Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 15 March 2019
Sec. Developmental Psychology

The Role of Callous-Unemotional Traits on Adolescent Positive and Negative Emotional Reactivity: A Longitudinal Community-Based Study

  • 1Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
  • 2Department of Obstetrics, Gynecology and Reproductive Sciences, Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, United States

Callous-unemotional (CU) traits are associated with lower emotional reactivity in adolescents. However, since previous studies have focused mainly on reactivity to negative stimuli, it is unclear whether reactivity to positive stimuli is also affected. Further, few studies have addressed the link between CU traits and emotional reactivity in longitudinal community samples, which is important for determining its generalizability and developmental course. In the current study, pupil dilation and self-ratings of arousal and valence were assessed in 100 adolescents (15–17 years) from a community sample, while viewing images with negative and positive valence from the International Affective Pictures System (IAPS). Behavioral traits (CU) were assessed concurrently, as well as at ages 12–15, and 8–9 (subsample, n = 68, low levels of prosocial behavior were used as a proxy for CU traits). The results demonstrate that CU traits assessed at ages 12–15 and 8–9 predicted less pupil dilation to both positive and negative images at ages 15–17. Further, CU traits at ages 12–15 and concurrently were associated with less negative valence ratings for negative images and concurrently to less positive valence ratings for positive images. The current findings demonstrate that CU traits are related to lower emotional reactivity to both negative and positive stimuli in adolescents from a community sample.

Introduction

Children with callous-unemotional (CU) traits are characterized by deficits in empathy and guilt, low distress, low fearfulness, and restricted emotional expression (Frick et al., 2014). CU traits constitute the affective-interpersonal dimension of psychopathy and have been applied downward in age to childhood to identify a developmental precursor to psychopathy (Frick and Ray, 2015). In children and adolescents with conduct problems, elevated CU traits have been concurrently and longitudinally linked to delinquency, aggressive behavior, lower levels of social skills and poorer treatment outcomes (Frick et al., 2014). Further, twin studies indicate that conduct problems in children with elevated CU traits have a considerably stronger genetic influence than in children with normative levels of CU traits (Frick et al., 2014). CU traits are considered to be detectable early in childhood, possibly as early as 2 years of age (Waller et al., 2012) and predict both earlier onset and greater persistence of conduct problems (Dandreaux and Frick, 2009; Rowe et al., 2010b). CU traits have also been shown to be relatively stable throughout childhood and adolescence (rs = 0.27–0.84, Frick et al., 2014), although the levels seem to increase in middle adolescence (Essau et al., 2006). Given the negative outcomes associated with elevated levels of CU traits, more knowledge is needed on potential underlying mechanisms/processes related to CU traits.

Callous-Unemotional Traits and Emotional Reactivity

Several theories on the etiology of psychopathy or CU traits have focused on deficits in reactivity to negatively valenced stimuli (Patrick, 1994; Lykken, 1995; Fowles and Dindo, 2009; Frick and Viding, 2009). That is, CU traits are proposed to be related to lower reactivity to unpleasant stimuli (in particular fear and distress), For instance, Frick and Viding (2009) have proposed that individuals high on CU traits and conduct problems have a specific temperament (lower sensitivity to punishment, fearlessness, lower responsiveness to distress in others) that increases the likelihood of antisocial behavior. These temperamental features hamper the motivation to avoid behaviors that generate harm or distress in others and would therefore explain the tendency for individuals high on CU traits to show little aversion to such behaviors. Such features would further interfere with the normative development of conscience (i.e., guilt and empathy; Frick and Viding, 2009), in line with theoretical accounts of conscience development, in which emotional arousal is viewed as fundamental (Kochanska, 1993; Thompson, 2014).

Yet, it is also possible that lower reactivity to positive stimuli could contribute to behaviors common in those with high CU traits. That is, not being reactive to positive stimuli and situations could impair the establishment of social relationships and a general sense of wellbeing in life that supports connection to social groups. A potential mechanism behind this lower reactivity could be differences in amygdala response. The amygdala is involved in the processing of both positive and negative emotions (Baxter and Murray, 2002) and it has been shown to be less functional in those with higher CU traits (Frick et al., 2014; Herpers et al., 2014). This explanation would also be in line with Blair’s Integrated Emotion Systems model that suggests that psychopathic/callous behaviors are primarily driven by poor functioning of the amygdala and related neural circuitry, which impairs the representations of positive and negative emotional information (Blair, 2004, 2006). More specifically, this results in deficits in approach and avoidance motivation, which then hinders the normal socialization process, driving the development of CU traits.

While much research has examined the link between CU traits and negative emotional reactivity, less has been done to test whether there are also deficits in positive emotional reactivity. That is, research has demonstrated that children and adolescents with elevated levels of CU traits display lower negative emotional reactivity (Frick et al., 2014; Herpers et al., 2014), even on the physiological level (Blair, 1999; Fung et al., 2005; Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous and Warden, 2008; Marsh et al., 2008; Isen et al., 2010; de Wied et al., 2012; White et al., 2012; Lozier et al., 2014). In studies in which emotional reactivity has been measured with the dot-probe task – a reaction time task that measures how emotional stimuli affects attentional orientation – stimuli of both positive and negative valence have been included and the findings have been that CU traits are specifically related to lower attention/lower reaction time to negatively valanced stimuli (Patrick, 1994; Lykken, 1995; Fowles and Dindo, 2009; Frick and Viding, 2009). However, the dot-probe task is not a direct measure of emotional reactivity; several motoric, affective, and cognitive process are involved in the participants’ reaction times (Vasey et al., 1996). Therefore, associations with CU traits may be different when using a direct measure of emotional reactivity.

There are, to our knowledge, only two studies on physiological emotional reactivity and CU traits which have included stimuli of positive valence (de Wied et al., 2012; Fanti et al., 2016), and the results have been inconsistent. Specifically, Fanti et al. (2016) found that CU traits were associated with lower reactivity to positive stimuli, while de Wied et al. (2012) found no relation. Research on psychopathic traits has also found lower emotional reactivity to positive stimuli in 12- to 18-year-old adolescents (Syngelaki et al., 2013), and in adults (Herpertz et al., 2001). Thus, it is still unclear whether CU traits are associated with lower physiological reactivity to stimuli of positive in addition to negative valence. Evidence from beyond the field of emotional reactivity suggests that this might be the case. For instance, in a meta-analysis, CU traits were related to deficits in emotion recognition across valence (Dawel et al., 2012). In addition, CU traits have been found to be associated with difficulty in perceiving positive social interactions (Fawcett et al., 2016b) as well as with lower responsiveness to parental affection (Dadds et al., 2014) and to reward (Marini and Stickle, 2010).

Self-ratings of emotional reactivity (i.e., arousal and valence) seem to also be affected by CU traits in adolescents. Specifically, in relation to negatively valenced stimuli, the presence of CU traits is related to less negative ratings of valence (Marini and Stickle, 2010; Fanti et al., 2016) and lower ratings of arousal (Sharp et al., 2006; Michonski and Sharp, 2010). For positively valenced visual stimuli, the findings for CU traits have been inconsistent. Masi et al. (2014) found no association, Sharp et al. (2006) found that CU traits were related to increased arousal, and Fanti et al. (2016) found that CU traits were related to less positive ratings of valence. Interestingly, adults with elevated CU traits (measured via the affective dimension of psychopathic traits) respond to emotional stimuli with similar self-ratings of arousal as controls, despite displaying lower physiological reactivity (Patrick et al., 1993; Herpertz et al., 2001). A potential reason for this difference may be that the adults’ responses are more influenced by social desirability than those of adolescents.

Remaining Questions

The research described in the previous section demonstrates that CU traits are concurrently related to lower emotional reactivity, at least to negative stimuli, in adolescents with conduct problems. Beyond the important question of whether CU traits are associated specifically with lower reactivity to stimuli of negative valence or if reactivity to positive stimuli is also affected, there are several additional areas concerning the relation between CU traits and emotional reactivity that have not yet been thoroughly investigated.

First, to our knowledge, no study has examined whether CU traits in childhood or early adolescence can predict emotional reactivity in late adolescence. From a developmental perspective it is important to acknowledge that age may influence predictive and concurrent associations to behavioral problems as well as potential biological correlates. Therefore, by using longitudinal designs we may gain further knowledge about stability and the predictive value of CU traits in relation to emotional reactivity.

Second, few studies have investigated CU traits dimensionally in relation to emotional reactivity in a community sample. Despite the fact that CU traits vary continuously in community samples (Essau et al., 2006), previous research on CU traits and emotional reactivity has primarily focused on individuals who display extreme scores (e.g., the top 10% of CU traits or forensic/clinical samples). This focus limits the generalizability of the potential effects of CU traits in non-clinical samples, in particular in children and adolescents without elevated levels of Disruptive Behavioral Problems (DBP; see review Herpers et al., 2012). Two notable exceptions have shown that CU traits predict negative behavioral outcomes independently of conduct problems and other psychiatric symptoms in community samples (Dadds et al., 2005; Moran et al., 2008).

The Current Study

Our aim was to explore whether CU traits are uniquely related to lower emotional reactivity for both positive and negative stimuli, predictively and concurrently in a community sample of adolescents. Emotional reactivity was measured physiologically with pupil dilation as well as with self-ratings of arousal and valence. Pupillometry is an effective way to measure emotional reactivity because pupils dilate with increased allocation of attention or arousal, for example due to emotional intensity or cognitive load (Laeng et al., 2012; Graur and Siegle, 2013). Further, pupillary changes in response to emotional stimuli are considered to be regulated by sympathetic nervous system activity (Bradley et al., 2008) and related to increased amygdala activity (Siegle et al., 2003). The measures have been used in a large range of studies targeting developmental populations (Falck-Ytter, 2008; Chatham et al., 2009; Gredebäck and Melinder, 2010, 2011; Geangu et al., 2011; Gredebäck et al., 2012; Sirois and Jackson, 2012; Hepach and Westermann, 2013; Nyström et al., 2015b; Fawcett et al., 2016a; Hellmer et al., 2016).

We compared adolescents’ emotional reactivity when viewing positive and negative stimuli to their levels of CU traits, both at the current time point and earlier in childhood. In order to ensure that the results were not influenced by co-occurring behavioral symptoms, our analyses for both pupil reactivity and self-ratings of arousal and valence controlled for two symptoms commonly co-occurring with CU traits: DBP and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). We hypothesized that CU traits would be uniquely related to lower physiological emotional reactivity (measured by pupil dilation) for images of both negative and positive valence, based on findings from research with clinical or extreme groups. In contrast, we had no a priori hypotheses for the relation between CU traits and self-ratings of arousal and valence, given the inconclusive findings in previous child and adult samples.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were initially recruited from two longitudinal community samples (N = 867), in which children were drawn either from randomly selected child healthcare centers (Sample A, n = 650) or daycare centers (Sample B, n = 217) in Sweden at ages 4–6 (Wåhlstedt, 2009; Wåhlstedt and Bohlin, 2010). Given that the high education level and socio-economic status of the participants’ parents in these two original samples (Wåhlstedt, 2009; Wåhlstedt and Bohlin, 2010) would typically lead to very low levels of behavioral problems (including CU traits), subsamples of children with high levels of ADHD symptoms were over-selected from these samples to increase dimensional variability for separate follow up assessments at ages 7–9 (Sample A, n = 233; Sample B, n = 111). The comorbidity between ADHD and later behavioral problems, which has robust support in the literature, was the justification for this decision (Bélanger et al., 2018). That is, the comorbidity between ADHD and DBP is suggested to be as high as 90% (Dunn and Kronenberger, 2003; Rommelse et al., 2009), and approximately 10–40% of youth with DBP show significant CU traits (Rowe et al., 2010a; Fanti, 2013). At this time point (T1), only parent and teacher ratings of children in Sample A were used in the current study. The reason for this was because ratings of the behavioral problems of interest (low levels of prosocial behaviors, hypothesized proxy to tap CU traits; see measure below) were not assessed in Sample B. Five years later, participants from Sample A and Sample B (n = 344) were contacted to take part in a shared follow-up (T2), which resulted in a sample of 317 children ages 12–15.

A subsample of the participants from the T2 follow-up (N = 159) was subsequently contacted to take part in an additional follow-up (T3). The selection of the subsample was based on aggregated parents and teacher ratings of CU traits from T2 to ensure sufficient variability in CU traits. Specifically, high levels of CU traits (see measure below) were defined as scoring in the highest 30% (n = 79) and low levels were defined as scoring in the lowest 50% (n = 80). Of the 159 parents contacted under these criteria, 70% (n = 112); gave permission for their child to participate and complete data was collected for 100 participants (high levels of CU traits n = 50 and low levels of CU traits n = 50). Reasons for attrition were that the child did not wish to participate (n = 4), the child did not show up for assessment (n = 2), or we were unable to schedule a time for the child to participate before summer break (n = 6).

At T2 and T3, adolescents’ levels of CU traits (Fanti et al., 2009; Docherty et al., 2016) and ADHD symptoms (DuPaul et al., 1998) were comparable to other large community samples (see Table 1 for descriptive data). This indicates that the oversampling technique used was successful in recruiting a community sample representative of the wider population, despite the fact that the local population from which it was drawn tends to be particularly high on parental education and income level.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive data on behavioral ratings.

The final sample of 100 children (45 girls) were 8–9 years at T1 (Sample A; n = 68, 30 girls, M = 8.5 years, SD = 2.0 months), 12–15 years at T2 (M = 13.0 years, SD = 6.2 months) and 15–17 years at T3 (M = 15.8 years, SD = 6.5 months). At each time point, parents received detailed written information about the study and returned a signed consent form if they wanted their child to participate. Participants themselves were asked for their assent on the day of testing and were reminded that participation was voluntary and they were free to ask any questions or stop participating at any time without giving a reason. Participants and their parents each received two movie vouchers (worth approximately 20 euros) and teachers received one movie voucher (worth approximately 10 euros) for each assessment in which they participated.

Materials

Questionnaires

Measures regarding CU traits, DBP (i.e., symptoms of ODD and conduct problems) and symptoms of ADHD were collected from parents and teachers at T1 and T2 and from parents at T3. Teacher ratings were not collected at T3 since the adolescents no longer had one consistent teacher who knew them well enough to complete the measures. Internal consistency as measured by Cronbach’s alpha ranged from adequate to high for the specific scales (0.71 to 0.96; see Table 1), except for conduct problems at T1, which was therefore not included in the analysis. Parent and teacher ratings were correlated at T1 (CU traits/low levels of prosocial behavior was used as a proxy, see below, T1, r = 0.31, p = 0.014; ODD T1, r = 0.32, p = 0.012; ADHD T1, r = 0.28, p = 0.030) and T2 (CU traits T2, r = 0.59, p < 0.001; DBP T2, r = 0.23, p = 0.035; ADHD T2, r = 0.41, p < 0.001), thus aggregated scores were used at these time points.

CU traits

To measure CU traits, the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (ICU; Frick, 2004) was used at T2 and T3. The ICU includes 24 items on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all true) to 3 (definitely true), with higher scores indicating greater CU traits. Previous research has verified the validity of the ICU in community samples of children and adolescents (Roose et al., 2010; Fanti and Centifanti, 2014). At T1 a reverse of the subscale prosocial behavior (i.e., low levels of prosocial behavior) from the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, 1997) was used as a proxy for CU traits since the ICU was not completed. The subscale includes five items, and ratings were made on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (does not apply at all) to 5 (applies very well). The subscale has been found to be a reliable indicator of CU traits and to load on the same factor as CU measures in factor analyses (Dadds et al., 2005; Pasalich et al., 2011; Hernandez-Lallement et al., 2018).

Disruptive behavior problems

To form a broad measure of DBP, scores concerning ODD and conduct problems were standardized and aggregated at T2 and T3 (parents T2, r = 0.76; teachers T2, r = 0.87; parents T3, r = 0.80, ps < 0.001). ODD symptoms were assessed using a well-validated rating scale containing the eight items for ODD as presented in DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994). Each item was rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 (never or rarely) to 3 (very often). Conduct problems were measured with the conduct problems subscale from the SDQ (Goodman, 1997). The subscale includes five items and ratings were made on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (does not apply at all) to 5 (applies very well). At T1, only the measure for ODD was used, since the internal consistencies for conduct problems were insufficient for both the parent and teacher ratings (see Table 1). The lack of internal consistency at ages 8–9 for conduct problems is, however, not unexpected considering that the subscale has been found to have low internal consistency at this age (Stone et al., 2010).

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms were assessed using a rating scale containing 18 items for ADHD as presented in DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994). Each item was rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 (never or rarely) to 3 (very often). This measure has been validated and is frequently used in ADHD research (DuPaul et al., 1998).

Emotional Reactivity Stimuli

Self-reported ratings of valence and arousal as well as data on pupil dilation (see detailed information below) were collected while the participants viewed emotional stimuli. The stimuli consisted of 38 images from the International Affective Picture System (Lang et al., 2008), which were initially chosen to include 19 pleasant images (for example, a butterfly or children playing) and 19 unpleasant images (for example, weapons, violent behavior; see Supplementary Table S1 for details). The IAPS has been shown to be valid for adolescent samples and provide adequate cross-cultural consistency (Lang and Bradley, 2007).

Procedure

The IAPS task was presented as one part of a larger battery of 11 tasks measuring various cognitive and social skills. Participants were tested individually in a quiet room at their school. The entire procedure took approximately 1 h and 20 min and was approved by the local ethical board. Gaze was recorded using a Tobii T120 eye tracker with a sampling rate of 120 Hz. Participants sat approximately 50 cm away from the eye tracker. A 9-point calibration was completed before participants viewed the image sequence. Each image filled the screen of the eye tracker and was displayed for 5,000 ms and was followed by the questions regarding valence and arousal presented on the screen. Specifically, participants provided self-ratings of valence (i.e., “how happy or sad does the photo make you”) and arousal (i.e., “how excited or calm does the photo make you”) on a single item Likert scale, ranging from 1 to 9 by selecting a number key (1–9) on a keyboard that was placed approximately 30 cm in front of the participant. For valence, low scores indicated an unpleasant emotional reaction while high scores a pleasant reaction. Concerning arousal, low scores indicated low levels of arousal, and high scores high levels of arousal. The next image was presented directly after the participant had rated both valence and arousal. The 38 images from the IAPS were shown inter-mixed with 12 additional images of angry, fearful, and neutral male and female faces which are not included in the current study. The 50 images were displayed to each participant in one of four possible semi-randomized orders. At T1 and T2, parent and teacher questionnaires were completed by regular post, and at T3 parent questionnaires were completed online.

Data Management and Reduction

Prior to analysis, questionnaire data were screened for outliers, defined as values ±3 SD and replaced with the value that was the next most extreme, in line with the Winsorizing procedure (Chen et al., 2001). Eye tracking data was processed in the MATLAB-based open source program TimeStudio version 3.151 (Nyström et al., 2015a; the processing procedure used in the current study can be downloaded into TimeStudio using uwid: ts-fd3-dd9). First, gaps in the data of up to 10 samples (83 ms) were interpolated linearly and the data was smoothed using a moving median over six samples (50 ms) and then a moving average over six samples (50 ms). Then the values for each trial were adjusted using a baseline of the first 500 ms of the image display, which was subtracted from the average pupil size from the analysis period of 1 to 3 s. The baseline helps to account for differences in light across individual images. Trials with less than 50% of pupil size data recorded were excluded.

Statistical Analyses

All relations between predictors and outcome were analyzed per trial using linear mixed-effects models in R (version 3.4.3, R; R Development Core Team, 2014) with the package lme4 (version 1.1-15; Bates et al., 2014). All models included a random effect of participant. Random effects are beneficial for taking into account the individual variability of participants or trials to strengthen analyses (Baayen et al., 2008). In the main analyses, behavioral ratings from parents and teachers at T1 and T2, and from parents at T3 were used. Data are available from the authors upon reasonable request.

Results

Descriptive statistics for behavioral symptoms, pupil data and control variables are presented in Table 1. Correlations for behavioral symptoms ratings used in the analyses are shown in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Inter-correlations between aggregated behavioral ratings.

Preliminary Analyses

We first compared the valence and arousal ratings of the participants in the current study with norm ratings for adults from the IAPS manual (Lang et al., 2008). There was significant agreement between IAPS norms and the average ratings from our participants for both arousal [ICC(37) = 0.74, p < 0.001] and valence [ICC(37) = 0.94, p < 0.001], as measured using Intraclass Correlations. Before proceeding to the main analysis, the images from the IAPS were spilt into two groups based on visual inspection of the histogram of self-ratings of valence which clearly shows a bimodal distribution (low, n = 20, high, n = 18; see Figure 1). We also examined the internal consistency by calculating Cronbach’s coefficient alpha for the ratings of the two groups of images (low and high valence). The coefficient alphas for low valence images were 0.89 for arousal and 0.92 for valence; for high valence images, they were 0.74 for arousal and 0.75 for valence (ps < 0.001).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Histogram showing the distribution of mean self-ratings of valence for the 38 images from the IAPS used in the present study. Visual inspection led to dividing the images into high and low valence groups with a split at the rating 5.

The IAPS images were assessed for average image luminance as well as contrast at high- and low-spatial frequency (HSF and LSF, respectively) to examine whether they could be confounded with arousal and valence (Delplanque et al., 2007). Luminance, HSF contrast, and LSF contrast were all found to be related to ratings of both arousal (luminance: b = -0.011, SE = 0.001, 95% CI [-0.013, -0.010], t = -14.21, p < 0.001; HSF contrast: b = 3.855, SE = 0.379, 95% CI [3.113, 4.597], t = 10.18, p < 0.001; LSF contrast: b = 5.980, SE = 0.562, 95% CI [4.879, 7.081], t = 10.65, p < 0.001) and valence (luminance: b = 0.017, SE = 0.001, 95% CI [0.016, 0.019], t = 19.15, p < 0.001; HSF contrast: b = -7.028, SE = 0.455, 95% CI [-7.919, -6.137], t = -15.46, p < 0.001; LSF contrast: b = -7.996, SE = 0.685, 95% CI [-9.338, -6.653], t = -11.68, p < 0.001). Thus, all three were included as control variables in the main analyses.

Sex of participant was not related to pupil dilation in a model including sex as predictor and pupil dilation as outcome (b = -0.001, SE = 0.023, 95% CI [-0.047, 0.044], t = -0.052, p > 0.1), or to self-ratings of valence in a model including sex as predictor and self-ratings of valence as outcome (b = -0.060, SE = 0.085, 95% CI [-0.226, 0.105], t = -0.712, p > 0.1), therefore sex was not included in further analyses with pupil dilation or valence as outcome. There was, however, a marginally significant effect of sex of participant on self-ratings of arousal in a model including sex as predictor and self-ratings of arousal as outcome (b = 0.232, SE = 0.123, 95% CI [-0.009, 0.472], t = 1.887, p = 0.062) with females (M = 5.45, SD = 1.90) reporting higher levels of arousal than males (M = 5.22, SD = 1.89). Sex was therefore included as a control in the analyses with self-ratings of arousal as outcome.

To confirm that arousal as measured by self-ratings was related to arousal as measured by pupil dilation, a model in which self-rating of arousal was the predictor and pupil dilation was the outcome was tested. Self-ratings of arousal did significantly predict pupil dilation (b = 0.019, SE = 0.002, 95% CI [0.015, 0.023], t = 9.48, p < 0.001; see Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Smoothed scatterplot showing the relationship between participants’ self-ratings of arousal and their change in pupil size from baseline on each trial. The regression indicates a significant relation between the two measures.

Predictive and Concurrent Relations Between Behavioral Symptoms and Pupil Dilation to Positively and Negatively Valenced Images

Linear mixed models including the behavioral symptoms of CU traits, DBP, and ADHD as predictors, and pupil dilation as outcome were used to investigate emotional reactivity predictively (T2) and concurrently (T3). Predictively, CU traits (T2) were related to less pupil dilation relative to baseline for images of both negative (b = -0.005, SE = 0.002, 95% CI [-0.010, -0.001], t = -2.253, p = 0.027) and positive (b = -0.004, SE = 0.002, 95% CI [-0.008, -0.0003], t = -2.090, p = 0.040) valence (see Figure 3). Concurrently (T3), none of the behavioral symptoms were related to pupil dilation (p’s > 0.1).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Smoothed scatterplots showing the significant relationship between participants’ CU scores and their change in pupil size from baseline on each trial.

For the subsample (n = 68) for which low prosocial behaviors (i.e., reversing the prosocial behavior scale as a proxy for CU traits) were assessed at T1, a linear mixed model including low prosocial behavior, ODD, and ADHD as predictors, and pupil dilation as outcome was tested. Low prosocial behavior was related to less pupil dilation relative to baseline for images of positive valence (b = -0.012, SE = 0.005, 95% CI [-0.023, -0.002], t = -2.370, p = 0.021, see Figure 3), but not for images of negative valence (p > 0.1).

Predictive and Concurrent Relations Between Behavioral Symptoms and Self-Ratings of Arousal and Valence to Positively and Negatively Valenced Images

Arousal

Linear mixed models including CU traits, DBP, ADHD as predictors, and self-ratings of arousal as outcome were conducted predictively (T2) and concurrently (T3). For negatively valenced images, CU traits were predictively (b = -0.030, SE = 0.015, 95% CI [-0.059, 0.000], t = -1.928, p = 0.057) and concurrently (b = -0.024, SE = 0.014, 95% CI [-0.052, 0.004], t = -1.668, p = 0.099) related to lower ratings of arousal with marginal significance. For positively valenced images, there were no significant relations between self-ratings of arousal and CU traits, however, there were significant associations for several control variables. Predictively, ADHD symptoms were uniquely related to higher ratings of arousal (b = 0.052, SE = 0.018, 95% CI [0.019, 0.086], t = 2.988, p = 0.004), as was participant sex (b = 0.369, SE = 0.177, 95% CI [0.028, 0.710], t = 2.088, p = 0.040), with females rating higher arousal levels than males. Concurrently, DBP were significantly related to lower ratings of arousal (b = -0.327, SE = 0.154, 95% CI [-0.624, -0.029], t = -2.116, p = 0.037) and ADHD was marginally related to higher ratings of arousal (b = 0.035, SE = 0.018, 95% CI [0.001, 0.070], t = 1.972, p = 0.052). Finally, for the subsample for which low prosocial behaviors (i.e., proxy for CU traits) were assessed at T1, a linear mixed model including prosocial behavior, ODD, ADHD as predictors, and self-ratings of arousal as outcome was tested. No relations were found (ps > 0.1).

Valence

Linear mixed models including CU traits, DBP, ADHD as predictors, and self-ratings of valence as outcome were conducted predictively (T2) and concurrently (T3). For negatively valenced images, CU traits were predictively (b = 0.033, SE = 0.015, 95% CI [0.003, 0.063], t = 2.203, p = 0.030) and concurrently (b = 0.032, SE = 0.015, 95% CI [-0.003, 0.061], t = 2.195, p = 0.031) related to less negative ratings of valence. In addition, ADHD symptoms were predictively related to less negative ratings of valence with marginal significance (b = 0.033, SE = 0.017, 95% CI [-0.001, 0.068], t = 1.932, p = 0.056). For positively valenced images, CU traits were concurrently (b = -0.026, SE = 0.010, 95% CI [-0.046, -0.006], t = -2.585, p = 0.011) related to less positive ratings of valence. No relations were found predictively (p’s > 0.1).

In addition, for the subsample for which low prosocial behaviors (i.e., proxy for CU traits) were assessed at T1, linear mixed models including prosocial behavior, ODD, ADHD as predictors, and self-ratings of valence as outcome were tested. No relations were found (p’s > 0.1).

Discussion

The current study examined the relation between positive and negative emotional reactivity and CU traits in adolescents from a community sample, both predictively and concurrently, while controlling for DBP and ADHD symptoms. In line with expectations, we found that CU traits were predictively associated with lower emotional reactivity (less pupil dilation) when viewing both positive and negative images from the IAPS. Specifically, lower emotional reactivity to images of both positive and negative valence at ages 15–17 was associated with CU traits at ages 12–15. In addition, low levels of prosocial behavior (proxy for CU traits) at ages 8–9 were associated with lower emotional reactivity to images of positive valence at ages 15–17. Interestingly, no associations were found between CU traits and emotional reactivity concurrently.

Given the previously mixed findings on CU traits and self-ratings of arousal, we did not have a clear hypothesis regarding potential associations for this part of our study. The results showed that CU traits were marginally associated with lower ratings of arousal to negative images, predictively, at ages 12–15, and concurrently in line with previous findings (Sharp et al., 2006; Michonski and Sharp, 2010; de Wied et al., 2012). Further, ADHD symptoms at ages 12–15 and concurrently were related to higher ratings of arousal to positive images, which is consistent with a recent study finding an association between increased positive emotionality (rated by parents) and ADHD symptoms (Forslund et al., 2016). In addition, DBP were concurrently related to lower ratings of arousal to positive images. Together these findings suggest that self-ratings of arousal have some relation to CU traits, but that the physiological measure of emotional reactivity, pupil dilation, is a more direct way to assess arousal and is less likely to be influenced by other participant factors, making the relation between arousal and CU traits more clear.

The current findings on emotional reactivity fail to provide support for the prevalent view that high levels of CU (or psychopathic) traits are specifically related to lower reactivity to stimuli of negative valence (Frick et al., 2014; Herpers et al., 2014) and instead suggest that emotional reactivity to positive images is also affected by CU traits. Only two prior studies on emotional reactivity and CU traits have included stimuli with positive valence (de Wied et al., 2012; Fanti et al., 2016) and the results have been mixed. Specifically, Fanti et al. (2016) found that CU traits were associated with lower neurophysiological activity in response to both positive and negative stimuli: videos depicting both violent and comedy scenes. Whereas de Wied et al. (2012) did not find conclusive evidence for a relation between CU and emotional reactivity to happiness. However, the latter study had some limitations, which constrain the ability to draw clear conclusions. First, the statistical power was limited since the analyses were conducted between groups with low sample sizes. Second, unique variance in relation to CU traits may have been masked by not taking into account the shared variance between DBP and CU traits (i.e., not controlling for DBP when examining the relation between CU traits and emotional reactivity). Thus our results provide a more conclusive statement on the relation between CU traits and positive emotional reactivity, adding to findings that have found similar relations between positive emotional reactivity and psychopathic traits in adolescents, ages 12–18 (Syngelaki et al., 2013), and in adults (Herpertz et al., 2001). Further, the findings are in line with research concerning others areas of socio-emotional processing in which CU traits have been found to be associated with lower ability to process positive affect, such as difficulty in perceiving positive social interactions (Fawcett et al., 2016b) and lower responsiveness to parental affection (Dadds et al., 2014).

To further advance developmental theories on CU traits, it may be important to consider the potential role that deficits in both perceiving and experiencing positive affect may have. Thus, while previous findings have mainly demonstrated that deficits in emotional reactivity are specific to stimuli of negative valence (in particular fear and distress) our current findings suggest that CU traits may be related to lower reactivity across valence, consistent with the Integrated Emotion Systems model proposed by Blair (2004, 2006). Impaired representation of emotional information of both positive and negative affect might contribute to CU traits, for example in that decreased approach and avoidance motivation (seeking situations associated with happiness and avoid those associated with fear and distress) might hamper normal socialization and social interaction.

Contrary to our expectations, CU traits were not concurrently related to lower emotional reactivity measured by pupil dilation. This is inconsistent with prior research with physiological measures of emotional reactivity, which have found CU traits related to lower reactivity in adolescence (Blair, 1999; Fung et al., 2005; Marsh et al., 2008; Isen et al., 2010; de Wied et al., 2012; White et al., 2012; Lozier et al., 2014). However, these studies have included clinical or extreme groups, which may have increased the potential to find relations between CU traits and emotional reactivity. It is possible that the discrepant findings between predictive and concurrent ratings of CU traits and physiological emotional reactivity in the current study are influenced by developmental differences (pre/early-adolescence vs. middle adolescence). That is, the normative levels of CU traits in childhood have been suggested to vary depending on age (Edens et al., 2001; Seagrave and Grisso, 2002), which is consistent with a community sample study (n = 1,433), in which adolescents 15–16 years old had significantly higher CU scores than adolescents either 13–14 or 17–18 years old (Essau et al., 2006). It is therefore possible that measures of CU traits rated during middle adolescence may to some degree capture behaviors that are specific to the age group, rather than underlying CU traits. Even the emotional reactivity of children changes during development. For example, brain imaging studies have shown that emotional reactivity decreases with age, which means that aversive affective cues are instead interpreted in a more specialized and cognitive manner when children become older (Michalska and Davis, 2018). However, there may also be other factors that underlie the discrepancy. For instance, the validity of parent and teacher reports tend to decrease from childhood to adolescence (Kamphaus and Frick, 1996), suggesting that ratings are less reliable overall in adolescence. Further, it is possible that the mechanism which results in the decreased automatic emotional reactivity is one that develops over time, meaning that the more relevant levels of CU traits are actually those from earlier in development, not those from the current timepoint.

The current study has several strengths in relation to previous work in this area. One strength is that we examined the role of CU traits in relation to emotional reactivity in a community sample. Prior research examining CU traits has mainly concerned children and adolescents that have already been identified as having problematic behavior (e.g., Pardini et al., 2003; Stickle et al., 2009). Thus, by using community sample, we are able to show that CU traits are related to lower emotional reactivity even in a non-clinical population. However, the use of a community sample may in turn put constraints on the ability to generalize the findings to clinical populations.

A second strength is that we examined relations between emotional reactivity and CU traits developmentally over late childhood and into adolescence. This study is the first to our knowledge that explores predictive relations between CU traits and emotional reactivity. However, given that we only measured emotional reactivity at the latest time point, ages 15–17, we cannot, for instance, provide insight into concurrent relations between CU traits and emotional reactivity earlier in childhood or shed light on how emotional reactivity in childhood is associated to CU traits in adolescence. Further, we must take into account that the earliest time point used low levels of prosocial behavior for a proxy of CU traits and it might not fully tap into all the aspects of CU traits, in particular the unemotional dimension. Nevertheless, the subscale has been found to be a reliable indicator of CU traits (Dadds et al., 2005; Pasalich et al., 2011) and been used as a proxy of CU traits (Musser et al., 2013). These are all important topics to be addressed in future research.

Finally, our study controlled for common comorbid symptoms in children and adolescents with CU traits, such as ADHD and DBP. This is important because it is possible for DBP to have suppressing effects on CU traits (Viding et al., 2012; Lozier et al., 2014) which can mask relations between CU traits and other factors. In addition, it is valuable to acquire knowledge of the cognitive and emotional processes that are uniquely related to CU traits. One reason for this is that children and adolescents with DBP and high levels of CU traits, compared with DBP and normative levels of CU traits, show poor response to treatments (Frick et al., 2014; Frick, 2016; Pisano et al., 2017). By controlling for common comorbid symptoms, we may contribute to better treatments for individuals with high levels of CU traits.

Despite our efforts to control for the low-level perceptual features of the images (i.e., contrast and luminance), it is still possible that the measures we used are not completely accurate due to differences in scanning patterns. That is, if different participants scan the images differently, they may spend more time in areas that are higher or lower on these features than for the images as a whole. We believe this is unlikely to affect our results for several reasons. First, pupil size correlated well with self-ratings of arousal, suggesting that measured pupil dilation was related to participants’ subjective emotional reactivity. Second, there was significant variability across the images such that there was no consistent difference for arousing aspects of the image to be lighter or darker than the background. Thus, even if individuals scanned the images differently, it would be unlikely to result in confounded pupil size over trials.

A second possible limitation is that we cannot reliably test the effects of having social content in the stimulus images. The images were not initially selected to be able to test this factor and thus were not evenly distributed as far as valence and arousal. In addition, there were 12 images of emotional and neutral faces inter-mixed with the images used in the current study. Viewing these faces could potentially have increased the social nature of the task as a whole. Future research could potentially examine whether the effect of CU traits on emotional responses vary depending on the social content and context of the stimuli.

Conclusion

The current study extends previous findings on emotional reactivity in relation to CU traits. First, we show that CU traits are dimensionally related to lower emotional reactivity in adolescents from a community sample, for both positive and negative stimuli, providing support for the Integrated Emotion Systems model (Blair, 2004, 2006). That is, a lack of engagement and reaction to positive situations, not only negative ones, likely contribute to CU traits and their associated outcomes. Second, we demonstrate that the relation between CU traits and emotional reactivity changes over development. Together these findings may be important for improving treatments for children and adolescents with elevated CU traits.

Data Availability

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

This study has been designed in accordance with the ethics rules of the Swedish Research Council, and reviewed and approved by the Regional Ethics Committee in Uppsala, Sweden (ref. 2010/190).

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the study design. ET, CW, and VW collected the data. ET and CF performed the data analysis and ET drafted the manuscript. CF, CW, GG, and VW provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Funding

This study was supported by a grant from the Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Welfare and Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation (KAW 2012.0120, Wallenberg Academy Fellowship).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00573/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^timestudioproject.com

References

American Psychiatric Association [APA] (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: Author.

Google Scholar

Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous, X., and Warden, D. (2008). Physiologically-indexed and self-perceived affective empathy in conduct-disordered children high and low on callous-unemotional traits. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 39, 503–517. doi: 10.1007/s10578-008-0104-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., and Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. J. Mem. Lang. 59, 390–412. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48.

Google Scholar

Baxter, M. G., and Murray, E. A. (2002). The amygdala and reward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 563–573. doi: 10.1038/nrn875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bélanger, S. A., Andrews, D., Gray, C., and Korczak, D. (2018). ADHD in children and youth: part 1 – etiology, diagnosis, and comorbidity. Paediatr. Child Health 23, 447–453. doi: 10.1093/pch/pxy109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, R. J. J. (2006). “Subcortical brain systems in psychopathy: The amygdala and associated structures,” in Handbook of Psychopathy, ed. C. J. Patric (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 296–312.

Google Scholar

Blair, R. J. R. (1999). Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with psychopathic tendencies. Pers.Individ.Dif. 27, 135–145. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00231-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, R. J. R. (2004). The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the modulation of antisocial behavior. Brain Cogn. 55, 198–208. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00276-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A., and Lang, P. J. (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology 45, 602–607. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00654.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatham, C. H., Frank, M. J., and Munakata, Y. (2009). Pupillometric and behavioral markers of a developmental shift in the temporal dynamics of cognitive control. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 106, 5529–5533. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810002106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, L. A., Welsh, A. H., and Chan, W. (2001). Estimators for the linear regression model based on Winsorized observations. Stat. Sin. 11, 147–172.

Google Scholar

Dadds, M. R., Allen, J. L., McGregor, K., Woolgar, M., Viding, E., and Scott, S. (2014). Callous-unemotional traits in children and mechanisms of impaired eye contact during expressions of love: a treatment target? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 55, 771–780. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dadds, M. R., Fraser, J., Frost, A., and Hawes, D. J. (2005). Disentangling the underlying dimensions of psychopathy and conduct problems in childhood: a community study. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 73, 400–410. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dandreaux, D. M., and Frick, P. J. (2009). Developmental pathways to conduct problems: a further test of the childhood and adolescent-onset distinction. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 37, 375–385. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9261-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawel, A., O’Kearney, R., McKone, E., and Palermo, R. (2012). Not just fear and sadness: meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 2288–2304. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Wied, M., van Boxtel, A., Matthys, W., and Meeus, W. (2012). Verbal, facial and autonomic responses to empathy-eliciting film clips by disruptive male adolescents with high versus low callous-unemotional traits. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 40, 211–223. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9557-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delplanque, S., N’diaye, K., Scherer, K., and Grandjean, D. (2007). Spatial frequencies or emotional effects? A systematic measure of spatial frequencies for IAPS pictures by a discrete wavelet analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 165, 144–150. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.05.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Docherty, M., Boxer, P., Huesmann, L. R., O’Brien, M., and Bushman, B. (2016). Assessing callous-unemotional traits in adolescents: determining cutoff scores for the inventory of callous and unemotional traits. J. Clin. Psychol. 73, 257–278. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, D. W., and Kronenberger, W. G. (2003). Attention-Deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Neurol. Clin. 21, 933–940. doi: 10.1016/S0733-8619(03)00009-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DuPaul, G., Anastopoulos, A., Power, T., Reid, R., Ikeda, M., and Mcgoey, K. (1998). Parent ratings of attention-deficit / hyperactivity disorder symptoms?: factor structure and normative data. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 20, 83–102. doi: 10.1023/A:1023087410712

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edens, J. F., Skeem, J. L., Cruise, K. R., and Cauffman, E. (2001). Assessment of “juvenile psychopathy” and its association with violence: a critical review. Behav. Sci. Law 19, 53–80. doi: 10.1002/bsl.425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Essau, C. A., Sasagawa, S., and Frick, P. J. (2006). Callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of adolescents. Assessment 13, 454–469. doi: 10.1177/1073191106287354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falck-Ytter, T. (2008). Face inversion effects in autism: a combined looking time and pupillometric study. Autism Res. 1, 297–306. doi: 10.1002/aur.45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanti, K. A. (2013). Individual, social, and behavioral factors associated with co-occuring conduct problems and callous unemotional traits. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 41, 811–824. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9726-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanti, K. A., and Centifanti, L. C. M. (2014). Childhood callous-unemotional traits moderate the relation between parenting distress and conduct problems over time. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 45, 173–184. doi: 10.1007/s10578-013-0389-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanti, K. A., Frick, P. J., and Georgiou, S. (2009). Linking callous-unemotional traits to instrumental and non-instrumental forms of aggression. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 31, 285–298. doi: 10.1007/s10862-008-9111-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanti, K. A., Panayiotou, G., Lombardo, M. V., and Kyranides, M. N. (2016). Unemotional on all counts: evidence of reduced affective responses in individuals with high callous-unemotional traits across emotion systems and valences. Soc. Neurosci. 11, 72–87. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1034378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fawcett, C., Wesevich, V., and Gredebäck, G. (2016a). Pupillary contagion in infancy evidence for spontaneous transfer of arousal. Psychol. Sci. 27, 997–1003. doi: 10.1177/0956797616643924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fawcett, C., Wesevich, V., Truedsson, E., Wåhlstedt, C., and Gredebäck, G. (2016b). Callous–unemotional traits affect adolescents’ perception of collaboration. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57, 1400–1406. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forslund, T., Brocki, K. C., Bohlin, G., Granqvist, P., and Eninger, L. (2016). The heterogeneity of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and conduct problems: cognitive inhibition, emotion regulation, emotionality, and disorganized attachment. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 34, 371–387. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fowles, D. C., and Dindo, L. (2009). Temperament and psychopathy a dual-pathway model. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 18, 179–183. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01632.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frick, P. J. (2004). The Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits. New Orleans, LA: The University of New Orleans.

Google Scholar

Frick, P. J. (2016). Early identification and treatment of antisocial behavior. Pediatr. Clin. North Am. 63, 861–871. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2016.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frick, P. J., and Ray, J. V. (2015). Evaluating callous-unemotional traits as a personality construct. J. Personal. 83, 710–722. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frick, P. J., Ray, J. V., Thornton, L. C., and Kahn, R. E. (2014). Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychol. Bull. 140, 1–57. doi: 10.1037/a0033076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frick, P. J., and Viding, E. (2009). Antisocial behavior from a developmental psychopathology perspective. Dev. Psychopathol. 21, 1111–1131. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409990071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fung, M. T., Raine, A., Loeber, R., Lynam, D. R., Steinhauer, S. R., Venables, P. H., et al. (2005). Reduced electrodermal activity in psychopathy-prone adolescents. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114, 187–196. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geangu, E., Hauf, P., Bhardwaj, R., and Bentz, W. (2011). Infant pupil diameter changes in response to others’ positive and negative emotions. PLoS One 6:e27132. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 38, 581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graur, S., and Siegle, G. (2013). Pupillary motility: bringing neuroscience to the psychiatry clinic of the future. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 13:365. doi: 10.1007/s11910-013-0365-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gredebäck, G., Eriksson, M., Schmitow, C., Laeng, B., and Stenberg, G. (2012). Individual differences in face processing: infants’ scanning patterns and pupil dilations are influenced by the distribution of parental leave. Infancy 17, 79–101. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00091.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gredebäck, G., and Melinder, A. (2010). Infants’ understanding of everyday social interactions: a dual process account. Cognition 114, 197–206. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gredebäck, G., and Melinder, A. (2011). Teleological reasoning in 4-month-old infants: pupil dilations and contextual constraints. PLoS One 6:e26487. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hellmer, K., Söderlund, H., and Gredebäck, G. (2016). The eye of the retriever: developing episodic memory mechanisms in preverbal infants assessed through pupil dilation. Dev. Sci. 21:e12520. doi: 10.1111/desc.12520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hepach, R., and Westermann, G. (2013). Infants’ sensitivity to the congruence of others’ emotions and actions. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 115, 16–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernandez-Lallement, J., Wingerden, M. V., and Kalenscher, T. (2018). Towards an animal model of callousness. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 91, 121–129. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herpers, P. C., Scheepers, F. E., Bons, D. M., Buitelaar, J. K., and Rommelse, N. N. (2014). The cognitive and neural correlates of psychopathy and especially callous–unemotional traits in youths: a systematic review of the evidence. Dev. Psychopathol. 26, 245–273. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herpers, P. C. M., Rommelse, N. N. J., Bons, D. M. A., Buitelaar, J. K., and Scheepers, F. E. (2012). Callous-unemotional traits as a cross-disorders construct. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 47, 2045–2064. doi: 10.1007/s00127-012-0513-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herpertz, S. C., Werth, U., Lukas, G., Qunaibi, M., Schuerkens, A., Kunert, H. J., et al. (2001). Emotion in criminal offenders with psychopathy and borderline personality disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 58, 737–745. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.8.737

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isen, J., Raine, A., Baker, L. A., Dawson, M. E., Bezdjian, S., and Lozano, D. I. (2010). Sex-specific association between psychopathic traits and electrodermal reactivity in children. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 119, 216–225. doi: 10.1037/a0017777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamphaus, R. W., and Frick, P. J. (1996). The Clinical Assessment of Children’s Emotion, Behavior, and Personality. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Google Scholar

Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Dev. 64, 325–347. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02913.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laeng, B., Sirois, S., and Gredebäck, G. (2012). Pupillometry a window to the preconscious? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 18–27. doi: 10.1177/1745691611427305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P., and Bradley, M. M. (2007). “The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) in the study of emotion and attention,” in Series in Affective Science. Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment, eds J. A. Coan and J. J. B. Allen (New York, NY: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., and Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction Manual. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.

Google Scholar

Lozier, L. M., Cardinale, E. M., VanMeter, J. W., and Marsh, A. A. (2014). Mediation of the relationship between callous-unemotional traits and proactive aggression by amygdala response to fear among children with conduct problems. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 627–636. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.4540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lykken, D. T. (1995). The Antisocial Personalities. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Marini, V. A., and Stickle, T. R. (2010). Evidence for deficits in reward responsivity in antisocial youth with callous-unemotional traits. Personal. Disord. 1, 218–229. doi: 10.1037/a0017675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, A. A., Finger, E. C., Mitchell, D. G., Reid, M. E., Sims, C., Kosson, D. S., et al. (2008). Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 712–720. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07071145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masi, G., Milone, A., Pisano, S., Lenzi, F., Muratori, P., Gemo, I., et al. (2014). Emotional reactivity in referred youth with disruptive behavior disorders: the role of the callous-unemotional traits. Psychiatry Res. 220, 426–432. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.07.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michalska, K. J., and Davis, E. L. (2018). The psychobiology of emotional development: the case for examining sociocultural processes. Dev. Psychobiol. doi: 10.1002/dev.21795

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michonski, J. D., and Sharp, C. (2010). Revisiting Lynam’s notion of the “fledgling psychopath”: are HIA-CP children truly psychopathic-like? Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 4:24. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-4-24

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moran, P., Ford, T., Butler, G., and Goodman, R. (2008). Callous and unemotional traits in children and adolescents living in great britain. Br. J. Psychiatry 192, 65–66. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.034876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Musser, E. D., Galloway-Long, H. S., Frick, P. J., and Nigg, J. T. (2013). Emotion regulation and heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 52, 163–171. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyström, P., Falck-Ytter, T., and Gredebäck, G. (2015a). The timestudio project: an open source scientific workflow system for the behavioral and brain sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 48, 542–552. doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0616-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyström, P., Gredebäck, G., Bölte, S., and Falck-Ytter, T. (2015b). Hypersensitive pupillary light reflex in infants at risk for autism. Mol. Autism 6:10. doi: 10.1186/s13229-015-0011-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pardini, D. A., Lochman, J. E., and Frick, P. J. (2003). Callous/unemotional traits and social-cognitive processes in adjudicated youths. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 42, 364–371. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200303000-00018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pasalich, D. S., Dadds, M. R., Hawes, D. J., and Brennan, J. (2011). Do callous-unemotional traits moderate the relative importance of parental coercion versus warmth in child conduct problems? An observational study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 52, 1308–1315. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02435.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patrick, C. J. (1994). Emotion and psychopathy: startling new insights. Psychophysiology 31, 319–330. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb02440.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., and Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion in the criminal psychopath: startle reflex modulation. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 102, 82–92. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.102.1.82

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pisano, S., Muratori, P., Gorga, C., Levantini, V., Luliano, R., Catone, G., et al. (2017). Conduct disorders and psychopathy in children and adolescents: aetiology, clinical presentation and treatment strategies of callous-unemotional traits. Italien J. Pediatr. 43, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/s13052-017-0404-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Development Core Team (2014). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (Version 3.1.1) [Computer software]. Available at: https://www.r-project.org/index.html

Rommelse, N. N., Altink, M. E., Fliers, E. A., Martinm, N. C., Buschgens, C. J., Hartman, C. A., et al. (2009). Comorbid problem in ADHD: degree of association, shared endophenotypes, and formation of distinct subtypes. Implications for a future DSM. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 37, 793–804. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9312-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roose, A., Bijttebier, P., Decoene, S., Claes, L., and Frick, P. J. (2010). Assessing the affective features of psychopathy in adolescence: a further validation of the inventory of callous and unemotional traits. Assessment 17, 44–57. doi: 10.1177/1073191109344153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, R., Costello, E. J., Angold, A., Copeland, W. E., and Maughan, B. (2010a). Developmental pathways in oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 119, 726–738. doi: 10.1037/a0020798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, R., Maughan, B., Moran, P., Ford, T., Briskman, J., and Goodman, R. (2010b). The role of callous and unemotional traits in the diagnosis of conduct disorder. J. Child Psychol. 51, 688–695. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02199.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seagrave, D., and Grisso, T. (2002). Adolescent development and the measurement of juvenile psychopathy. Law Hum. Behav. 26, 219–239. doi: 10.1023/A:1014696110850

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharp, C., van Goozen, S., and Goodyer, I. (2006). Children’s subjective emotional reactivity to affective pictures: gender differences and their antisocial correlates in an unselected sample of 7-11-year-olds. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 143–150. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01464.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Stenger, V. A., Konecky, R., and Carter, C. S. (2003). Use of concurrent pupil dilation assessment to inform interpretation and analysis of fMRI data. NeuroImage 20, 114–124. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00298-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirois, S., and Jackson, I. R. (2012). Pupil dilation and object permanence in infants. Infancy 17, 61–78. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00096.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stickle, T. R., Kirkpatrick, N. M., and Brush, L. N. (2009). Callous-unemotional traits and social information processing: multiple risk-factor models for understanding aggressive behavior in antisocial youth. Law Hum. Behav. 33, 515–529. doi: 10.1007/s10979-008-9171-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, L. L., Otten, R., Engels, R. C., Vermulst, A. A., and Janssens, J. M. (2010). Psychometric properties of the parent and teacher versions of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire for 4-to 12-year-olds: a review. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 13, 254–274. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0071-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Syngelaki, E. M., Fairchild, G., Moore, S. C., Savage, J. C., and van Goozen, S. H. M. (2013). Affective startle potentiation in juvenile offenders: the role of conduct problems and psychopathic traits. Soc. Neurosci. 8, 112–121. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2012.712549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, R. A. (2014). “Conscience development in early childhood,” in Handbook of Moral Development, 2nd Edn, eds M. Killen and J. G. Smetana (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 73–92.

Google Scholar

Vasey, M. W., El-Hag, N., and Daleidon, E. L. (1996). Anxiety and the processing of emotionally threatening stimuli: distinctive patterns of selective attention among high and low-test-anxious children. Child Dev. 67, 1173–1185. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01789.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viding, E., Sebastian, C. L., Dadds, M. R., Lockwood, P. L., Cecil, C. A., De Brito, S. A., et al. (2012). Amygdala response to preattentive masked fear in children with conduct problems: the role of callous-unemotional traits. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 1109–1116. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12020191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wåhlstedt, C. (2009). Neuropsychological deficits in relation to symptoms of ADHD: independent contributions and interactions. Child Neuropsychol. 15, 262–279. doi: 10.1080/09297040802524198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wåhlstedt, C., and Bohlin, G. (2010). DSM-IV-defined inattention and sluggish cognitive tempo: independent and interactive relations to neuropsychological factors and comorbidity. Child Neuropsychol. 16, 350–365. doi: 10.1080/09297041003671176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waller, R., Gardner, F., Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., and Wilson, M. N. (2012). Do harsh and positive parenting predict parent reports of deceitful-callous behavior in early childhood? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 946–953. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02550.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, S. F., Marsh, A. A., Fowler, K. A., Schechter, J. C., Adalio, C., Pope, K., et al. (2012). Reduced amygdala response in youths with disruptive behavior disorders and psychopathic traits: decreased emotional response versus increased top-down attention to nonemotional features. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 750–758. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11081270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: callous-unemotional traits, emotional reactivity, pupil dilation, longitudinal, community sample

Citation: Truedsson E, Fawcett C, Wesevich V, Gredebäck G and Wåhlstedt C (2019) The Role of Callous-Unemotional Traits on Adolescent Positive and Negative Emotional Reactivity: A Longitudinal Community-Based Study. Front. Psychol. 10:573. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00573

Received: 15 November 2018; Accepted: 28 February 2019;
Published: 15 March 2019.

Edited by:

Caspar Addyman, Goldsmiths, University of London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Sarah Jessen, Universität zu Lübeck, Germany
Elena Nava, University of Milano-Bicocca, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Truedsson, Fawcett, Wesevich, Gredebäck and Wåhlstedt. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Cecilia Wåhlstedt, cecilia.wahlstedt@psyk.uu.se

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.