Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 08 March 2019
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

Mindfulness and Symptoms of Depression and Anxiety in the General Population: The Mediating Roles of Worry, Rumination, Reappraisal and Suppression

  • 1Department of Psychology and Institute of Health Sciences (iUNICS), University of the Balearic Islands, Palma, Spain
  • 2Balearic Islands Health Research Institute (IdISBa), Palma, Spain
  • 3School of Psychology, The University of Western Australia, Perth, WA, Australia
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, Miguel Servet University Hospital, Zaragoza, Spain
  • 5Department of Nursing and Physiotherapy, University of the Balearic Islands, Palma, Spain

The present study examined the effects of mindfulness on depression and anxiety, both direct and indirect through the mediation of four mechanisms of emotional regulation: worry, rumination, reappraisal and suppression. Path analysis was applied to data collected from an international and non-clinical sample of 1151 adults, including both meditators and non-meditators, who completed an online questionnaire battery. Our results show that mindfulness are related to lower levels of depression and anxiety both directly and indirectly. Suppression, reappraisal, worry and rumination all acted as significant mediators of the relationship between mindfulness and depression. A similar picture emerged for the relationship between mindfulness and anxiety, with the difference that suppression was not a mediator. Our data also revealed that the estimated number of hours of mindfulness meditation practice did not affect depression or anxiety directly but did reduce these indirectly by increasing mindfulness. Worry and rumination proved to be the most potent mediating variables. Altogether, our results confirm that emotional regulation plays a significant mediating role between mindfulness and symptoms of depression and anxiety in the general population and suggest that meditation focusing on reducing worry and rumination may be especially useful in reducing the risk of developing clinical depression.

Introduction

Mindfulness, the process by which one attends to present-moment sensations, thoughts, emotions and experiences in a non-judgmental manner (e.g., Kabat-Zinn et al., 1992; Marlatt and Kristeller, 1999), has been reported to exert beneficial effects on health and well-being, both in non-clinical (Brown and Ryan, 2003; Coffey et al., 2010; Bränström et al., 2011; Tan and Martin, 2016; Demarzo et al., 2017) and clinical (e.g., Sephton et al., 2007; Galante et al., 2013; Kuyken et al., 2015; Crane et al., 2017; Mitchell et al., 2017; Chu et al., 2018) samples. Of particular interest in the present study, mindfulness, whether dispositional or developed through meditation or intervention, has consistently been associated with lower rates of depression and anxiety (Baer, 2006; Carmody and Baer, 2008; Hofmann et al., 2010; Desrosiers et al., 2013; Tran et al., 2014), including in the general population (Freudenthaler et al., 2017). Since depression and anxiety have major impacts on well-being and are two of the most prevalent psychiatric disorders among primary care patients (Ormel et al., 1994), with sub-clinical symptoms also affecting the general population (Lacruz et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2016), understanding the mechanisms through which mindfulness may impact positively on such symptoms commands interest. While the issue has received an increasing amount of attention in recent years, much remains to investigate regarding the mechanisms underpinning this effect. In this study, we explore the relationship between mindfulness and anxiety and depression in a large sample and within a single statistical model, examining the role of a series of mediators (suppression, reappraisal, ruminations, and worry), and assessing for the first time the role of meditation experience as an independent factor.

Some evidence suggests that emotional regulation, the set of strategies and processes that shape the experience and expression of emotions (Gross, 1998), impact on well-being (Gross and John, 2003) and may play a mediating role between mindfulness and depression. On the one hand, links have been reported between mindfulness and emotional regulation in clinical samples (Desrosiers et al., 2013), in meditators (Tran et al., 2014), and in the general population (Freudenthaler et al., 2017). On the other hand, deficits of emotional regulation are frequently observed in patients suffering from clinical depression or anxiety disorders (Abercrombie et al., 1998; Etkin et al., 2004; Garnefski and Kraaij, 2006; Radkovsky et al., 2014). One prevalent and relevant distinction is that between cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression (Goldin et al., 2008; Cutuli, 2014). Reappraisal is a cognitive strategy modifying emotional responses through a reformulation of the meaning of a situation. Expressive suppression, in contrast, is a strategy aimed at inhibiting behaviors associated with emotional responses (e.g., facial expressions), reducing the expression of emotions but not their experience (Gross, 2002). Expressive suppression is regarded as a maladaptive strategy and is positively correlated with depression and anxiety (Campbell-Sills et al., 2006). Reappraisal, in contrast, is negatively associated with depression and anxiety (Martin and Dahlen, 2005; Desrosiers et al., 2013; Garland et al., 2011; Peh et al., 2017). Importantly, reappraisal is an essential component of mindfulness meditation and interventions (Goleman and Schwartz, 1976; Feldman et al., 2010; Jha et al., 2010), and has been highlighted as one of the mechanisms through which mindfulness works (Hölzel et al., 2011). Expressive suppression, on the other hand, runs contrary to an important aspect of mindfulness, namely the awareness and acceptance of ones’ emotions (Bishop et al., 2004), but fits with the avoidance behavior common in depression and anxiety disorders (e.g., Struijs et al., 2017). In line with these contentions, mindfulness has been found to be positively associated with cognitive reappraisal but negatively so with expressive suppression (Garland et al., 2011; Hölzel et al., 2011; Froeliger et al., 2012; Desrosiers et al., 2013; Brockman et al., 2017).

Apart from cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression, other emotional regulation mechanisms have been documented, such as rumination and worry. Rumination consists in repetitive and intrusive thoughts about past negative emotional experiences, including their perceived cause and expected consequences. Several studies reported a negative relationship between rumination and mindfulness in community samples (Kumar et al., 2008; Feldman et al., 2010; Raes and Williams, 2010), as well as a reduction of depressive symptoms in individuals with remitted major depressive disorder who underwent mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (Shahar et al., 2010). Rumination is intrinsic to clinical depression (Joormann et al., 2006) and correlates with symptoms of depression and anxiety in community samples (Burwell and Shirk, 2007; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). Some researchers have suggested that rumination might mediate the negative relationship between mindfulness and depression (Desrosiers et al., 2013; Alleva et al., 2014) or anxiety (Desrosiers et al., 2013).

Worry can be described as “a chain of thoughts and images, negatively affect-laden, and relatively uncontrollable; it represents an attempt to engage in mental problem-solving on an issue of which outcome is uncertain but contains the possibility of one or more negative outcomes” (Borkovec et al., 1983, p. 10). Uncontrollable worry is a core feature of anxiety disorders (Hoyer et al., 2002, 2009) but also affects adults who do not qualify for such diagnostic (Ruscio et al., 2005; Olatunji et al., 2010). Though research on the effect of mindfulness on worry is relatively scarce, recent evidence indicates that worry decreases when dispositional mindfulness increases, and that mindfulness meditation is effective in reducing the negative impact of distressing images in habitual worriers (Verplanken and Fisher, 2014). Furthermore, mindfulness-based interventions appear to reduce worry in individuals with significant anxiety-related distress (Lenze et al., 2014). Both rumination and worry are characterized by their relative uncontrollability and are thought to reflect reduced cognitive control (Raes and Williams, 2010; Beckwé et al., 2014; Hallion et al., 2014). Mindfulness is characterized by the orienting of attention toward the present moment (as opposed to the past, as is characteristic of rumination, or to the future, as is the case of worry), and the non-judgmental awareness of the transient nature of thoughts and emotions.

In sum, evidence suggests that expressive suppression, worry and rumination are negatively associated with mindfulness, while reappraisal exhibit the opposite relationship. Importantly, evidence indicates that the first three contribute to depression and anxiety or their related symptoms, while the fourth reduce such symptoms. In this study, we sought to test the hypothesis that, in the general population, mindfulness might reduce symptoms of depression and anxiety through the reduction of worry, rumination and expressive suppression, and through the enhancement of reappraisal. While some studies reported findings speaking to this issue, these have focused on clinical samples (Desrosiers et al., 2013) or have considered difficulties in emotion regulation but not specific mechanisms of such regulation (Freudenthaler et al., 2017). Past studies are also heterogeneous with respect to their operationalization of mindfulness. Research on mindfulness typically includes three types of studies: studies based on interventions (e.g., the mindfulness-based stress reduction program or breathing exercises), comparisons of meditators and non-meditators, or studies focusing on dispositional mindfulness (but typically not controlling for, or factoring in, the meditation practice (MP) of participants). We would argue that the distinction between dispositional mindfulness and meditation, though not typically addressed in past studies, is an interesting one in relation to depression and anxiety. The distinction between dispositional and cultivated mindfulness (i.e., developed through meditation or mindfulness training) remains debated (Grossman et al., 2004; Rau and Williams, 2016; Tang et al., 2016; Wheeler et al., 2016). While one can score highly on a measure of dispositional mindfulness without practicing mindfulness meditation, evidence suggests that such meditation is associated with greater mindfulness (Shapiro et al., 2011; Soler et al., 2014) and has measurable effects on brain functioning (Tang et al., 2015). Though discussing the distinction between dispositional and cultivated mindfulness falls beyond the scope of our study, we would argue that both mindfulness level and experience of mindfulness meditation should be taken into account and distinguished when examining the impact of mindfulness on depression and anxiety. Indeed, meditation consists in specific practices that some have regarded as specifically promoting emotional regulation, cognitive control or attention (Tang et al., 2016). As such, MP might constitute a specific type of training and install or develop mechanisms absent or less developed in non-meditators.

We present below the results of a questionnaire study administered to a large, non-clinical, and international sample in which we measured dispositional mindfulness and obtained a numerical estimate of the amount of mindfulness practice accumulated by participants, together with measures of depression and anxiety and of four emotional regulation mechanisms: cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression, rumination and worry. We hypothesized that (1) mindfulness would lead to reduced levels of depression and anxiety; that (2) mechanisms of emotional regulation would mediate that effect; and (3) that mindfulness MP would contribute to mindfulness. Furthermore, we also examined the prediction that mindfulness meditation would affect depression and anxiety on its own right (that is, independently of its effect on mindfulness). Finally, we aimed to compare the relative contributions of the different emotional regulation mechanisms to the relationship between mindfulness and depression/anxiety.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 1494 participants took part in the study. After excluding participants who reported having been diagnosed with a psychological or psychiatric disorder (333) and participants whose level of education could not be ascertained with precision (10), our sample included 1151 adults (900 women) aged 18 to 76 (M = 36.34, SD = 14.34, Min = 18, Max = 76). Among these, 491 reported practicing mindfulness-based meditation (42.7%). All participants were English (N = 410) or Spanish speakers (N = 741) and completed the questionnaires in their language of preference (English or Spanish). The sample included participants from 39 countries (mostly from Spain, 53%, Canada, 17%, United States, 9%, Argentina, 5%, and United Kingdom, 4%). The participants’ level of education was scored as follows: primary school education (score of 1); secondary school, high school, vocation/technical school (score of 2); some higher education, graduate or university degree (score of 3); post-graduate studies such as MSc, PhD, or professional degree (score of 4). The mean level of education of our participants was 3.01 (SD = 0.756, Min = 1, Max = 4).

Procedure

Between May 2015 and August 2016, participants were recruited through email announcements (locally, nationally and internationally), and advertisements on online experiment websites for a study on mindfulness and attention or attention and distraction (this variation was introduced in order to attract participants with and without experience of MP). Data were collected through an online survey implemented using the survey development environment Qualtrics. Participation was voluntary and unremunerated. Participants did however receive personalized feedback with their results on the various questionnaires used. Participants first answered some demographic questions (age, sex, level of education) before indicating whether they had ever been diagnosed with any psychological or psychiatric disorder, and providing information regarding their history of meditative practice. They then completed the series of questionnaires described below, presented in a random order except for the questionnaire measuring mindfulness, which was always administered first. This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of American Psychological Association with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Bioethical Committee of the University of the Balearic Islands.

Measures

Mindfulness: Short Five-Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ)

We administered the short form of the FFMQ (Bohlmeijer et al., 2011) consisting of 24 items measuring five facets of mindfulness (Baer et al., 2006): Observe, Describe, Non-judging of Inner Experience, Acting with Awareness, and Non-reactivity to Inner Experience. Participants rated themselves on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = often, 5 = always). For the Spanish version, we used the equivalent items from the Spanish adaptation of the FFMQ (Cebolla et al., 2012). Items from all five subscales were used to compute a total mindfulness score. High scores indicate higher levels of mindfulness. The reliability of the FFMQ, measured in our sample was good (Chronbach’s α = 0.884).

Depression and Anxiety: Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS)

The HADS contains 14 items assessing levels of depression and anxiety (Zigmond and Snaith, 1983). Half the items measure depression (HADS-D), the others measure anxiety (HADS-A). For each item, the participants selected one of four responses (e.g., “I feel as if I am slowed down” with one of the following options: “nearly all the time,” “very often,” “sometimes,” or “not at all”). For the Spanish version, we used the items from the Spanish version of the HADS (De las Cuevas Castresana et al., 1995). Higher scores indicate greater levels of depression. The HADS-D and HADS-A measures exhibited good reliability, Chronbach’s α (measured in our sample) of 0.753 and 0.798, respectively.

Rumination: Leuven Adaptation of the Rumination on Sadness Scale (LARSS)

Rumination was measured using the 17-item LARSS (Raes et al., 2008) and our Spanish translation for Spanish speaking participants. This translation was carried out by a bilingual psychologist external to the study. This translated version was then translated back to English by the first author (bilingual English-Spanish) and compared to the original version to ensure its equivalence. Items (e.g., “I keep thinking about my problems to try and examine where things went wrong”) are rated on a five-point scale (not at all to very much). Higher scores indicate higher levels of rumination, Chronbach’s α = 0.957 (measured in our sample).

Worry: Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ)

The PSWQ is a questionnaire measuring the tendency to worry. In this study, we used the 3-item version of the PSWQ (Berle et al., 2011) and the equivalent items from the Spanish version (Sandin et al., 2009). Each item is rated on a 5-point scale (not at all typical to very typical). Higher scores indicate higher levels of worry. The PSWQ’s reliability, measured in our sample, was good, Chronbach’s α = 0.878.

Emotional Regulation: Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ)

We used the ERQ (Gross and John, 2003) and our Spanish translation (following the same procedure as for the LARSS questionnaire) to assess mechanisms of emotional regulation: cognitive reappraisal and emotive suppression. The ERQ includes 10 items divided in two subscales, one measuring reappraisal (ERQ-Reappraisal, 6 items) and one measuring suppression (ERQ-Suppression, 4 items). Each item is rated on a 7-point scale (from strongly disagree to strongly agree). Both sub-scales exhibited good levels of reliability (measured in our sample): Chronbach’s α = 0.856 for ERQ-R, and Chronbach’s α = 0.806 for ERQ-S.

History of Meditation

To measure the participants’ practice of mindfulness meditation, we first asked them whether they practiced or had practiced meditation. If they responded positively, we then assessed how frequently and for how long they practiced the following mindfulness-based types of meditation: Concentrative o Sadhana (e.g., breathing, mantras), Vipassana or contemplative, Visualization techniques, observation of body sensations (e.g., body scan, tai chi, yoga), compassion-based meditation, informal practice of mindfulness (e.g., doing daily activities in a mindful way), or other. The latter two were not regarded as evidence of formal MP. For each type of meditative practice, participants were asked (1) how frequent their MP was (daily, 3–4 times a week, once a week, or never); (2) how long they had been practicing since; and (3) how long their meditation sessions typically were. Based on each participant’s answers to these questions, we derived a numerical estimate of the total number of hours of MP using the following formula:

MP=t=15wft x 52 x nytxdt60

In this formula, t is the type of meditation (1 to 5; concentrative, Vipassana, Visualization, Body scan and Compassion type meditation), wft is the weekly frequency of meditation based on the participants’ report (7 for “daily”; 3.5 for “3 or 4 times a week,” 1 for “once a week,” 0 for “never”), nyt is the number of years of practice, and dt is the typical number of minutes of practice per session. For the data analysis, we used the log value of this estimated number of hours of MP.

Data Analysis

The questionnaires were set up in order to require all items to be completed. As a result, the data set analyzed below contained no missing data. The data were analyzed using Structural Equation Modeling and a confirmatory analysis approach. The analysis was conducted using AMOS v23 (Arbuckle, 2014). Parameter estimation was based on the maximum likelihood method (non-robust) based on the covariance matrix from the data. Model test statistics, direct and indirect effects were tested based on bootstrapped standard errors, with 5000 bootstrap samples, rendering our estimation method non-parametric (Nevitt and Hancock, 2001). Goodness-of-fit was assessed using the RMSEA and CFI indexes. A RMSEA value inferior to 0.06, and a CFI superior to 0.95, were deemed to offer good model fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999). We selected variables based on our initial hypothesis and we selected variables according to their statistical significance and modification indices of the initial model. To compare models, we calculated the difference in Ataike information criterion and tested it using a chi-square test.

Results

The descriptive statistics of the participants’ scores on the questionnaire measures described in the previous section are presented in Table 1. An initial structural equation model was designed in which depression and anxiety were predicted by mindfulness and meditation, four factors were introduced as mediators of the relationship between mindfulness and depression (reappraisal, suppression, ruminations and worry), and the effects of age, gender (1: female, 2: male), level of education (1–4), and language (1: English, 2: Spanish) on depression and anxiety were controlled for. This initial model revealed no association between level of education and depression (M = −0.012, SE = 0.026, 95% CI: −0.062 to 0.039, p = 0.599) or anxiety (M = 0.008, SE = 0.021, 95% CI: −0.033 to 0.49, p = 0.692), nor between gender and anxiety (M < 0.001, SE = 0.020, 95% CI: −0.040 to 0.038, p = 0.989), and no direct association between meditation and depression (M = −0.014, SE = 0.026, 95% CI: −0.065 to 0.038, p = 0.599) or anxiety (M = −0.011, SE = 0.020, 95% CI: −0.031 to 0.050, p = 0.620). Hence these associations were removed and the resulting, adjusted model, evaluated. This adjusted model is illustrated in Figure 1 (for visual clarity, the correlations among error terms and control variables included in the model are not presented in this figure but reported in Table 2). The adjusted model provided a good fit to the data [χ2(15) = 24.2, p = 0.062, CFI = 0.997, RMSEA = 0.023, 90% CI: 0.000 to 0.039, AIC = 126.176].

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the participants’ (N = 1151) scores on the variables measured in this study.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Direct and indirect relationships between mindfulness and depression/anxiety, taking into account the meditating role of emotional regulation (cognitive reappraisal and emotive suppression), rumination and worry. Numbers represent the standardized path coefficients. p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01, ∗∗∗p < 0.001.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Point estimates (M), standard error (SE), lower and upper bounds of the 95% confidence interval (Lower, Upper) and p-value (p) of the correlations allowed among error terms (e) and control variables in the adjusted model (these correlations were included in the model based on the modification indices of the initial model).

Given the potential theoretical importance of the lack of relationship between MP and depression/anxiety, we also performed partial correlations using bootstrapping (5000 samples) and calculated the Bayes Factor to ascertain whether meditation had any impact on either dependent variable when mindfulness was controlled for: No partial correlation was observed between the amount of MP and anxiety (r = −0.043, SE = 0.029, 95% CI: −0.100 to 0.014, p = 0.146, BF10 = 0.131) or between the amount of MP and depression (r = −0.003, SE = 0.029, 95% CI: −0.059 to 0.054 p = 0.930, BF10 = 0.056). These results demonstrate that meditation does not affect depression or anxiety when mindfulness is controlled for, and the Bayes Factors provide strong evidence in favor of the absence of correlation.

The statistical details of the path coefficients are presented in Table 3. As visible from Figure 1, mindfulness affected depression and anxiety both directly and indirectly. Cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression, worry and rumination mediated the relationship between mindfulness and depression: mindfulness increased cognitive reappraisal (negatively correlated with depression) while it decreased expressive suppression, worry and rumination (all positively correlated with depression). The direct effect of mindfulness on depression was significant (M = −0.234, SE = 0.031, 95% CI: −0.296 to −0.173, p < 0.001), as was its indirect effect (M = −0.270, SE = 0.022, 95% CI: −0.313 to −0.226, p < 0.001). A similar picture applied to the relationship between mindfulness and anxiety, with the difference that the meditation included cognitive reappraisal, worry and rumination (not expressive suppression). Mindfulness affected anxiety both directly (M = −0.235, SE = 0.025, 95% CI: −0.283 to −0.184, p < 0.001) and indirectly (M = −0.324, SE = 0.020, 95% CI: −0.364 to −0.285, p < 0.001). Finally, to ascertain the contribution and test the significance of the direct and indirect paths between mindfulness and depression and anxiety, we dropped them from the model (by setting their path weight to zero) and tested the significance of the resulting change in model fit (measured using Akaike’s Information Criterion, AIC, Akaike, 1974) using the chi-square test. As visible from Table 4, all paths made a significant contribution to the adjusted model fit. As reflected by the difference in AIC with and without these paths, the largest contribution to the model fit was provided by the mediation of the worry variable, followed by that of rumination, reappraisal, the direct path from mindfulness to anxiety, the mediation by suppression, and finally the direct path from mindfulness to depression. Finally, to confirm that MP did not affect depression and anxiety directly, we tested a modified model including these direct paths and assessed their coefficients as well as the resulting model fit relative to the model reported above. This analysis confirmed that meditation does not directly affect anxiety (M = 0.042, SE = 0.58 95%CI: −0.075 to 0.158, p = 0.465) or depression (M = −0.032, SE = 0.064, 95%CI: −0.160 to 0.092, p = 0.605). The introduction of these coefficients increased the AIC index by a negligible amount (3.077) and produced no significant difference in model fit [χ2(2) = 0.9, p = 0.638].

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Point estimates (M), standard error (SE), lower and upper bounds of the 95% confidence interval (Lower, Upper) and p-value (p) of the standardized coefficients of the adjusted models depicting the direct and indirect relationship between mindfulness and depression/anxiety, as well as the relationship between meditation practice and mindfulness.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Contribution of the direct and indirect paths between mindfulness and depression/anxiety.

Discussion

In the present study, we sought to explore the mechanisms underpinning the relationship between mindfulness and symptoms of depression and anxiety in a general population. More specifically, we sought to determine the extent to which four mechanisms of emotional regulation (cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression, rumination and worry) mediate this relationship, factoring in the experience of participants with mindfulness meditation. All four emotional regulation mechanisms mediated the relationship between mindfulness and depression, while all but expressive suppression mediated that between mindfulness and anxiety. As expected, mindfulness decreased depression and anxiety by increasing reappraisal (negatively associated with depression and anxiety) and reducing worry, rumination and suppression (negatively associated with depression and, expect for suppression, with anxiety). Worry and rumination proved to be the most potent mediating factors, while suppression and reappraisal played a significant but relatively smaller role. These mediating factors did not entirely account for the relationship between mindfulness and depression or anxiety, however. MP significantly increased mindfulness but had not direct effect on depression or anxiety.

Our findings are consistent with past reports of a negative association between mindfulness on the one hand, and rumination (Kumar et al., 2008; Feldman et al., 2010; Raes and Williams, 2010; Burg and Michalak, 2011; Desrosiers et al., 2013), worry (Desrosiers et al., 2013; Lenze et al., 2014; Verplanken and Fisher, 2014), or suppression (Brockman et al., 2017; Prakash et al., 2017) on the other. Our data are also in line with previous findings of a positive association between mindfulness and reappraisal (Garland et al., 2011; Hölzel et al., 2011; Froeliger et al., 2012; Desrosiers et al., 2013). One original contribution of our study is the comparison of these factors within the same study, however. All four factors acted as independent mediators of the relationship between mindfulness and depression, and three (worry, rumination, and reappraisal) mediated the impact of mindfulness on anxiety. In that respect, our findings differ from those of Desrosiers et al. (2013). Desrosiers et al. examined the relationship between mindfulness and symptoms of depression and anxiety in a sample of 187 participants with mood and anxiety disorders. While they too used reappraisal, rumination and worry as mediating factors in their analysis, they did not include suppression. Instead, they used a measure of non-acceptance of emotional experience, which denotes a propensity to experience negative metareactions to, and avoid the experience of, negative emotions (Gratz and Roemer, 2004; Coffey et al., 2010). Non-acceptance therefore arguably differs from expressive suppression insofar as the latter is a lower-level strategy aimed at inhibiting the behavioral expression of emotional responses. In Desrosiers et al.’s study, worry was the only significant mediator of the relationship between mindfulness and anxiety, and rumination was the only factor mediating the effect of mindfulness on depression. Furthermore, the presence of these mediators eliminated the direct effect of mindfulness on anxiety or depression. In our study, these direct effects remained significant despite the combined effects of all mediating factors. It may be that these discrepancies reflect differences in sample characteristics. Indeed, Desrosiers et al.’s participants presented with mood or anxiety disorders. Our sample, over six times larger, consisted of participants without a history of psychological or psychiatric problems (though this assertion is based on self-reported data in our case, the fact that 333 participants from our initial, unfiltered, sample did report such problems does suggest that this measure was sensitive). Apart from differences with respect to sample characteristics, our study also differs from Desrosiers et al.’s with respect to the measures of depression and anxiety. While Desrosiers et al. used subscales of the Mood and Anxiety Symptoms Questionnaire (Watson et al., 1995), we used the subscales of the HADS (Zigmond and Snaith, 1983). Both scales have been shown to distinguish well between depression and anxiety (Bjelland et al., 2002; Buckby et al., 2007) but, in the absence of any direct comparison between the two, one cannot rule out that they may measure slightly different aspects of these two constructs and, therefore, any comparison between the two studies remains tentative.

In our study, MP correlated positively with mindfulness but did not affect depression or anxiety directly. This suggests that it may not be the actual mental processes exercised by participants during meditation per se that help reduce symptoms of depression and anxiety but, rather, the resulting acquisition of aptitudes or skills that affect the way in which participants handle life events and situations more generally. Emotional regulation mechanisms appear to be important variables in that respect. Our analysis indicates that worry and ruminations were the two most potent mediators of the relationship between mindfulness and symptoms of depression and anxiety. Hence, the data suggest that individuals with high dispositional mindfulness are more likely to efficiently regulate their emotions and better handle intrusive thoughts and emotions relating to past or expected events. The practice of mindfulness-based meditation might enhance these abilities. Such contention fits with the prominence, in mindfulness meditation, of paying attention to the present moment (as opposed to directing one’s attention to past or future events). One possibility is that individuals with good meta-cognitive abilities may be more likely to engage in decentering from internal experiences and maintain a certain perspective when faced with events capable of triggering negative emotions. Such abilities may be facilitated and developed though mindfulness-based meditation, possibly through their progressive automatization. Decentering and meta-cognition have recently been argued to be substantial pillars of mindfulness (Shapiro et al., 2006; Jankowski and Holas, 2014; Norman, 2017) and are implicitly referred to in Kabat-Zinn (2003) definition of mindfulness as a state of consciousness that results from being aware of continuous changes in the content of consciousness.

Overall, our results are compatible with the hypothesis that mindfulness, dispositional or enhanced through meditation, can improve well-being by reducing symptoms of depression and anxiety, and that healthy emotional regulation is an important mediator of this effect. Consequently, with respect to strategies for reducing symptoms of depression and anxiety in the general population and help reduce the risk of sub-clinical symptoms converting to clinical disorders, mindfulness-based interventions or meditation exercises focusing specifically on the reduction of worry and rumination may prove especially efficient and beneficial as potential protectors against depression and anxiety in the general population. These conclusions are tentative and should be the focus of future work using complementary methods. While the use of an online survey allowed us to reach a large and more diverse sample of participants than would have been possible in a traditional laboratory-based study, caution must of course be exerted when interpreting the findings since online studies, like laboratory studies, rely on volunteers who may not be entirely representative of the general population (see Birnbaum, 2004; Reips, 2002, for a discussion of the advantages and limitations of internet-based research). While we sought to diversify our sample by portraying our study in two different ways (as a study on mindfulness and attention, or as a study on attention and distraction) and by disseminating it through multiple channels, our study should be interpreted in the context of a larger body of work combining different methodologies and sample types. Finally, it is worth noting that structural equation models are inherently based on correlational data and that causal relationships are therefore derived from a theoretical hypothesis rather than from the data per se. We think that the contention we put to the test, namely that mindfulness contributes to a reduction of depression and anxiety and that this effect occurs partly through emotional regulation mechanisms (an hypothesis shared by other researchers, e.g., Desrosiers et al., 2013; Freudenthaler et al., 2017), is a credible one in view of the evidence accumulated so far in the field.

Data Availability

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

FP, MG-T, JG-C, PA, and MG contributed to initial research proposal and the acquisition of research funding for this study. FP led the project, programmed the implementation of the online questionnaires, administered the data, and wrote the draft manuscript. MG-T, JG-C, PA, AY, and MG contributed to editing. MG-T and MG carried out the early exploratory analysis of the data. FP and AY finalized the analysis in the form reported in this manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by a Bursary for Scientific Research (Ref. 178/14) from the BIAL Foundation. FP’s contract at the University of the Balearic Islands is co-financed by the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities through their program for the incentivization and permanent incorporation of doctors (2016’s call, Ref IEDI-2016-00742). FP is also an Adjunct Senior Lecturer at the University of Western Australia. PA was supported by the Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities (REF PSI2016-75484-R), the Spanish State Agency for Research (AEI), and the European Regional Development Fund (FEDER). The project has received funding from DGA group (B17-17R) and the Network for Prevention and Health Promotion in primary Care (RD16/0007/0005) grant from the Instituto de Salud Carlos III of the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness, co-financed with European Union ERDF funds.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Christian Peake for his help with the early exploration of the data.

References

Abercrombie, H. C., Schaefer, S. M., Larson, C. L., Oakes, T. R., Lindgren, K. A., Holden, J. E., et al. (1998). Metabolic rate in the right amygdala predicts negative affect in depressed patients. Neuroreport 9, 3301–3307. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199810050-00028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans. Automat. Contr. 19, 716–723. doi: 10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alleva, J., Roelofs, J., Voncken, M., Meevissen, Y., and Alberts, H. (2014). On the relation between mindfulness and depressive symptoms: rumination as a possible mediator. Mindfulness 5, 72–79. doi: 10.1007/s12671-012-0153-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arbuckle, J. L. (2014). Amos (Version 23.0). Chicago: IBM SPSS.

Google Scholar

Baer, R. A. (2006). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: a conceptual and empirical review. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 10, 125–143. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpg015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., and Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment 13, 27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckwé, M., Deroost, N., Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., and De Raedt, R. (2014). Worrying and rumination are both associated with reduced cognitive control. Psychol. Res. 78, 651–660. doi: 10.1007/s00426-013-0517-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berle, D., Starcevic, V., Moses, K., Hannan, A., Milicevic, D., and Sammut, P. (2011). Preliminary validation of an ultra-brief version of the penn state worry questionnaire. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 18, 339–346. doi: 10.1002/cpp.724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birnbaum, M. H. (2004). Human research and data collection via the internet. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 55, 803–832. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141601

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfullness: a proposed operational definition. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 11, 230–241. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bph077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjelland, I., Dahl, A. A., Haug, T. T., and Neckelmann, D. (2002). The validity of the hospital anxiety and depression scale: an updated literature review. J. Psychosom. Res. 52, 69–77. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00296-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohlmeijer, E., Ten Klooster, P. M., Fledderus, M., Veehof, M., and Baer, R. A. (2011). Psychometric properties of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire in depressed adults and development of a short form. Assessment 18, 308–320. doi: 10.1177/1073191111408231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borkovec, T. D., Robinson, E., Pruzinsky, T., and DePree, J. A. (1983). Preliminary exploration of worry: some characteristics and processes. Behav. Res. Ther. 21, 9–16. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(83)90121-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bränström, R., Duncan, L. G., and Moskowitz, J. T. (2011). The association between dispositional mindfulness, psychological well-being, and perceived health in a swedish population-based sample. Br. J. Health Psychol. 16, 300–316. doi: 10.1348/135910710X501683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brockman, R., Ciarrochi, J., Parker, P., and Kashdan, T. (2017). Emotion regulation strategies in daily life: mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 46, 91–113. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2016.1218926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckby, J. A., Yung, A. R., Cosgrave, E. M., and Killackey, E. J. (2007). Clinical utility of the mood and anxiety symptom questionnaire (MASQ) in a sample of young help-seekers. BMC Psychiatry 7:50. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-7-50

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burg, J. M., and Michalak, J. (2011). The healthy quality of mindful breathing: Associations with rumination and depression. Cogn. Ther. Res. 35, 179–185. doi: 10.1007/s10608-010-9343-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burwell, R. A., and Shirk, S. R. (2007). Subtypes of rumination in adolescence: associations between brooding, reflection, depressive symptoms, and coping. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 36, 56–65. doi: 10.1080/15374410709336568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D. H., Brown, T. A., and Hofmann, S. G. (2006). Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 1251–1263. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmody, J., and Baer, R. A. (2008). Relationships between mindfulness practice and levels of mindfulness, medical and psychological symptoms and well-being in a mindfulness-based stress reduction program. J. Behav. Med. 31, 23–33. doi: 10.1007/s10865-007-9130-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cebolla, A., García-Palacios, A., Soler, J., Guillen, V., Baños, R., and Botella, C. (2012). Psychometric properties of the spanish validation of the five facets of mindfulness questionnaire (FFMQ). Eur. J. Psychiatry 26, 118–126. doi: 10.4321/S0213-61632012000200005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chu, C.-S., Stubbs, B., Chen, T.-Y., Tang, C.-H., Li, D.-J., Yang, W.-C., et al. (2018). The effectiveness of adjunct mindfulness-based intervention in treatment of bipolar disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 225, 234–245. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coffey, K. A., Hartman, M., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). Deconstructing mindfulness and constructing mental health: understanding mindfulness and its mechanisms of action. Mindfulness 1, 235–253. doi: 10.1007/s12671-010-0033-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crane, R. S., Brewer, J., Feldman, C., Kabat-Zinn, J., Santorelli, S., Williams, J. M. G., et al. (2017). What defines mindfulness-based programs? The warp and the weft. Psychol. Med. 47, 990–999. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716003317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cutuli, D. (2014). Cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression strategies role in the emotion regulation: an overview on their modulatory effects and neural correlates. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:175. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De las Cuevas Castresana, C., Garcia-Estrada Perez, A., and Gonzalez de Rivera, J. L. (1995). Hospital anxiety and depression scale” y psicopatología afectiva. An. Psiquiatr. 11, 126–130.

Google Scholar

Demarzo, M., Montero-Marin, J., Puebla-Guedea, M., Navarro-Gil, M., Herrera-Mercadal, P., Moreno-González, S., et al. (2017). Efficacy of 8- and 4-session mindfulness-based interventions in a non-clinical population: a controlled study. Front. Psychol. 8:1343. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desrosiers, A., Vine, V., Klemanski, D. H., and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2013). Mindfulness and emotion regulation in depression and anxiety: common and distinmct mechanisms of action. Depress. Anxiety 30, 654–661. doi: 10.1002/da.22124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Etkin, A., Klemenhagen, K. C., Dudman, J. T., Rogan, M. T., Hen, R., Kandel, E. R., et al. (2004). Individual differences in trait anxiety predict the response of the basolateral amygdala to unconsciously processed fearful faces. Neuron 44, 1043–1055. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, G., Greeson, J., and Senville, J. (2010). Differential effects of mindful breathing, progressive muscle relaxation, and loving-kindness meditation on decentering and negative reactions to repetitive thoughts. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 1002–1011. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freudenthaler, L., Turba, J. D., and Tran, U. S. (2017). Emotion regulation mediates the associations of mindfulness on symptoms of depression and anxiety in the general population. Mindfulness 8, 1339–1344. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0709-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Froeliger, B. E., Garland, E. L., Modlin, L. A., and McClernon, F. J. (2012). Neurocognitive correlates of the effects of yoga meditation practice on emotion and cognition: a pilot study. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:48. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galante, J., Iribarren, S. J., and Pearce, P. F. (2013). Effects of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy on mental disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. J. Res. Nurs. 18, 133–155. doi: 10.1177/1744987112466087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garland, E. L., Gaylord, S. A., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2011). Positive reappraisal mediates the stress-reductive effects of mindfulness: an upward spiral process. Mindfulness 2, 59–67. doi: 10.1007/s12671-011-0043-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garnefski, N., and Kraaij, V. (2006). Relationships between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms: a comparative study of five specific samples. Pers. Individ. Dif. 40, 1659–1669. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.12.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., and Gross, J. J. (2008). The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 577–586. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goleman, D. J., and Schwartz, G. E. (1976). Meditation as an intervention in stress reactivity. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 44, 456–466. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.44.3.456

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gratz, K. L., and Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 26, 41–54. doi: 10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 224–237. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.1.224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology 39, 281–291. doi: 10.1017/S0048577201393198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J., and John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for aff ect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., and Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits. J. Psychosom. Res. 57, 35–43. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00573-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallion, L. S., Ruscio, A. M., and Jha, A. P. (2014). Fractionating the role of executive control in control over worry: a preliminary investigation. Behav. Res. Ther. 54, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., and Oh, D. (2010). The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and depression: a meta-analytic review. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 78, 169–183. doi: 10.1037/a0018555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hölzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., Vago, D. R., and Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness meditation work? proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 537–559. doi: 10.1177/1745691611419671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoyer, J., Becker, E. S., and Margraf, J. (2002). Generalized anxiety disorder and clinical worry episodes in young women. Psychol. Med. 32, 1227–1237. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702006360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoyer, J., Gloster, A. T., and Herzberg, P. Y. (2009). Is worry different from rumination? Yes, it is more predictive of psychopathology! Psychosoc. Med. 6:Doc06. doi: 10.3205/psm000062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jankowski, T., and Holas, P. (2014). Metacognitive model of mindfulness. Conscious. Cogn. 28, 64–80. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jha, A. P., Stanley, E. A., Kiyonaga, A., Wong, L., and Gelfand, L. (2010). Examining the protective effects of mindfulness training on working memory capacity and affective experience. Emotion 10, 54–64. doi: 10.1037/a0018438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joormann, J., Dkane, M., and Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumination? diagnostic specificity and relation to depressive biases. Behav. Ther. 37, 269–280. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 10, 144–156. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpg016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kabat-Zinn, J., Massion, A. O., Kristeller, J., Peterson, L. G., Fletcher, K. E., Pbert, L., et al. (1992). Effectiveness of a meditation-based stress reduction program in the treatment of anxiety disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 149, 936–943. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.7.936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, S., Feldman, G., and Hayes, A. (2008). Changes in mindfulness and emotion regulation in an exposure-based cognitive therapy for depression. Cognit. Ther. Res. 32, 734–744. doi: 10.1007/s10608-008-9190-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuyken, W., Hayes, R., Barrett, B., Byng, R., Dalgleish, T., Kessler, D., et al. (2015). Effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy compared with maintenance antidepressant treatment in the prevention of depressive relapse or recurrence (PREVENT): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet 386, 63–73. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)62222-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacruz, M. E., Schmidt-Pokrzywniak, A., Dragano, N., Moebus, S., Deutrich, S. E., Möhlenkamp, S., et al. (2016). Depressive symptoms, life satisfaction and prevalence of sleep disturbances in the general population of germany: results from the heinz nixdorf recall study. BMJ Open 6:e007919. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-007919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenze, E. J., Hickman, S., Hershey, T., Wendleton, L., Ly, K., Dixon, D., et al. (2014). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for older adults with worry symptoms and co-occurring cognitive dysfunction. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 29, 991–1000. doi: 10.1002/gps.4086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marlatt, G. A., and Kristeller, J. L. (1999). “Mindfulness and meditation,” in Integrating Spirituality Into Treatment: Resources for Practitioners, ed. W. R. Miller (Washington: American Psychological Association), 67–84. doi: 10.1037/10327-004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, R. C., and Dahlen, E. R. (2005). Cognitive emotion regulation in the prediction of depression, anxiety, stress, and anger. Pers. Individ. Dif. 39, 1249–1260. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, J. T., McIntyre, E. M., English, J. S., Dennis, M. F., Beckham, J. C., and Kollins, S. H. (2017). A pilot trial of mindfulness meditation training for adhd in adulthood: impact on core symptoms, executive functioning, and emotion dysregulation. J. Atten. Disord. 21, 1105–1120. doi: 10.1177/1087054713513328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nevitt, J., and Hancock, G. R. (2001). Performance of bootstrapping approaches to model test statistics and parameter standard error estimation in structural equation modeling. Struct. Equ. Model. 8, 353–377. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0803-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 3, 400–424. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman, E. (2017). Metacognition and mindfulness: the role of fringe consciousness. Mindfulness 8, 95–100. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0494-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olatunji, B. O., Moretz, M. W., and Zlomke, K. R. (2010). Linking cognitive avoidance and GAD symptoms: the mediating role of fear of emotion. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 435–441. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ormel, J., VonKorff, M., Ustun, T. B., Pini, S., Korten, A., and Oldehinkel, T. (1994). Common mental disorders and disability across cultures. Results from the WHO Collaborative Study on Psychological Problems in General Health Care. JAMA 272, 1741–1748. doi: 10.1001/jama.1994.03520220035028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peh, C. X., Liu, J., Bishop, G. D., Chan, H. Y., Chua, S. M., Kua, E. H., et al. (2017). Emotion regulation and emotional distress: the mediating role of hope on reappraisal and anxiety/depression in newly diagnosed cancer patients. Psychooncology 26, 1191–1197. doi: 10.1002/pon.4297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prakash, R. S., Whitmoyer, P., Aldao, A., and Schirda, B. (2017). Mindfulness and emotion regulation in older and young adults. Aging Ment. Heal. 21, 77–87. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1100158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radkovsky, A., McArdle, J. J., Bockting, C. L. H., and Berking, M. (2014). Successful emotion regulation skills application predicts subsequent reduction of symptom severity during treatment of major depressive disorder. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 82, 248–262. doi: 10.1037/a0035828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raes, F., Hermans, D., Williams, J. M. G., Bijttebier, P., and Eelen, P. (2008). A “triple W”-model of rumination on sadness: why am I feeling sad, what’s the meaning of my sadness, and wish I could stop thinking about my sadness (but I can’t!). Cogn. Ther. Res. 32, 526–541. doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9137-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raes, F., and Williams, J. M. G. (2010). The relationship between mindfulness and uncontrollability of ruminative thinking. Mindfulness 1, 199–203. doi: 10.1007/s12671-010-0021-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rau, H. K., and Williams, P. G. (2016). Dispositional mindfulness: a critical review of construct validation research. Pers. Individ. Dif. 93, 32–43. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reips, U. D. (2002). Standards for internet-based experimenting. Exp. Psychol. 49, 243–256. doi: 10.1026//1618-3169.49.4.243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruscio, A. M., Lane, M., Roy-Byrne, P., Stang, P. E., Stein, D. J., Wittchen, H.-U., et al. (2005). Should excessive worry be required for a diagnosis of generalized anxiety disorder? results from the us national comorbidity survey replication. Psychol. Med. 35:1761. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705005908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandin, B., Chorot, P., Valiente, R. M., and Lostao, L. (2009). Validación Española del cuestionario de preocupación PSWQ: estructura factorial y propriedades psicométricas. Rev. Psicopatol. Y Psicol. Clínica 14, 107–122. doi: 10.5944/rppc.vol.14.num.2.2009.4070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sephton, S. E., Salmon, P., Weissbecker, I., Ulmer, C., Floyd, A., Hoover, K., et al. (2007). Mindfulness meditation alleviates depressive symptoms in women with fibromyalgia: results of a randomized clinical trial. Arthri. Rheum. 57, 77–85. doi: 10.1002/art.22478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shahar, B., Britton, W. B., Sbarra, D. A., Figueredo, A. J., and Bootzin, R. R. (2010). Mechanisms of change in mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: preliminary evidence from a randomized controlled trial. Spec. Issue Stress Gener. 3, 402–418. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2010.3.4.402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, S. L., Brown, K. W., Thoresen, C., and Plante, T. G. (2011). The moderation of Mindfulness-based stress reduction effects by trait mindfulness: results from a randomized controlled trial. J. Clin. Psychol. 67, 267–277. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., and Freedman, B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. J. Clin. Psychol. 62, 373–386. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soler, J., Cebolla, A., Feliu-Soler, A., Demarzo, M. M. P., Pascual, J. C., Baños, R., et al. (2014). Relationship between meditative practice and self-reported mindfulness: The MINDSENS composite index. PLoS One 9:e86622. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0086622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Struijs, S. Y., Lamers, F., Vroling, M. S., Roelofs, K., Spinhoven, P., and Penninx, B. W. J. H. (2017). Approach and avoidance tendencies in depression and anxiety disorders. Psychiatry Res. 256, 475–481. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, L. B. G., and Martin, G. (2016). Mind full or mindful: a report on mindfulness and psychological health in healthy adolescents. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 21, 64–74. doi: 10.1080/02673843.2012.709174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Y. Y., Hölzel, B. K., and Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 213–225. doi: 10.1038/nrn3916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Y. Y., Hölzel, B. K., and Posner, M. I. (2016). Traits and states in mindfulness meditation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17:59. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2015.7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tran, U. S., Cebolla, A., Glück, T. M., Soler, J., Garcia-Campayo, J., and von Moy, T. (2014). The serenity of the meditating mind: a cross-cultural psychometric study on a two-factor higher order structure of mindfulness, its effects, and mechanisms related to mental health among experienced meditators. PLoS One 9:e110192. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verplanken, B., and Fisher, N. (2014). Habitual worrying and benefits of mindfulness. Mindfulness 5, 566–573. doi: 10.1007/s12671-013-0211-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, K., Lu, H., Cheung, E. F. C., Neumann, D. L., Shum, D. H. K., and Chan, R. C. K. (2016). “Female preponderance” of depression in non-clinical populations: a meta-analytic study. Front. Psychol. 7:1398. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Weber, K., Assenheimer, J. S., Clark, L. A., Strauss, M. E., and McCormick, R. A. (1995). Testing a tripartite model: I. Evaluating the convergent and discriminant validity of anxiety and depression symptom scales. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 104, 3–14. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.104.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheeler, M. S., Arnkoff, D. B., and Glass, C. R. (2016). What is being studied as mindfulness meditation? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17:59. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2015.6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zigmond, A. S., and Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 67, 361–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mindfulness, depression, anxiety, emotional regulation, rumination, meditation

Citation: Parmentier FBR, García-Toro M, García-Campayo J, Yañez AM, Andrés P and Gili M (2019) Mindfulness and Symptoms of Depression and Anxiety in the General Population: The Mediating Roles of Worry, Rumination, Reappraisal and Suppression. Front. Psychol. 10:506. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00506

Received: 21 November 2018; Accepted: 20 February 2019;
Published: 08 March 2019.

Edited by:

Francisco J. Ruiz, Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz, Colombia

Reviewed by:

Paula Odriozola-González, University of Valladolid, Spain
Juan C. Suárez-Falcón, National University of Distance Education (UNED), Spain

Copyright © 2019 Parmentier, García-Toro, García-Campayo, Yañez, Andrés and Gili. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fabrice B. R. Parmentier, fabrice.parmentier@uib.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.