Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 08 May 2019
Sec. Movement Science

Perceived Impact as the Underpinning Mechanism of the End-Spurt and U-Shape Pacing Patterns

Updated
  • School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel

The end-spurt and U-shape reflect common pacing patterns across a variety of fields (e.g., running, swimming, cycling). To date, however, the literature lacks a clear, parsimonious account for these effects. Here, I propose these pacing patterns can be accounted for by a psychological mechanism termed perceived impact. As athletes perceive their actions to better affect task-progress, they become more motivated and perform better accordingly. To illustrate, if an athlete has five more laps to go during a race, completing the current lap closes 20% of the remaining distance. Alternatively, when she has two more laps to go, the current lap closes 50% of the remaining distance. In the latter case, the impact of completing a single lap on task-progress is perceived to be higher. Her motivation will increase accordingly near the end of the track – giving rise to an end-spurt. I demonstrate the mechanism’s predictive power by reproducing previous findings through simulations. I then move to discuss how this framework is theoretically insightful in view of previous accounts such as the Central Governor Model and the Psyco-Biological Model. I conclude this work with applied strategies for practitioners in their daily routines.

Introduction

People set goals in their everyday lives. For example, a researcher might set the goal of submitting a grant proposal before a deadline. If she got only one week left, she will likely work hard writing the grant despite severe sleep deprivation. Athletes also set goals in competetive events. For example, in a one-mile race, an athlete might set the goal of “finish the race as fast as possible.” If she got only one lap to go, she will likely perform at her best despite extreme pain. The latter example reflects a pacing pattern known as an “end-spurt” (e.g., Tucker et al., 2006). End-spurts are commonly preceded by an initial decline in performance, from the starting point of a task to its middle; a pacing pattern known as a “U-shape” (e.g., McGibbon et al., 2018).

I suggest the end-spurt and the related U-shape patterns in pacing stem from a simple motivational mechanism – perceived impact: the perceived extent by which one’s actions influence goal-progress. This mechanism is demonstrated in everyday lives and in competitive sports (e.g., when consumers walk toward the end of an aisle in a supermarket; Van Den Bergh et al., 2016; Open-water swimmers during the world championship; Veiga et al., 2019). Indeed, exercise scientists previously suggested that the end-spurt mostly stems from psychological rather than physiological mechanisms (e.g., centrally induced fatigue, perception of effort, concious experience; Noakes, 2012; Smirmaul et al., 2013; Renfree et al., 2014).

Notably, coaches rarely rely on scientific work in their practice (e.g., Stoszkowski and Collins, 2016); which led to suggestions such as including case studies in quantitative research (Halperin, 2018). Some researchers further emphasized that single-case designs could be usefull in applied settings to identify applied principles, orient practice and develop applied intervention procedures for both team and individual sports (e.g., Di Fronso et al., 2016). One reason for this behavior might be the complexity of scientific theories. Previous models of pacing behavior are rather complex – consisting of numerous factors (e.g., rate of heat accumulation, level of motivation, extent of fluid loss, degree of self-belief, extent of muscle fatigue, emotional state, extent of mental fatigue, caffeine intake, amphetamines intake, concious/non-concious experiences, etc., Noakes, 2012; Edwards and Polman, 2013) – and hence are harder to interpret and apply in the field. Due to its simplicity, the proposed mechanism of perceived impact helps closing the gap between coaches and researchers – it consists of only one, easily interpretable factor, and can be efficiently implemented in practice.

In the present work, I first discuss the end-spurt and U-shape patterns in sports science and psychology. I then move to discuss the theoretical framework of the perceived impact mechanism and exemplify how it can replicate findings from Tucker et al. (2006) using simulations. I discuss the mechanism in view of models from exercise and sports science such as the Psycho-Biological Model (PBM; Smirmaul et al., 2013), the Central Governor Model (CGM; Noakes, 2012) and conciousness based accounts (e.g., Edwards and Polman, 2013). Last, I discuss how the present mechanism integrates, differentiates and extends these accounts and suggest practical implications to the field.

The End-Spurt and U-Shape: an Overview

The end-spurt refers to athletes’ tendency to increase effort (i.e., velocity, power output) closer to the end-point of a physical task (Noakes, 2012; Edwards and Polman, 2013). An end-spurt is commonly preceded by an initial decline in effort from the starting point to the middle of the task; which gives rise to a U-shape pattern. In other words, a U-shape pattern refers to athletes’ tendency to decrease effort mid-way through a physical task, yet increase their effort back again near its end (Edwards and Polman, 2013). The end-spurt pattern in pacing was reliably documented in a variety of domains: repetitive maximal voluntary contractions (Halperin et al., 2014), repetitive self-paced concentric knee extension (Froyd et al., 2016), first serve velocity in tennis (Martin et al., 2019), races of 1600 m and up to 10 k (Tucker et al., 2006), rowing of 2000 m (Garland, 2005; Muehlbauer et al., 2010), swimming of 400 m and up to 25 k (McGibbon et al., 2018; Veiga et al., 2019) and cycling (Foster et al., 2004). For example, a typical 1600 m race athlete would start at a high velocity in the first lap, decrease his velocity during the second and third lap, but would increase his velocity again in the final lap (demonstrating an end-spurt which is a part of a U-shape pattern; Tucker et al., 2006).

The end-spurt, as well as the U-shape patterns, were also studied in psychology. This literature, however, remained mostly uncited in sports science. A large body of psychological research referred to end-spurts as “goal gradients” (Hull, 1932, 1934, 1938); which are defined as the tendency of both animals and humans to increase their effort near their goal’s end state (e.g., Brown, 1948; Losco and Epstein, 1977; Bonezzi et al., 2011; for a review see Heilizer, 1977). For example, when presented with food, mice increase their running velocity (e.g., Hull, 1934) and exert more pulling strength (e.g., Brown, 1948) the closer they are to the track’s end. Similarly, humans increase their walking speed as they approach the end of an aisle in a supermarket (Van Den Bergh et al., 2016); are more likely to donate money to a charity campaign if they are told the target amount is about to be reached (Koo and Fishbach, 2008; Cryder et al., 2013); and perform better in solving simple math problems near the end of their task (Catalano, 1974). People also buy coffee more frequently near to the end of their stamp-card (Kivetz et al., 2006; see Koo and Fishbach, 2012, for similar results with sushi).

A number of studies also referred to U-shape patterns in effort allocation (e.g., Muehlbauer et al., 2010; McGibbon et al., 2018) as the “stuck-in-the-middle” effect. For example, students perform better in a “words in a word” game; successfully constructing more words out of the target word in the beginning and end of a nine-word series, compared to its middle (Bonezzi et al., 2011; Study 1). People were also found to demonstrate a stuck-in-the-middle pattern when correcting typos through a series of nine articles; finding typos at a higher rate in the beginning and end of the series compared to its middle (Bonezzi et al., 2011; Study 3). The stuck-in-the-middle effect was also found while following religious traditions: Jewish students were more likely to light Hanukah candles (a Jewish holiday that lasts eight days, in which people traditionally light candles every day) in the first and last days of the holiday compared to its middle (Touré-Tillery and Fishbach, 2012).

Overall, the end-spurt and U-shape patterns in sports reflect specific cases of the goal gradient and stuck-in-the-middle effects in psychology, when the latter are observed in physical tasks. These two patterns, however, were rarely discussed in the same context in the sports science literature, and received little attention regarding their common underlying mechanisms.

Theoretical Grounds of the Perceived Impact Mechanism

Psychologists suggested that the goal gradient and stuck-in-the-middle patterns stem from a common mechanism: the perceived impact of one’s current action1. The perceived impact mechanism relies on phenomena from three domains: motivation, perception, and progress-monitoring.

Expectancy-value models from the motivation literature (e.g., Atkinson, 1957; see Feather, 1982 for a review) suggest that motivation increases with higher influence actions are expected to have on goal progress (e.g., “how plausible it is that I can achieve this goal”/“how hard would it be for me to achieve this goal?”). For example, a 12-year old who is doing well relative to his class in football, is more likely to continue and play football later on in his life. In other words, motivation increases with higher perceived impact (e.g., perceived impact as competence; Ryan and Deci, 2000; perceived impact as self-efficacy; Bandura, 1977).

In goal-pursuits, a unit of progress is referred to as a “step” toward task completion. In the case of a goal gradient, the perceived impact of the current step is determined by the ratio between this step and the required steps for reaching the task’s end (e.g., reading one page out of 200; completing the first lap out of four; Förster et al., 1998; Heath et al., 1999).

As one gets closer to task completion, each step is perceived to reduce a larger portion of the remaining gap to the task’s end. For example, the first step out of five (i.e., five steps to go) reduces 20% of the five-step gap; whereas the fourth step (i.e., two steps to go) reduces 50% of the two-step gap to the task’s end. A step is perceived as more impactful the larger the gap it reduces, and correspondingly induces higher levels of motivation (e.g., Förster et al., 1998; Heath et al., 1999; Koo and Fishbach, 2012; Cryder et al., 2013).

The stuck-in-the-middle effect is explained by the same mechanism on a broader context of progress-monitoring. Generally, people monitor their rate of progress in goal-pursuits (e.g., Carver and Scheier, 2002). In goal gradients, people use the task’s end for progress-monitoring. Accordingly, if the starting point serves as the reference for monitoring progress, then closer to the starting point perceived impact would be higher and hence, motivation would be higher than later on (e.g., reading one page out of two so far; completing one lap out of three so far; Bonezzi et al., 2011; Koo and Fishbach, 2012). If both the starting and end points can serve as references, people tend to use the nearest reference point for progress-monitoring, or in other words, follow “the small area” principle (Koo and Fishbach, 2012). In such cases, people would use the starting point as a reference as they start, and then switch to use the end point as a reference mid-way through the task (Bonezzi et al., 2011). This behavior gives rise to the stuck-in-the-middle effect: actors invest more effort in the beginning of the task and toward its end (when a reference point is close) and less through the task’s middle (where both reference points are far).

Formulation of the Perceived Impact Mechanism

To illustrate, perceived impact of a step in goal-pursuit can be stated formally by a simple function. For example, let a step in goal-progress be equal to one, s be a series of numbers in an increasing order, representing the index of each step (e.g., s = 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7), and PIsi a number between 0 to 100, representing the percent of perceived impact of the current step si (e.g., PIsi = 50%; representing the impact of the current step out of the maximum possible impact a step can have on goal-progress). Accordingly, min(s) and max(s) are the smallest and highest values in s, which represent the starting- and ending-points, respectively. According to the small area principle, people use the nearest reference point, and tend to switch between the beginning and ending points in the middle of the task. Therefore, if si < max(s)2, then:

PIsi=1min(s)+si          (1)

Else:

PIsi=1max(s)si          (2)

Figure 1 depicts PIsi through each of a seven steps series according to the formula. As noted earlier, the pattern of perceived impact through goal progress corresponds to U-shape/the stuck-in-the-middle pattern.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Percent of perceived impact in each step within a seven steps series. In the beginning of the task, the starting point is used as a reference (step 1), thus the percieved impact is higher near the beginning. The reference point is then switched when crossing the middle (step 4) to the end point (step 7) of the task, and hence percieved impact grows as the end point draws near. Overall, this creates a U-shape pattern in percieved impact and therefore a U-shape/stuck-in-the-middle pattern in motivation and effort exertion.

This formula can be further adjusted to simulate real-world data. To illustrate, I partially reproduced Figure 5 from Tucker et al. (2006). Tucker et al. (2006) analyzed pacing patterns in men’s world record performances in 5000-m (n = 32) and 10,000-m (n = 34) events between the years 1921 to 20042. In addition, they presented unpublished data from one mile events (sample size not stated). Figure 5 from Tucker et al.’s (2006) paper presents mean running speed by event interval in each of the three datasets. Their results indicated that the final interval was faster than the preceding one, and the overall pacing pattern corresponded to a U-shape across all intervals in all events from one-mile to 10,000 m.

The simulations followed the formula in equations (1) and (2). A step was defined as a progress of 100 m in a race, e.g., a one mile event contained 16 steps. Three additional parameters were added to the model: a constant (k) which controls for the average pace in each event, a scaling factor which controls for the steepness of the curve in each event, and a random noise factor which controls for the between-subject variance in each step. The simulation produced three separate datasets for each event (i.e., one-mile, 5000-m, 10,000-m) according to the sample size stated in Tucker et al. (2006), except for the one-mile event in which I estimated the sample size to be 30 (see Figure 2). The R synax used for the simulations and generated datasets are available in the Supplemental Materials section2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Simulation results based on Tucker et al.’s (2006) Figure 5. Average running speed divided by intervals during men world-record performances in one-mile, 5000-m, and 10,000-m events. Error bars represent 95% confidence interval.

In order to determine a U-shape pattern, I tested a quadratic regression for each of the events; a U-shape is indicated by a significant interval3 coefficient in the regression model. The regression results indicated a U-shape pattern in all events (see Table 1). Next, I tested for an end-spurt by a paired sample t-test that compared the last interval to its preceding one in each event. In all events, the last interval was faster than its preceding interval: one-mile, t(29) = −3.45, p = 0.001, d = 0.62; 5000-m, t(31) = −4.41, p < 0.001, d = 0.78; 10,000-m, t(33) = −4.35, p < 0.001, d = 0.74. These results indicated an end-spurt in all events.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. A summary of the quadratic regression results in each of the simulated events.

Overall, the simulated data that was generated according to the perceived impact model succesfully replicated Tucker et al. (2006) results, demostrating an end-spurt which was a part of a more general U-shape pattern.

Integration of the Perceived Impact Mechanism With Previous Models

Notably, the end-spurt was previously mentioned in the context of fatigue related models in sports science: (1) the CGM (Noakes and Marino, 2007; Tucker and Noakes, 2009; Noakes, 2012), according to which the brain induces fatigue in the course of a task in order to maintain homeostasis, and (2) the PBM (Marcora, 2008, 2010; Marcora and Staiano, 2010; Smirmaul et al., 2013), according to which the perception of effort increases in the course of a task until reaching the limit of potential motivavtion, which determines task disengagement.

Although differ in their theoretical approaches, both the CGM and the PBM share a number of common principles that may underly the end-spurt phenomena: (1) both may suggest that end-spurts occur due to a reduction of uncertainty regarding the amount of effort to be exerted closer to the end-point (e.g., Smirmaul et al., 2013), and (2) pacing patterns are caused due to a brain based mechanim, rather than the athletes’ physiology (e.g., lack of available muscle energy, an increase in blood lactate, etc., Noakes and Marino, 2007; Tucker and Noakes, 2009; Noakes, 2012).

The perceived impact mechanism does not contradict either one of the CGM or the PBM frameworks; perceived impact can account for the end-spurt and U-shape patterns in view of both. According to the CGM, for example, effort can be regarded as more effective the less its investment risks homeostasis. Hence, as the end of a race draws near, time until task disengagement decreases along with the risk to homeostasis – making effort perceived as more impactful. According to the PBM, for example, investing effort is perceived to be more effective closer to the end of a race because it is less likely for an athlete to reach the point of exhaustion before the race ends (i.e., for the perception of effort to increase past the limit set by potential motivation). Of note, the perceived impact mechanism is theoretically distinct; and emphasizes motivational rather than affective changes in the course of a task, as underlying pacing patterns (e.g., PBM – perception of effort, CGM – fatigue).

Other mechanisms were also suggested to account for the end-spurt, some partially rely on previous models such as the CGM. These accounts emphasize conciousness and decision-making as the determinants of pacing behavior (e.g., Edwards and Polman, 2013; Renfree et al., 2014; Micklewright et al., 2017). For example, the decision to increase effort near the end of the race is determined by the athlete’s decision, made through different levels of consciousness. This decision may itself be the result of a heuristic – a rule of thumb in decision making used instead of deliberate thinking (e.g., “one must increase effort near the end of the race”). These accounts explicitly state possible mechanisms underlying the end-spurt. However, as was previously noted, mice were also found to demonstrate an end-spurt. This suggests that: (1) a relatively low level of consciousness is involved in this effect, and (2) heuristics-based models may not be sufficient to account for this behavior, as they were studied and applied specifically in humans.

In general, consciousness related models are of high theoretical value, yet offer a complex and maybe even an untestable account for the end-spurt. It is unclear how the consciousness factor would affect pacing behavior (e.g., would a decision made in low rather than high consciousness levels increase the magnitude of the end-spurt?). The perceived impact mechanism accounts for the end-spurt parsimoniously using a single factor (i.e., an increase in motivation with proximity to a reference point), which was validated in the literature using experimental data, and, as demonstrated above, is able to conceptually replicate the results of former studies through simulations4.

Practical Implication of the Perceived Impact Mechanism

The perceived impact mechanism offers strong, field-relevant predictions regarding athletes’ level of motivation through physical tasks. It can therefore offer practical applications for both coaches and athletes. For example, an athletes is expected to maximize her performance near the starting-point of a race, when instructed to monitor her progress in reference to the distance passed so far (e.g., “one lap completed”). Similarly, she is also expected to maximize performance when instructed to monitor progress in reference to the remaining distance to the end of the race near the end point (e.g., “one lap to go”). This prediction is partially supported by previous findings in the field of consumer behavior (e.g., Koo and Fishbach, 2012).

Apart from athletes, the mechanism also offers relevant implications for the general population. People naturally tend to adopt arbitrary reference points when performing a demanding physical task (e.g., Allen et al., 2016). Trainers can utilize this tendency to increase their trainees’ motivation and performance in training sessions, by simply counting the set repetitions or seconds in either an ascending or a descending order. For example, when assigning a trainee with the task of doing 20 squats, a trainer can count the repetitions in an ascending order from repetition one to 10 (e.g., “one,” “two,” “three”), and then switch to count in a descending order (e.g., “three,” “two,” “one”). On a broader perspective, trainees might tend to be more motivated in the beginning and end of a training session. Trainers can thereby construct the whole training session accordingly – with challenging exercises assigned to the beginning and end of the session, while easier exercises assigned to its middle.

Conclusion

This work reviewed the end-spurt and U-shape patterns both in the physical and mental domains, and suggests a unifying framework for their understanding – the perceived impact mechanism. This account builds upon a single motivational factor, perceived impact, as the main determinant of these patterns both in sports science and psychology, and bears high theoretical and practical value.

Author Contributions

AE wrote the manuscript, simulations and analyzed the data.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank Israel Halperin for his kind assistance and motivational support during the preparation of this manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01082/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ I will refer perceived impact of one’s current action as perceived impact.
  2. ^ The authors also analyzed the pacing pattern of an 800-m event in which end-spurts were not found. Hence, these events were not included in the model.
  3. ^ The simulation includes an option to adjust parameters given real data.
  4. ^ Of note, the mentioned phenomena are discussed in the context of human physiological constraints, and the mechanism accounts only for situations in which an increase in effort near the end of a task was observed so far (e.g., 10 k races).

References

Allen, E. J., Dechow, P. M., Pope, D. G., and Wu, G. (2016). Reference-dependent preferences: evidence from marathon runners. Manag. Sci. 63, 1657–1672. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.2015.2417

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychol. Rev. 64 (6 Pt 1), 359–372. doi: 10.1037/h0043445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 84, 191–215. doi: 10.1037//0033-295x.84.2.191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonezzi, A., Brendl, C. M., and De Angelis, M. (2011). Stuck in the middle: the psychophysics of goal pursuit. Psychol. Sci. 22, 607–612. doi: 10.1177/0956797611404899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, J. S. (1948). Gradients of approach and avoidance responses and their relation to level of motivation. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 41, 450–465. doi: 10.1037/h0055463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., and Scheier, M. F. (2002). Control processes and self-organization as complementary principles underlying behavior. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6, 304–315. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0604_05

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catalano, J. F. (1974). End-spurt in addition of numbers. Percept. Motor Skills 39, 121–122. doi: 10.2466/pms.1974.39.1.121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cryder, C. E., Loewenstein, G., and Seltman, H. (2013). Goal gradient in helping behavior. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 1078–1083. doi: 10.1155/2019/7320365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Fronso, S., Robazza, C., Edson Filho, L. B., Comani, S., and Bertollo, M. (2016). Neural markers of performance states in an olympic athlete: an EEG case study in air-pistol shooting. J. Sports Sci. Med. 15, 214–222.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Edwards, A. M., and Polman, R. C. J. (2013). Pacing and awareness: brain regulation of physical activity. Sports Med. 43, 1057–1064. doi: 10.1007/s40279-013-0091-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feather, N. T. (1982). Expectations and Actions: Expectancy-Value Models in Psychology. New Jersey, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Förster, J., Higgins, E. T., and Idson, L. C. (1998). Approach and avoidance strength during goal attainment: regulatory focus and the “goal looms larger” effect. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 75, 1115–1131. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.75.5.1115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, C., deKoning, J. J., Hettinga, F., Lampen, J., Dodge, C., Bobbert, M., et al. (2004). Effect of competitive distance on energy expenditure during simulated competition. Int. J. Sports Med. 25, 198–204. doi: 10.1055/s-2003-45260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Froyd, C., Beltrami, F. G., Millet, G. Y., and Noakes, T. D. (2016). Central regulation and neuromuscular fatigue during exercise of different durations. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 1024–1032. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garland, S. W. (2005). An analysis of the pacing strategy adopted by elite competitors in 2000 m rowing. Br. J. Sports Med. 39, 39–42. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2003.010801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halperin, I. (2018). Case studies in exercise and sport sciences: a powerful tool to bridge the science-practice gap. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 13, 824–825. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.2018-0185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halperin, I., Aboodarda, S. J., Basset, F. A., Byrne, J. M., and Behm, D. G. (2014). Pacing strategies during repeated maximal voluntary contractions. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 114, 1413–1420. doi: 10.1007/s00421-014-2872-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heath, C., Larrick, R. P., and Wu, G. (1999). Goals as reference points. Cogn. Psychol. 38, 79–109. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heilizer, F. (1977). A review of theory and research on the assumptions of Miller’s response competition (conflict) models: response gradients. J. Gen. Psychol. 97, 17–71. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1977.9918503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hull, C. L. (1932). The goal-gradient hypothesis and maze learning. Psychol. Rev. 39, 25–43. doi: 10.1037/h0072640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hull, C. L. (1934). The rat’s speed-of-locomotion gradient in the approach to food. J. Comp. Psychol. 17, 393–422. doi: 10.1037/h0071299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hull, C. L. (1938). The goal-gradient hypothesis applied to some” field-force’ problems in the behavior of young children. Psychol. Rev. 45, 271–299. doi: 10.1037/h0053885

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kivetz, R., Urminsky, O., and Zheng, Y. (2006). The goal-gradient hypothesis resurrected: purchase acceleration, illusionary goal progress, and customer retention. J. Mark. Res. 43, 39–58. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.43.1.39

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koo, M., and Fishbach, A. (2008). Dynamics of self-regulation: how (un) accomplished goal actions affect motivation. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 94, 183–195. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.2.183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koo, M., and Fishbach, A. (2012). The small-area hypothesis: effects of progress monitoring on goal adherence. J. Consum. Res. 39, 493–509. doi: 10.1086/663827

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Losco, J., and Epstein, S. (1977). Relative steepness of approach and avoidance gradients as a function of magnitude and valence of incentive. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 86, 360–368. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.86.4.360

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcora, S. (2010). Counterpoint: afferent feedback from fatigued locomotor muscles is not an important determinant of endurance exercise performance. J. Appl. Physiol. 108, 454–456. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00976.2009a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcora, S. M. (2008). Do we really need a central governor to explain brain regulation of exercise performance?. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 104, 929–931. doi: 10.1007/s00421-008-0818-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcora, S. M., and Staiano, W. (2010). The limit to exercise tolerance in humans: mind over muscle?. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 109, 763–770. doi: 10.1007/s00421-010-1418-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, C., Bideau, B., Touzard, P., and Kulpa, R. (2019). Identification of serve pacing strategies during five-set tennis matches. Int. J. Sports Sci. Coach. 14, 32–42. doi: 10.1177/1747954118806682

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGibbon, K. E., Pyne, D. B., Shephard, M. E., and Thompson, K. G. (2018). Pacing in swimming: a systematic review. Sports Med. 48, 1621–1633. doi: 10.1007/s40279-018-0901-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Micklewright, D., Kegerreis, S., Raglin, J., and Hettinga, F. (2017). Will the conscious–subconscious pacing Quagmire help elucidate the mechanisms of self-paced exercise? new opportunities in dual process theory and process tracing methods. Sports Med. 47, 1231–1239. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0642-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muehlbauer, T., Schindler, C., and Widmer, A. (2010). Pacing pattern and performance during the 2008 Olympic rowing regatta. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 10, 291–296. doi: 10.1080/17461390903426659

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noakes, T. D., and Marino, F. E. (2007). Arterial oxygenation, central motor output and exercise performance in humans. J. Physiol. 585, 919–921. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.145110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noakes, T. D. O. (2012). Fatigue is a brain-derived emotion that regulates the exercise behavior to ensure the protection of whole body homeostasis. Front. Physiol. 3:82. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2012.00082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Renfree, A., Martin, L., Micklewright, D., and Gibson, A. S. C. (2014). Application of decision-making theory to the regulation of muscular work rate during self-paced competitive endurance activity. Sports Med. 44, 147–158. doi: 10.1007/s40279-013-0107-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.55.1.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smirmaul, B. P. C., Dantas, J. L., Nakamura, F. Y., and Pereira, G. (2013). The psychobiological model: a new explanation to intensity regulation and (in) tolerance in endurance exercise. Revista Brasileira de Educação Física e Esporte 27, 333–340. doi: 10.1590/s1807-55092013005000008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoszkowski, J., and Collins, D. (2016). Sources, topics and use of knowledge by coaches. J. Sports Sci. 34, 794–802. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2015.1072279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Touré-Tillery, M., and Fishbach, A. (2012). The end justifies the means, but only in the middle. J. Exp. Psychol. 141, 570–583. doi: 10.1037/a0025928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker, R., Lambert, M. I., and Noakes, T. D. (2006). An analysis of pacing strategies during men’s world-record performances in track athletics. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 1, 233–245. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.1.3.233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker, R., and Noakes, T. D. (2009). The physiological regulation of pacing strategy during exercise: a critical review. Br. J. Sports Med. 43:e1. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2009.057562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Den Bergh, B., Heuvinck, N., Schellekens, G. A., and Vermeir, I. (2016). Altering speed of locomotion. J. Consum. Res. 43, 407–428. doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucw031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veiga, S., Rodriguez, L., González-Frutos, P., and Navandar, A. (2019). Race strategies of open water swimmers in the 5-km, 10-km and 25-km races of the 2017 FINA World Swimming Championships. Front. Psychol. 10:654. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: motivation, pacing, end-spurt, U-shape, effort

Citation: Emanuel A (2019) Perceived Impact as the Underpinning Mechanism of the End-Spurt and U-Shape Pacing Patterns. Front. Psychol. 10:1082. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01082

Received: 16 November 2018; Accepted: 25 April 2019;
Published: 08 May 2019.

Edited by:

Ana-Maria Cebolla, Free University of Brussels, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Selenia Di Fronso, Università degli Studi G. d’Annunzio Chieti e Pescara, Italy
Beat Knechtle, University Hospital Zurich, Switzerland

Copyright © 2019 Emanuel. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Aviv Emanuel, avivemanuel@mail.tau.ac.il; aviv.emanuel@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.