible effects (e.g., Rabelo et al., 2015). However, we emphasize that meta-analyses are necessary to provide concrete evidence concerning the relation between the robustness of results and the location on the grid of the 2 × 2 taxonomy (i.e., low bodily engagement + high task integration and high bodily engagement + low task integration). In sum, study designs which are low both in bodily engagement and task integration according to the taxonomy of Skulmowski and Rey (2018) should be considered as potentially problematic.

Categorization Based on Directness

Besides embodiment studies that are focused on tasks, others revolve around metaphor-based effects and priming (Wilson and Golonka, 2013). An appropriate way to categorize this type of study relies on the directness of the embodiment manipulation as defined by Lee and Schwarz (2014). Lee and Schwarz (2014) discuss embodied cognition studies based on Lakoff and Johnson's (1999) metaphor model of cognition. Lakoff and Johnson's (1999) theory states that conceptual knowledge heavily relies on metaphors and that cognitive processes are influenced by the body. Lee and Schwarz (2014) summarize several metaphoric relations that have been utilized in embodied cognition research, such as the idea that “fishy” smells are connected to the feeling of suspicion based on the corresponding metaphorical relation found in the English language (they cite Lee and Schwarz, 2012, as an example). In addition, Lee and Schwarz (2014) present several types of embodiment studies (see Skulmowski and Rey, 2017a, for a discussion of these types). Lee and Schwarz (2014) acknowledge that there are embodiment effects not relying on metaphors, such as physiological effects, and call them “[d]irect, non-metaphoric effects” (p. 100, italics removed). Furthermore, among other types, they describe metaphor-based studies which affect judgments based on how the perception of a setting is manipulated by bodily, metaphor-based influences (Lee and Schwarz, 2014). In addition, Lee and Schwarz (2014) point out that there are metaphor-based embodiment effects that depend on participants' lack of awareness of the embodiment manipulation. It needs to be noted that Lee and Schwarz (2014) discuss these aspects as they relate to incidental study designs with a focus on the field of decision-making. However, we want to generalize certain aspects of their overview for the entire field of embodied cognition research. Embodied cognition research could be viewed on a continuum ranging from direct effects as defined by Lee and Schwarz (2014) to indirect effects (comparable to the metaphorical effects described by Lee and Schwarz, 2014).

Lakens (2014) reviews several failed replication attempts that match the criteria for indirect effects and concludes that one may have doubts concerning such types of embodiment studies. In line with Laken's (2014) statements, we support the idea that studies falling on the indirect end of the spectrum should use larger sample sizes. Generally speaking, there have been several criticisms directed toward study designs involving metaphor-based manipulations (e.g., Lakens, 2014; Pecher, 2017; Skulmowski and Rey, 2017a). Based on these grounds, we argue to treat such designs as potentially problematic.

Adjustments Based on Moderating Factors and Boundary Conditions

Fay and Maner (2015) recently used moderating factors and boundary conditions as an explanation for the non-replicability of certain embodiment effects. According to their view, instances in which findings concerning embodiment manipulations were not replicable may be the result of moderators or boundary conditions that were not considered (Fay and Maner, 2015). Therefore, they argue for giving more thought to moderators and boundary conditions in embodiment research (Fay and Maner, 2015). Recent examples include the moderating factor gender (e.g., Kaspar et al., 2016) as well as the effects of the cognitive mode activated during a task (Maglio and Trope, 2012; Skulmowski and Rey, 2017a).

In addition, it should be noted that some types of measures are more appropriate for embodied cognition research than others (Meier et al., 2012), for example when assessing cognitive load (Skulmowski and Rey, 2017b). In sum, researchers should be aware of potential moderators and boundary conditions when planning their embodiment studies.

Conclusion

In light of the current controversy surrounding the replicability of psychological science, we wish to emphasize a variety of aspects that may warrant a higher sample size or other amendments to embodied cognition study designs. One method of assessment concerns the degree of bodily engagement and the extent to which an embodiment manipulation affects how a task can be solved as described by Skulmowski and Rey (2018). Another method of categorizing studies presented in this paper is a continuum between direct and indirect effects (based on theoretical considerations of Lee and Schwarz, 2014). As outlined above, strengthening the factor of bodily engagement may increase effect sizes (Skulmowski and Rey, 2018). Lastly, we emphasized that embodied cognition study designs should check for moderating effects and boundary conditions as studies (e.g., Skulmowski and Rey, 2017a) have demonstrated that even strong bodily manipulations need to be directed at suitable processing modes. These three factors clearly are not an exhaustive list of factors that need to be kept in mind when planning an embodied cognition study. However, we argue that the three aspects discussed in this paper may help to identify potentially problematic study designs; and that using one of the methods described by Perugini et al. (2014) and Simonsohn (2015) could help to improve embodiment research. Additional research needs to be done to determine more precise guidelines based on meta-analyses that could be grounded in the models presented in this paper.

Author Contributions

AS and GDR drafted and revised the article and approved the submitted version for publication.

Funding

We thank the German Research Foundation/DFG for funding (GRK 1780/1). The publication costs of this article were funded by the German Research Foundation/DFG and the Technische Universität Chemnitz in the funding program Open Access Publishing.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ackerman, J. M., Nocera, C. C., and Bargh, J. A. (2010). Incidental haptic sensations influence social judgments and decisions. Science 328, 1712–1715. doi: 10.1126/science.1189993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alban, M. W., and Kelley, C. M. (2013). Embodiment meets metamemory: weight as a cue for metacognitive judgments. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 39, 1628–1634. doi: 10.1037/a0032420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptions of perceptual symbols. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 637–660. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99532147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 617–645. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded cognition: past, present, and future. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2, 716–724. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamin, D. J., Berger, J. O., Johannesson, M., Nosek, B. A., Wagenmakers, E. J., Berk, R., et al. (2018). Redefine statistical significance. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 6–10. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0189-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghi, A. M., Glenberg, A. M., and Kaschak, M. P. (2004). Putting words in perspective. Mem. Cogn. 32, 863–873. doi: 10.3758/BF03196865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chambers, C. D. (2013). Registered reports: a new publishing initiative at Cortex. Cortex 49, 609–610. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fay, A. J., and Maner, J. K. (2015). Embodied effects are moderated by situational cues: warmth, threat, and the desire for affiliation. Brit. J. Soc. Psychol. 54, 291–305. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferguson, C. J. (2015). “Everybody knows psychology is not a real science”: Public perceptions of psychology and how we can improve our relationship with policymakers, the scientific community, and the general public. Am. Psychol. 70, 527–542. doi: 10.1037/a0039405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraley, R. C., and Vazire, S. (2014). The N-pact factor: evaluating the quality of empirical journals with respect to sample size and statistical power. PLoS ONE 9:e109019. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0109019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hostetter, A. B., and Alibali, M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: gestures as simulated action. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 15, 495–514. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.3.495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson-Glenberg, M. C., Birchfield, D. A., Tolentino, L., and Koziupa, T. (2014). Collaborative embodied learning in mixed reality motion-capture environments: two science studies. J. Educ. Psychol. 106, 86–104. doi: 10.1037/a0034008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaspar, K., Jurisch, A., and Schneider, M. (2016). Embodied cognition and humor: the impact of weight sensations on humor experience and the moderating role of gender. Curr. Psychol. 35, 377–385. doi: 10.1007/s12144-015-9304-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakens, D. (2014). Grounding social embodiment. Soc. Cogn. 32, 168–183. doi: 10.1521/soco.2014.32.supp.168

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to Western Thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

LeBel, E. P. (2015). A new replication norm for psychology. Collabra. 1:4 doi: 10.1525/collabra.23

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. W. S., and Schwarz, N. (2012). Bidirectionality, mediation, and moderation of metaphorical effects: the embodiment of social suspicion and fishy smells. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 103, 737–749. doi: 10.1037/a0029708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. W. S., and Schwarz, N. (2014). “Metaphors in judgment and decision making,” in The Power of Metaphor: Examining Its Influence on Social Life, eds M. J. Landau, M. D. Robinson, and B. P. Meier (Washington, DC: APA), 85–108.

Google Scholar

Maglio, S. J., and Trope, Y. (2012). Disembodiment: abstract construal attenuates the influence of contextual bodily state in judgment. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 211–216. doi: 10.1037/a0024520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mavilidi, M. F., Okely, A. D., Chandler, P., Cliff, D. P., and Paas, F. (2015). Effects of integrated physical exercises and gestures on preschool children's foreign language vocabulary learning. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 27, 413–426. doi: 10.1007/s10648-015-9337-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maxwell, S. E., Lau, M. Y., and Howard, G. S. (2015). Is psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does “failure to replicate” really mean? Am. Psychol. 70, 487–498. doi: 10.1037/a0039400

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meier, B. P., Schnall, S., Schwarz, N., and Bargh, J. A. (2012). Embodiment in social psychology. Top. Cogn. Sci. 4, 705–716. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01212.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paas, F., and Sweller, J. (2012). An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 24, 27–45. doi: 10.1007/s10648-011-9179-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pashler, H., and Wagenmakers, E. J. (2012). Editors' introduction to the special section on replicability in psychological science: a crisis of confidence? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 528–530. doi: 10.1177/1745691612465253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pecher, D. (2017). Curb Your Embodiment. Top. Cogn. Sci. 10, 501–517. doi: 10.1111/tops.12311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perugini, M., Gallucci, M., and Costantini, G. (2014). Safeguard power as a protection against imprecise power estimates. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 9, 319–332. doi: 10.1177/1745691614528519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabelo, A. L., Keller, V. N., Pilati, R., and Wicherts, J. M. (2015). No effect of weight on judgments of importance in the moral domain and evidence of publication bias from a meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 10:e0134808. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ronay, R., Tybur, J. M., van Huijstee, D., and Morssinkhof, M. (2017). Embodied power, testosterone, and overconfidence as a causal pathway to risk-taking. Comprehens. Results Soc. Psychol. 2, 28–43. doi: 10.1080/23743603.2016.1248081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simons, D. J. (2014). The value of direct replication. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 9, 76–80. doi: 10.1177/1745691613514755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simonsohn, U. (2015). Small telescopes: detectability and the evaluation of replication results. Psychol. Sci. 26, 559–569. doi: 10.1177/0956797614567341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skulmowski, A., and Rey, G. D. (2017a). Bodily effort enhances learning and metacognition: investigating the relation between physical effort and cognition using dual-process models of embodiment. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 13, 3–10. doi: 10.5709/acp-0202-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skulmowski, A., and Rey, G. D. (2017b). Measuring cognitive load in embodied learning settings. Front. Psychol. 8:1191. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skulmowski, A., and Rey, G. D. (2018). Embodied learning: introducing a taxonomy based on bodily engagement and task integration. Cogn. Res. Principles Implicat. 3:6. doi: 10.1186/s41235-018-0092-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, A. D., and Golonka, S. (2013). Embodied cognition is not what you think it is. Front. Psychol. 4:58. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 625–636. doi: 10.3758/BF03196322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwaan, R. A., Etz, A., Lucas, R. E., and Donnellan, M. B. (2017). Making replication mainstream. Behav. Brain Sci. 41:e120 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X17001972

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive psychology, embodied cognition, replicability, methodology, statistics

Citation: Skulmowski A and Rey GD (2018) Adjusting Sample Sizes for Different Categories of Embodied Cognition Research. Front. Psychol. 9:2384. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02384

Received: 03 September 2018; Accepted: 12 November 2018;
Published: 20 December 2018.

Edited by:

Claudia Repetto, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Italy
Anna M. Borghi, La Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2018 Skulmowski and Rey. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alexander Skulmowski, alexander.skulmowski@phil.tu-chemnitz.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.