Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 19 June 2019
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Analysing Psychosocial and Contextual Factors Underpinning Bullying and Cyberbullying View all 21 articles

Psychological Predictors of Bullying in Adolescents From Pluricultural Schools: A Transnational Study in Spain and Ecuador

  • 1Department of Psychology, Universidad de Córdoba, Córdoba, Spain
  • 2Department of Education, University Laica Eloy Alfaro of Manabí, Manta, Ecuador

This study aimed to analyze the levels of personal aggression and victimization, ethnic-cultural aggression and victimization, self-esteem, empathy, social skills and gender in adolescents as potential predictors of bullying in Spain and Ecuador. The wide pluricultural sample comprised secondary education students from both countries (N = 25,190, average age = 13.92, SD = 1.306; NSpain = 14,437; NEcuador = 10,753), who took part in the study by filling in a self-report. The results revealed that predictive models of bullying for both countries explain 50–70% of variance. A transnational predictive pattern of personal victimization can be observed based on the levels of ethnic-cultural victimization, ethnic-cultural aggression, personal aggression, self-deprecation, and affective empathy. A transnational predictive pattern of personal aggression is evidenced depending on the levels of ethnic-cultural aggression, personal victimization, self-deprecation, ethnic-cultural victimization, and the fact of being female. We concluded that bullying can largely be predicted by involvement in ethnic-cultural discrimination. These results are discussed, and educational inferences are drawn for prevention.

Introduction

School is a developmental context for adolescents where they have the opportunity to interact and join a peer group (Salmivalli, 2010; Eccles and Roeser, 2011). In recent decades, the school population has been more and more culturally diverse due to globalization and the increase in migratory flows (Hull and Hellmich, 2018; Kastoryano, 2018). This makes that peer relationships are also relationships between different cultural groups (Marks et al., 2014; Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al., 2018).

In the last decades, the phenomenon of interpersonal violence known as bullying has been the major concern in schools. This phenomenon is characterized by a perverse dynamic in which the roles of bully and victim emerge (Hong and Espelage, 2012; Beltrán-Catalán et al., 2018). The victim is subdued by the bully or bullies in terms of intentional physical and/or psychological damage, repeatedly over time and in an asymmetrical power relation (Olweus, 2013). This abuse is an immoral behavior that breaks the most basic rules of peer reciprocity (Ortega-Ruiz et al., 2012) and makes the victim feel more and more vulnerable and unable to protect him/herself (Sentse et al., 2017). Bullying victimization has serious consequences. For example, at an academic level, a lower performance, the desire of not attending school or even a school dropout could be observed, whereas at a healthy level, anxiety, depression, sleep disturbance, self-harm, suicide attempts or even suicide could appear (Wolke and Lereya, 2015). Bullies and bullying bystanders also suffer the negative effects of this violent phenomenon since they take the risk of internalizing and consolidating aggressive, immoral and little empathic interrelational patterns as well as suffering social imbalances throughout their development (Vanderbilt and Augustyn, 2010; Ortega-Ruiz et al., 2012). Some studies have revealed that there is an overlap between aggression and victimization in bullying; in fact, they even show that they seem to intertwine (e.g., Mishna, 2003; Del Rey et al., 2012).

Insofar as cultural diversity is present in schools, educational centers are concerned about the ethnic-cultural discrimination that threatens and harms the health and development of students from cultural minorities (e.g., Priest et al., 2014; Cooper and Sánchez, 2016; Brüggemann and D’Arcy, 2017). Ethnic-cultural aggressions predict the psychological imbalance of those who suffer them (Benner and Graham, 2013) and act as an educational barrier (Baysu et al., 2016). The co-occurrence of bullying victimization and ethnic-cultural discriminatory victimization makes more predictable the emergence of suicidal ideation than when these phenomena occur separately (Garnett et al., 2014).

In order to prevent and palliate bullying among students, more and more researchers have stated that their predictors and associated factors should be studied in different cultural contexts and within different ethnic-cultural groups (e.g., Garnett et al., 2014; Vera et al., 2017). Several transnational studies on ways of violence in minors have shown consistency in some predictors and inconsistency in others, allowing us to propose strategies adapted to every country (e.g., Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al., 2018). Comparing transnational knowledge may help to fit prevention and intervention to every single context (e.g., Arenas et al., 2015; Mucci et al., 2016; Bartoll et al., 2019). In order to foresee the emergence and consolidation of the dynamics of bullying, it is necessary to know more about the characteristics of personality and the types of interpersonal behaviors that allow us to predict this phenomenon. Hereunder, there is a review about some of the potential bullying predictive factors in adolescents from culturally diverse schools: (1) personal, such as self-esteem, empathy, social skills and gender; (2) and interpersonal, like personal victimization and personal aggression (bullying), or ethnic-cultural victimization and ethnic-cultural aggression (discrimination).

There is a negative association between the levels of victimization and self-esteem in adolescents (Blood et al., 2011; Chen and Wei, 2011; Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al., 2015). Additionally, victimization is negatively linked to social adjustment and to the number of friends in school (Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al., 2014). Those adolescents who suffer bullying victimization take a greater risk of internalizing problems like having a low global self-esteem (Mishna et al., 2016). On the other hand, it has been observed that self-esteem acts as an internal element influencing on the overcoming of victimization experiences (Sapouna and Wolke, 2013), so it can be considered as a protective factor. It seems that there is a cause-effect relation between victimization and self-esteem (Fredstrom et al., 2011).

Tsaousis’ review and meta-analysis (2016) reveal that the existing negative association between aggression and self-esteem is weaker than the one between victimization and self-esteem (Tsaousis, 2016). This researcher underscores that the results from different studies regarding this subject support two hypotheses: low self-esteem acts as a precursor of aggression toward peers; and in adolescents with high self-esteem, they develop aggressive behaviors toward their peers when their self-esteem is threatened by them.

Part of the studies on cognitive empathy and involvement in bullying has found a link between both (e.g., Caravita et al., 2009; Dini et al., 2016; Rieffe and Camodeca, 2016), while another part has not found any relation between them (van Noorden et al., 2015). However, within the studies that have evidenced such connection, the results are inconsistent. In some studies, it is said that cognitive empathy is positively associated with a violent behavior in both genders (Caravita et al., 2009; Rieffe and Camodeca, 2016). However, other studies conclude that cognitive empathy is negatively associated with levels of aggression in bullying (Mitsopoulou and Giovazolias, 2015; Dini et al., 2016). One of the few existing longitudinal studies has revealed that cognitive empathy does not predict involvement as a bully or the other way around (Stavrinides et al., 2010). Nevertheless, there are studies stating that low cognitive empathy and gender—being male—are predictors of an aggressive behavior (Kokkinos and Kipritsi, 2012).

Affective empathy is negatively associated with aggression in bullying (Mitsopoulou and Giovazolias, 2015; van Noorden et al., 2015; Rieffe and Camodeca, 2016), especially more in boys than girls (Caravita et al., 2009). It has been seen that a low affective empathy is a predictor of the involvement as a bully and the other way around (Stavrinides et al., 2010). However, other studies do not show any association between affective empathy and involvement in bullying (Dini et al., 2016).

A negative correlation has been observed between the levels of victimization and cognitive and affective empathy (Kokkinos and Kipritsi, 2012). van Noorden et al.’s systematic review (2015) concludes that victimization is not usually associated with affective empathy, but it is usually negatively associated with cognitive empathy: the victims experience what others feel but they do not understand what they feel.

Adolescents’ assertive responses to different situations in the school context are considered as more effective by peers and teachers, so they are more adaptive (Dirks et al., 2009, 2014). Girls give more assertive responses in comparison to boys when facing challenging situations between peers (Rose and Rudolph, 2006). In adolescents, educational training in assertiveness is seen as a solution for bullying (Boket et al., 2016), since it can improve the coping of social situations, modify the aggressive behavior, improve social skills, and balance the emotional status (Keliat et al., 2015). Several recent studies have revealed that the educational intervention to promote assertive behaviors has a positive effect in: (1) the reduction of victimization (Keliat et al., 2015; Avşar and Ayaz-Alkaya, 2017), and (2) the reduction of aggressive behaviors toward peers in bullying (Ttofi and Farrington, 2011; Schroeder et al., 2012; Keliat et al., 2015; Avşar and Ayaz-Alkaya, 2017).

Despite the fact that many existing educational programs for the prevention of bullying in schools try to promote the development of conflict-resolution skills, a very little percentage of adolescents has shown to have used these skills to deal with bullying situations (Didaskalou et al., 2017). It has been seen that cooperative resolution skills negatively correlate with the emission of aggressive behaviors and the justification and acceptance of a violent behavior; however, these skills positively correlate with empathy toward bullying victims (Garaigordobil, 2012, 2017).

The high belief about their skills to communicate, resolve conflicts, and handle emotions with friends is related to low levels of victimization in adolescents (Fitzpatrick and Bussey, 2014). Bullying victims show less communicative and conflict-resolution skills (Haynie et al., 2001; Kochel et al., 2015). On the other hand, bullies show less communicative skills associated with a successful performance in collaborative tasks than defenders; bullies give less useful explanations and less guidance instructions to their peers than defenders (Murphy and Faulkner, 2011).

In the last two decades, attention has increasingly been paid in studies to the weight of cultural diversity regarding coexistence and violence phenomena among peers. Some studies reveal that the higher the presence of ethnic-cultural diversity is among students, the higher the bullying prevalence will be (Vervoort et al., 2010; Jansen et al., 2016). In those adolescents who belong to cultural minorities, the content of the suffered intimidation is often based on the differences of origin, ethnic group, language or migratory status (Maynard et al., 2016; Brietzke and Perreira, 2017). It has been observed that the minority status within each school is related both to the general or personal victimization and to the particularly racist victimization (Fisher et al., 2015). Being a victim of physical, verbal or relational bullying shows a strong association with being a victim of ethnic-cultural discrimination (Monks et al., 2008; Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al., 2014, 2015; Cardoso et al., 2017). Additionally, it has been seen that being a victim of discrimination between adolescents from different ethnic groups is positively related to being a victim of discrimination between peers from the same ethnic group (Benner and Wang, 2017). Nevertheless, there is a research gap regarding the connection between personal aggression in bullying and ethnic-cultural discriminatory aggression.

In view of the increasing study of bullying concerning ethnic-cultural diversity in the last years, some educational projects addressing discrimination among peers (Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al., 2017; Earnshaw et al., 2018) are being developed. However, it is necessary to have a greater body of knowledge about the nature of the relations between bullying and ethnic-cultural discrimination so that these educational proposals are more effective.

The Present Study

The review of scientific literature enables us to observe that there are hardly any transnational studies about bullying predictors—personal aggression and personal victimization—that has been carried out with a wide sample. Thus, we need to know if they act as bullying predictors as well as to what extent they are stable or different among countries: (1) personal factors such as self-esteem, empathy, social skills, and gender and (2) interpersonal factors such as the levels of involvement in personal victimization (bullying), personal aggression (bullying), ethnic-cultural victimization (discrimination), and/or ethnic-cultural aggression (discrimination). We have carried out a comparative study on students from two different countries: Spain and Ecuador. These two countries have significant cultural links; however, they deal with ethnic-cultural diversity in different ways. Ecuador is multiethnic and multicultural, while Spain is neither multiethnic nor multicultural despite having recognized rights to cultural diversity. The objectives of the present study are:

1. To know what personal and interpersonal factors are predictors of personal victimization.

2. To know what personal and interpersonal factors are predictors of personal aggression.

3. To know which predictive patterns of personal victimization and personal aggression are common in Spain and Ecuador and which ones are unique in each country.

The following hypotheses have been studied:

1. Personal aggression and personal victimization will be predicted by means of social skills, self-esteem, and empathy.

2. Being female will be a positive predictor of personal victimization and a negative predictor of personal aggression.

3. Personal aggression and personal victimization will reciprocally be positive predictors.

Ethnic-cultural aggression and ethnic-cultural victimization will be predictors of personal aggression and personal victimization.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 25,190 participants took part in the study. Two data collections were carried out following the same methodology, one in all Spain and another one in Ecuador. The Spanish sample was obtained in the whole national territory, while the Ecuadorian sample was obtained in zone 4 of Ecuador, composed of the extensive provinces of Manabí and Santo Domingo de Los Tsáchilas. The subjects were selected using a random cluster sampling. From the total sample, 42.7% (10,753 subjects) were from the Ecuadorian collection, while 57.3% (14,437 subjects) were from the Spanish collection. The participants were studying in equivalent years within the two different educational systems. The average age of the sample is 13.92 (SD = 1.306), being the average age of the Spanish sample 14.03 (SD = 1.390) and of the Ecuadorian sample 13.77 (SD = 1.169). Regarding gender, a similar proportion of boys and girls was found in the sample (49.9% were girls). For the Spanish sample, this percentage amounted to 50.7% and for the Ecuadorian sample, it fell to 48.9%.

Instruments

To measure the levels of bullying aggression and victimization among peers, we used the European Bullying Intervention Project Questionnaire—EBIPQ—(Ortega-Ruiz et al., 2016). The questionnaire is composed of 14 items, 7 items regarding victimization and 7 for aggression. Those items have five response options (1 = never; 2 = once or twice; 3 = once or twice a month; 4 = about once a week; 5 = more than once a week). The reliability indexes are high, not only for the Spanish sample (αvictimization-SPA = 0.827; αaggression-SPA = 0.823) but also for the Ecuadorian one (αvictimization-ECU = 0.826; αaggression-ECU = 0.855). The CFA showed optimal levels for the two-factor structure of the instrument (χ2 S-B = 10371.7069, p = 0.00; CFI = 0.97; NNFI = 0.96; RMSEA = 0.067).

An adaptation of the EBIPQ was used to measure involvement in victimization and aggression due to ethnic-cultural discrimination: the European Bullying Intervention Project Questionnaire—Ethnic-Cultural Discrimination Version (EBIPQ-ECD Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al., 2019). The EBIPQ-ECD has a similar structure to the EBIPQ, presenting the same number of items and the same measurement scale. Its psychometric properties in this study are even better than the ones of the EBIPQ for both the Spanish sample (αvictimization-SPA = 0.862; αaggression-SPA = 0.855) and the Ecuadorian sample (αvictimization-ECU = 0.858; αaggression-ECU = 0.873). The CFA showed optimal levels for the two-factor structure of the instrument (χ2 S-B = 12087.4424, p = 0.00; CFI = 0.98; NNFI = 0.97; RMSEA = 0.071).

To address the measurement of self-esteem, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale—RSES—(Rosenberg, 1989) was used, adapted, and validated by Martín-Albo et al. (2007) and Viejo (2014). The scale has widely been used with samples of adolescents. The two-factor model of the scale fits the best for this study, independently considering self-confidence (five items positively formulated) and self-deprecation (five items negatively formulated). The values of the scale of measurement range from 1 to 4. The reliability analyses have shown enough indexes for both the Spanish sample (αself-confidence-SPA = 0.816; αself-deprecation-SPA = 0.808) and the Ecuadorian sample (αself-confidence-ECU = 0.719; αself-deprecation-ECU = 0.645). The CFA showed optimal levels for the two-factor structure of the instrument (χ2 S-B = 2906.1713, p = 0.00; CFI = 0.98; NNFI = 0.97; RMSEA = 0.053).

The Basic Empathy Scale (BES; Jolliffe and Farrington, 2011) was used to measure the participants’ levels of empathy, specifically the Oliva et al.’s adaptation (2011) of nine items, distinguishing between affective empathy (four items) and cognitive empathy (five items). The scale of measurement of each item ranges from 1 (Totally disagree) to 5 (Totally agree). The reliability values for both subscales are very high not only for the Spanish sample (αcognitive_empathy-SPA = 0.867; αaffective_empathy-SPA = 0.825) but also for the Ecuadorian sample (αcognitive_empathy-ECU = 0.901; αaffective_empathy-ECU = 0.833). The CFA showed optimal levels for the two-factor structure of the instrument (χ2 S-B = 4087.9534, p = 0.00; CFI = 0.99; NNFI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.070).

The social skills scale (Oliva et al., 2011) is composed of 12 items with values ranging from 1 (Totally false) to 7 (Totally true). The scale is divided into three sub-dimensions: Communicative or relational skills (five items), assertiveness (three items), and conflict-resolution skills (four items). The scores of the reliability tests are acceptable or good for most subscales in both samples (αcommunicative_social-SPA = 0.774; αassertiveness-SPA = 0.644; αconflict-resolution-SPA = 0.767; αcommunicative_social-ECU = 0.832; αassertiveness-ECU = 0.779; αconflict-resolution-ECU = 0.822). The CFA showed optimal levels for the three-factor structure of the instrument (χ2 S-B = 4704.5658, p = 0.00; CFI = 0.98; NNFI = 0.97; RMSEA = 0.056).

Procedure

We proceeded to obtain the authorization from the respective educational administration and the educational centers in both countries. Written informed parental/guardian consent was obtained by means of the support of educational centers. The students were informed and ensured of the anonymous, confidential, and voluntary nature of their participation in the study. The registered data were codified in a data matrix through the SPSS 22 software. The collection was carried out in Ecuador by means of paper questionnaires, whereas it was done by online questionnaires in Spain. The procedure was approved by the Ethics committee of the University of Córdoba and was conducted following the national and international ethical standards.

Analysis

For this study, multiple linear regressions were used to explore what the predictive variables of aggression and victimization were in traditional bullying. The other variables have been used as predictors with the “Introduce” method for that purpose. The collinearity diagnostics were optimal. The levels of tolerance ranged from 0.34 to 0.94 for all the variables considered in the study. VIF values ranged from 1.06 to 3.48 and Durbin-Watson statistic showed values between 1.797 and 1.947 in all the cases. All the variables included in the regressions were measured in scale, except the gender variable, which was conversed in a dummy variable regarding the gender “girl” (0 = otherwise and 1 = girl).

Results

Overall, four multiple linear regressions were carried out to address the research objectives. In all the models, the following phenomena were included as independent variables: received ethnic-cultural victimization, ethnic-cultural aggression, self-confidence, self-deprecation, affective empathy, cognitive empathy, communication or relationship skills, assertiveness, conflict-resolution skills, and belonging to the “girl” category. This last variable was recoded in a dummy variable as it has already been mentioned. Similarly, victimization was included for the regression predicting aggression and the other way around.

The predictive model for victimization explained nearly 70% of the variance in the Spanish sample. Almost all the variables showed certain influence on the regression model for victimization in Spain, except cognitive empathy and gender. The strongest detected predictor is the score in ethnic-cultural victimization (β = 0.757; p = 0.000), followed by the involvement as traditional bully (β = 0.368; p = 0.000). A high score in ethnic-cultural aggression was found as a remarkable protective factor of this type of victimization (β = −0.298; p = 0.000). As remarkable psychological variables, high levels of self-deprecation predict a higher score in victimization (β = 0.078; p = 0.000), unlike self-confidence (β = −0.043; p = 0.000). See Table 1 for the details of parameter estimates in the analyses.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Multiple linear regression model for personal victimization (Spain).

Regarding the Ecuadorian sample, the predictive model explained over 50% of variance. In such model, the score in victimization was detected to be predicted by means of a high score in ethnic-cultural victimization (β = 0.535; p = 0.000) and a high score in traditional aggression (β = 0.382; p = 0.000). However, high scores in ethnic-cultural aggression predicted low levels of traditional victimization (β = −0.135; p = 0.000). As remarkable psychological variables, self-deprecation (β = 0.035; p = 0.000), cognitive empathy (β = 0.037; p = 0.002), and affective empathy (β = 0.039; p = 0.000) could be mentioned. The variables that in this case did not significantly predict the model were self-confidence, being girl and all the variables related to social skills. See Table 2 for the details of parameter estimates in the analyses for the Ecuadorian sample.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Multiple linear regression model for personal victimization (Ecuador).

Regarding aggression, the model predicted over 70% of variance in the Spanish sample. Only three variables did not make a significant contribution to the model: affective empathy, cognitive empathy, and conflict-resolution skills. The variable with the highest explanatory force of traditional aggression was ethnic-cultural aggression (β = 0.784; p = 0.000), followed by traditional victimization (β = 0.358; p = 0.000). As a negative predictor, ethnic-cultural victimization is emphasized (β = −0.240; p = 0.000). The higher the level of ethnic-cultural victimization is, the lower the level of traditional aggression will be. The psychological variable with the highest influence on the model was communicative skills (β = 0.032; p = 0.000), predicting high levels of traditional aggression. Being a girl was a protection factor (β = −0.036; p = 0.000) in comparison to traditional aggression. See Table 3 for the details of parameter estimates in the analyses.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Multiple linear regression model for personal aggression (Spain).

Regarding the sample collected in Ecuador, the model of traditional aggression explained 54% of variance. In this regression, self-confidence, cognitive empathy, and the three variables related to social skills did not provide a significant effect to the model. The predictor with the greatest proportion in traditional aggression was aggression due to ethnic-cultural reasons (β = 0.535; p = 0.000), followed by the score in traditional victimization (β = 0.356; p = 0.000). The higher the scores of ethnic-cultural victimization are, the lower the levels of traditional aggression are (β = −0.064; p = 0.000). The most important psychological variables in the model were self-deprecation as a positive factor (β = 0.036; p = 0.000) and cognitive empathy as a negative factor (β = −0.034; p = 0.001). Being a girl proved to be a factor negatively related to the score in traditional aggression (β = −0.033; p = 0.000). See Table 4 for the details of parameter estimates in the analyses.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Multiple linear regression model for personal aggression (Ecuador).

Discussion and Conclusions

The predictive models provided for personal victimization and personal aggression (bullying) explain almost three-quarters of the variance in the Spanish sample and half of the variance in the Ecuadorian sample. Within the introduced variables, interpersonal variables showed a greater predictive weight on peer victimization and aggression than personal variables. The weight and presence of predictor factors, not only in bullying victimization but also in bullying aggression, have partially been similar in the samples collected in Spain and Ecuador. Some predictive patterns are common in both countries, but others are unique.

Regarding bullying victimization, the conclusion is that there is a fairly robust transnational predictive pattern: there are three significantly important predictors, which are victimization due to ethnic-cultural discrimination, aggression because of bullying and aggression due to ethnic-cultural discrimination, and two less important predictors that are self-deprecation and affective empathy. Ethnic-cultural victimization is a positive predictor of victimization, being the one with the greatest predictor power among all the factors that we studied. Bullying aggression is a great positive predictor of victimization, whereas ethnic-cultural aggression is also a negative predictor at a similar level. Both self-deprecation and affective empathy act as positive predictors of bullying victimization.

Beyond this homogeneous prediction pattern of bullying victimization detected in both countries, the transnational comparison enables us to emphasize some unique predictors. In the Spanish sample, we observed more predictive lightweight factors of victimization: assertiveness as a positive predictor, self-confidence, communication and relationship, and conflict-resolution as negative predictors. In contrast, we observed cognitive empathy, being a positive predictor, as a predictive lightweight factor in the Ecuadorian sample. Social skills—assertiveness, communication and relationship, conflict-resolution—play a predictor role in bullying victimization in Spain, whereas it does not play it in Ecuador.

Regarding victimization, self-esteem and empathy both have a statistically significant influence on victimization in the Ecuadorian sample; however, social skills do not have this influence. The first hypothesis of the study is therefore partially corroborated. Nevertheless, regarding the Spanish sample, the first hypothesis is corroborated, since the three personal factors that were studied make possible the prediction of victimization. The fact that social skills predict victimization is consistent with the results obtained in some studies (Haynie et al., 2001; Fitzpatrick and Bussey, 2014; Kochel et al., 2015; Jenkins and Fredrick, 2017). However, despite the fact that several studies have revealed that assertiveness acts as a protective factor of victimization (Keliat et al., 2015; Boket et al., 2016), strangely enough, in our study, it has been observed that it acts as a positive predictor of victimization. The fact that low self-esteem predicts high levels of victimization is consistent with some observations from previous studies (Blood et al., 2011; Chen and Wei, 2011; Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al., 2014, 2015). High self-esteem, as Sapouna and Wolke (2013) state, acts as a protective factor of victimization. Affective empathy was expected not to be a predictor of victimization (van Noorden et al., 2015); however, it has been evidenced that affective empathy is a positive predictor of victimization in Spain and in Ecuador. On the other hand, the expectation of cognitive empathy negatively predicting victimization was not met. Unexpectedly, cognitive empathy acts as a positive predictor of victimization in the Ecuadorian sample. These last conclusions are conflicting with the negative correlations found by Kokkinos and Kipritsi (2012) between empathy—affective and cognitive empathy—and victimization.

Regarding bullying aggression, the conclusion is that there is a stable transnational predictive pattern: ethnic-cultural aggression, bullying victimization, and self-deprecation are positive predictors, while ethnic-cultural victimization and being a girl are negative predictors. Ethnic-cultural aggression and bullying victimization are the most powerful predictors in Spain and Ecuador. Nevertheless, ethnic-cultural victimization is also a great predictor in Spain, while it is a less important predictor in Ecuador. Being a girl and self-deprecation are lightweight predictors in both countries. Regarding lightweight predictors of aggression, the conclusion is that there are specificities in the contrast by countries. Communication and relationship skills as well as self-confidence are considered positive predictors in Spain, whereas assertiveness is a negative predictor. Affective empathy is a negative predictor in Ecuador. Just like in the prediction of victimization, social skills again seem to be relevant in Spain, in contrast with the lack of predictive importance they appear to have in Ecuador.

In terms of aggression, self-esteem and empathy make possible the prediction of aggression in the Ecuadorian sample. Additionally, social skills do not contribute in a statistically significant way to the model. Considering the Spanish sample in the aggression model, both social skills and self-esteem have a statistically significant effect whereas empathy does not. This evidence about the two samples regarding aggression enables us to partially confirm the first hypothesis of the study. The observation in the Spanish sample that low assertiveness predicts aggression is aligned with the model of social-skill deficit regarding aggression (Crick and Dodge, 1994; Camodeca and Goossens, 2005); while the observation that high social and relational skills predict aggression seems to support the model of a bully with popularity and leadership while she/he manipulates his/her classmates. This model typically appears in contexts in which the norms and the educational action try to prevent and palliate bullying (Waasdorp et al., 2013; Rivas-Drake et al., 2014; Vera et al., 2017). Apart from that, the expectation of aggression being predicted by means of low levels of self-esteem was met in the Ecuadorian and Spanish samples. High self-deprecation predicted aggression in both countries; however, high self-esteem also predicted aggression only in Spain. These two opposed conclusions seem to support, respectively, two hypotheses issued by Tsaousis (2016) following his extensive review: low self-esteem acts as a precursor of aggression, while high self-esteem is a precursor of aggressive behaviors toward peers in response to the peers’ threat. As expected, the levels of cognitive empathy do not predict aggression in any of the countries of the study. It has also been evidenced that low affective empathy predicts aggressive behaviors in Ecuador. These results are consistent with the conclusions drawn by one of the few longitudinal studies carried out in that respect (Stavrinides et al., 2010). On the other hand, these results are inconsistent with the conclusions drawn by the systematical review carried out by van Noorden et al. (2015).

The second hypothesis of the study was partially corroborated. Contrary to what was expected, being a girl does not predict a greater likelihood to be victimized in any of the samples from Spain or Ecuador. But it was observed that being a girl predicted a lower involvement in bullying aggressions, which is in line with the conclusion of Kokkinos and Kipritsi’s (2012) study. This is consistent with the fact that female adolescents develop more assertive behaviors than male adolescents in difficult situations among peers (Rose and Rudolph, 2006). An aspect that we should remark in the Ecuadorian sample is that low affective empathy and being a boy are factors that increase the likelihood of being a bully, which is in line with the relation between these aspects as Caravita et al. (2009) stated.

The conclusion is therefore that aggression and victimization among adolescents are predicted reciprocally. This confirms the third initial hypothesis stated following the evidence of the existing overlap between the roles of bully and bullying victim in most of the people involved in this violent phenomenon (e.g., Mishna, 2003; Del Rey et al., 2012).

The results confirm the fourth hypothesis: ethnic-cultural victimization and aggression are predictors of bullying aggression and victimization. The evidence of the important predictive force of involvement in ethnic-cultural victimization over involvement in bullying victimization is a great step forward regarding the observations provided by some previous studies that had already emphasized the existence of some type of relation between both (Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al., 2014, 2015; Cardoso et al., 2017). Victimization can be predicted in terms of involvement in ethnic-cultural aggression. Bullying aggression is predictable depending on involvement in ethnic-cultural victimization and/or ethnic-cultural aggression. From these last findings, no precedents have been found in the scientific literature. It is interesting that ethnic-cultural aggression is a negative predictor of bullying victimization, and ethnic-cultural victimization is a negative predictor of bullying aggression in both samples. As a possible explanation for this, taking into account that the power imbalance between bully and victim plays a key role in the dynamics of bullying (Ortega-Ruiz et al., 2012; Olweus, 2013; Sentse et al., 2017), it is proposed that a significant proportion of this power imbalance between peers in pluricultural contexts is related to the difference of ethnic-cultural status. This ethnic-cultural status could be expressed and built at a large extent between peers depending on the emission and reception of ethnic-cultural discriminatory behaviors. This way, those who attack others discriminatorily could be more feared and less subject to bullying victimization at the same time. Those who suffer discriminatory victimization could be self-perceived and perceived by others with little or no power over their peers, what would confer them a disadvantageous situation to emit aggressive behaviors of bullying toward others.

The development of the research has considered the use of robust and reliable instruments over wide samples in Spain and Ecuador. Educational inferences are set out on the provided conclusions and discussion in order to improve the prevention of bullying, the limitations of the study, and the future lines of research.

A great part of the educational interventions based on the scientific evidence to prevent and palliate bullying has considered the educational work to build up self-esteem and to develop empathy and/or social skills (Ttofi and Farrington, 2011; Boket et al., 2016; Avşar and Ayaz-Alkaya, 2017; Didaskalou et al., 2017; Lösel and Ttofi, 2017). The conclusions of the study contribute to support this line of proposals. Nevertheless, the greatest part of the interventions has disregarded dealing with violent and discriminatory phenomena based on cultural differences (Earnshaw et al., 2018). The study proposes that educational policies and interventions take into account the approach of ethnic-cultural discrimination to prevent bullying, due to its high predictive value on this phenomenon. In the educational centers, the processing of contents and the development of educational strategies are necessary to promote the intercultural coexistence and the eradication of ethnic-cultural discrimination. Recently, new educational proposals against bullying are being developed, paying particular attention to the ethnic-cultural diversity and to the risk of discrimination and exclusion. An example of this is the Educational Model of Intercultural Coexistence—MECI in Spanish—(Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al., 2017). This work proposal is consistent with a budding but emerging line of bullying preventive and palliative psychoeducational models based on the stigma for having one or several socially devalued characteristics (Earnshaw et al., 2018).

Some of the limitations of the study are inherent to the type of methodology and instruments used in the research. As it is a cross-sectional study, the results are aimed toward the prediction and not toward the inference of causal relations between variables. The use of the self-report questionnaire enables us to collect data of self-perception, but not about the hetero-perception among peers. Starting from the results provided about the prediction of bullying aggression and victimization among adolescents, it would be convenient to develop a longitudinal study considering the same variables in the future. This would make possible to know more about the potential causal relations. Additionally, it would be interesting to take measures not only about self-perception but also about hetero-perception among peers. It would be convenient to start a line of studies on bullying and discrimination among adolescents taking into account socioeconomical indicators, as it is being done regarding other violent and/or psychological phenomena in adults (e.g., Mucci et al., 2016; Bartoll et al., 2019). These guidelines would allow us to advance in the study of bullying and ethnic-cultural discrimination as both are ecological and dynamic in the peer network.

Data Availability

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

All authors made substantial contribution to the theoretical framework, design, data collection or interpretation of this study. All contributed to this article and approved its publication.

Funding

This work was supported by the Save the Children® under the project Bullying, Cyberbullying and Associated Factors (contract with the Foundation of the Autonomous University of Madrid; Code: 056900), and the Spanish National I+D+I Plan under project PSI2016-74871-R.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the children, parents, and school administrators for their participation and also want to thank the NGO Save the Children for their financial support and help in collecting data.

References

Arenas, A., Giorgi, G., Montani, F., Mancuso, S., Perez, J. F., Mucci, N., et al. (2015). Workplace bullying in a sample of Italian and Spanish employees and its relationship with job satisfaction, and psychological well-being. Front. Psychol. 15:1912. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avşar, F., and Ayaz-Alkaya, S. (2017). The effectiveness of assertiveness training for school-aged children on bullying and assertiveness level. J. Pediatr. Nurs. 36, 186–190. doi: 10.1016/j.pedn.2017.06.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartoll, X., Gotsens, M., Marí-Dell’Olmo, M., Palència, L., Calvo, M., Esnaola, S., et al. (2019). Stable socioeconomic inequalities in ischaemic heart disease mortality during the economic crisis: a time trend analysis in 2 Spanish settings. Arch. Public Health 77:12. doi: 10.1186/s13690-019-0339-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baysu, G., Celeste, L., Brown, R., Verschueren, K., and Phalet, K. (2016). Minority adolescents in ethnically diverse schools: perceptions of equal treatment buffer threat effects. Child Dev. 87, 1352–1366. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beltrán-Catalán, M., Zych, I., Ortega-Ruiz, R., and Llorent, V. J. (2018). Victimisation through bullying and cyberbullying: emotional intelligence, severity of victimisation and technology use in different types of victims. Psicothema 30, 183–188. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2017.313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benner, A. D., and Graham, S. (2013). The antecedents and consequences of racial/ethnic discrimination during adolescence: does the source of discrimination matter? Dev. Psychol. 49, 1602–1613. doi: 10.1037/a0030557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benner, A. D., and Wang, Y. (2017). Racial/ethnic discrimination and adolescents’ well-being: the role of cross-ethnic friendships and friends’ experiences of discrimination. Child Dev. 88, 493–504. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blood, G. W., Blood, I. M., Tramontana, G. M., Sylvia, A. J., Boyle, M. P., and Motzko, G. R. (2011). Self-reported experience of bullying of students who stutter: relations with life satisfaction, life orientation, and self-esteem. Percept. Mot. Skills 113, 353–364. doi: 10.2466/07.10.15.17.PMS.113.5.353-364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boket, E. G., Bahrami, M., Kolyaie, L., and Hosseini, S. A. (2016). The effect of assertiveness skills training on reduction of emotional victimization of female high school students. Int. J. Humanit. Cult. Stud. 9, 690–699. doi: 10.3126/ijls.v9i4.12679

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brietzke, M., and Perreira, K. (2017). Stress and coping: Latino youth coming of age in a New Latino destination. J. Adolesc. Res. 32, 407–432. doi: 10.1177/0743558416637915

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brüggemann, C., and D’Arcy, K. (2017). Contexts that discriminate: international perspectives on the education of Roma students. Race Ethn. Educ. 20, 575–578. doi: 10.1080/13613324.2016.1191741

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camodeca, M., and Goossens, F. A. (2005). Aggression, social cognitions, anger and sadness in bullies and victims. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 186–197. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00347.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caravita, S. C. S., Di Blasio, P., and Salmivalli, C. (2009). Unique and interactive effects of empathy and social status on involvement in bullying. Soc. Dev. 18, 140–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00465.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardoso, J. B., Szlyk, H. S., Goldbach, J., Swank, P., and Zvolensky, M. J. (2017). General and ethnic-biased bullying among latino students: exploring risks of depression, suicidal ideation, and substance use. J. Immigr. Minor. Health 20, 816–822. doi: 10.1007/s10903-017-0593-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J.-K., and Wei, H.-S. (2011). The impact of school violence on self-esteem and depression among Taiwanese junior high school students. Soc. Indic. Res. 100, 479–498. doi: 10.1007/s11205-010-9625-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, A. C., and Sánchez, B. (2016). The roles of racial discrimination, cultural mistrust, and gender in Latina/o youth’s school attitudes and academic achievement. J. Res. Adolesc. 26, 1036–1047. doi: 10.1111/jora.12263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crick, N. R., and Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychol. Bull. 115, 74–101. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Rey, R., Elipe, P., and Ortega, R. (2012). Bullying and cyberbullying: overlapping and predictive value of the co-occurrence. Psicothema 24, 608–613. http://www.psicothema.com/psicothema.asp?id=4061.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Didaskalou, E., Skrzypiec, G., Andreou, E., and Slee, P. (2017). Taking action against victimisation: Australian middle school students’ experiences. J. Psychol. Couns. Sch. 27, 105–122. doi: 10.1017/jgc.2016.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dini, B., Sad, N., Milovanovi, I., and Sad, N. (2016). Empathy and peer violence among adolescents: moderation effect of gender empathy and peer violence among adolescents: moderation effect of gender. Sch. Psychol. Int. 37, 359–377. doi: 10.1177/0143034316649008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dirks, M. A., Suor, J. H., Rusch, D., and Frazier, S. L. (2014). Children’s responses to hypothetical provocation by peers: coordination of assertive and aggressive strategies. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 42, 1077–1087. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9862-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dirks, M. A., Treat, T. A., and Weersing, V. R. (2009). The judge specificity of evaluations of youth social behavior: the case of peer provocation. Soc. Dev. 19, 736–757. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00559.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Earnshaw, V. A., Reisner, S. L., Menino, D. D., Poteat, V. P., Bogart, L. M., Barnes, T. N., et al. (2018). Stigma-based bullying interventions: a systematic review. Dev. Rev. 48, 178–200. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2018.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, J. S., and Roeser, R. W. (2011). Schools as developmental contexts during adolescence. J. Res. Adolesc. 21, 225–241. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00725.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, S., Middleton, K., Ricks, E., Malone, C., Briggs, C., and Barnes, J. (2015). Not just black and white: peer victimization and the intersectionality of school diversity and race. J. Youth Adolesc. 44, 1241–1250. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0243-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, S., and Bussey, K. (2014). The role of perceived friendship self-efficacy as a protective factor against the negative effects of social victimization. Soc. Dev. 23, 41–60. doi: 10.1111/sode.12032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredstrom, B. K., Adams, R. E., and Gilman, R. (2011). Electronic and school-based victimization: unique contexts for adjustment difficulties during adolescence. J. Youth Adolesc. 40, 405–415. doi: 10.1007/s10964-010-9569-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garaigordobil, M. (2012). Cooperative conflict-solving during adolescence: relationships with cognitive-behavioural and predictor variables. Infanc. Aprendiz. 35, 151–165. doi: 10.1174/021037012800217998

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garaigordobil, M. (2017). Conducta antisocial: conexión con bullying/cyberbullying y estrategias de resolución de conflictos. Psychosoc. Interv. 26, 47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.psi.2015.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garnett, B. R., Masyn, K. E., Austin, S. B., Miller, M., Williams, D. R., and Viswanath, K. (2014). The intersectionality of discrimination attributes and bullying among youth: an applied latent class analysis. J. Youth Adolesc. 43, 1225–1239. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-0073-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haynie, D. L., Nansel, T., and Eitel, P. (2001). Bullies, victims, and bully/victims: distinct groups of at-risk youth. J. Early Adolesc. 21, 29–49. doi: 10.1177/0272431601021001002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, J. S., and Espelage, D. L. (2012). A review of research on bullying and peer victimization in school: an ecological system analysis. Aggress. Violent Behav. 17, 311–322. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2012.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hull, G. A., and Hellmich, E. A. (2018). Locating the global: schooling in an interconnected world. Teach. Coll. Rec. 120, 1–36. https://www.tcrecord.org

Google Scholar

Jansen, P. W., Mieloo, C. L., and Berkel, A. D. (2016). Bullying and victimization among young elementary school children: the role of child ethnicity and ethnic school composition. Race Soc. Probl. 8, 271–280. doi: 10.1007/s12552-016-9182-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkins, L. N., and Fredrick, S. S. (2017). Social capital and bystander behavior in bullying: internalizing problems as a barrier to prosocial intervention. J. Youth Adolesc. 46, 757–771. doi: 10.1007/s10964-017-0637-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jolliffe, D., and Farrington, D. P. (2011). Is low empathy related to bullying after controlling for individual and social background variables? J. Adolesc. 34, 59–71. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kastoryano, R. (2018). Multiculturalism and interculturalism: redefining nationhood and solidarity. Comp. Migr. Stud. 6:17. doi: 10.1186/s40878-018-0082-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keliat, P. B. A., Tololiu, T. A., Daulima, D. N. H. C., and Erawati, E. (2015). Effectiveness assertive training of bullying prevention among adolescents in West Java Indonesia. Int. J. Nurs. 2, 128–134. doi: 10.15640/ijn.v2n1a14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kochel, K. P., Ladd, G. W., Bagwell, C. L., and Yabko, B. A. (2015). Bully/victim profiles’ differential risk for worsening peer acceptance: the role of friendship. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 41, 38–45. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2015.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokkinos, C. M. M., and Kipritsi, E. (2012). The relationship between bullying, victimization, trait emotional intelligence, self-efficacy and empathy among preadolescents. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 15, 41–58. doi: 10.1007/s11218-011-9168-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lösel, F., and Ttofi, M. M. (2017). Systematic reviews of the effectiveness of developmental prevention programs in reducing delinquency, aggression, and bullying. Aggress. Violent Behav. 33, 91–106. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2016.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marks, A. K., Ejesi, K., and García Coll, C. (2014). Understanding the U.S. immigrant paradox in childhood and adolescence. Child Dev. Perspect. 8, 59–64. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martín-Albo, J., Núñez, J. L., Navarro, J. G., and Grijalvo, F. (2007). The Rosenberg self-esteem scale: translation and validation in university students. Span. J. Psychol. 10, 458–467. doi: 10.1017/S1138741600006727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maynard, B. R., Vaughn, M. G., Salas-Wright, C. P., and Vaughn, S. (2016). Bullying victimization among school-aged immigrant youth in the United States. J. Adolesc. Health 58, 337–344. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mishna, F. (2003). Peer victimization: the case for social work intervention. Fam. Soc. 84, 513–522. doi: 10.1606/1044-3894.142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mishna, F., Khoury-Kassabri, M., Schwan, K., Wiener, J., Craig, W., Beran, T., et al. (2016). The contribution of social support to children and adolescents’ self-perception: the mediating role of bullying victimization. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 63, 120–127. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2016.02.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitsopoulou, E., and Giovazolias, T. (2015). Personality traits, empathy and bullying behavior: a meta-analytic approach. Aggress. Violent Behav. 21, 61–72. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2015.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monks, C. P., Ortega-Ruiz, R., and Rodríguez-Hidalgo, A. J. (2008). Peer victimization in multicultural schools in Spain and England. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 5, 507–535. doi: 10.1080/17405620701307316

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mucci, N., Giorgi, G., Roncaioli, M., Fiz Perez, J., and Arcangeli, G. (2016). The correlation between stress and economic crisis: a systematic review. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 12, 983–993. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S98525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, S., and Faulkner, D. (2011). The relationship between bullying roles and children’s everyday dyadic interactions. Soc. Dev. 20, 272–293. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00597.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliva, A., Antolín Suárez, L., Pertegal Vega, M. Á., Ríos Bermúdez, M., Parra Jiménez, Á., Hernando Gómez, Á., et al. (2011). Instrumentos para la evaluación de la salud mental y el desarrollo positivo adolescente y los activos que lo promueven. (Seville: Junta de Andalucía. Consejería de Salud). Available at: http://www.formajoven.org/AdminFJ/doc_recursos/201241812465364.pdf

Google Scholar

Olweus, D. (2013). School bullying: development and some important challenges. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 9, 751–780. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortega-Ruiz, R., Del Rey, R., and Casas, J. A. (2016). Evaluar el bullying y el cyberbullying validación española del EBIP-Q y del ECIP-Q. Psicol. Educ. 22, 71–79. doi: 10.1016/j.pse.2016.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortega-Ruiz, R., Elipe, P., Mora-Merchán, J. A., Genta, M. L., Brighi, A., Guarini, A., et al. (2012). The emotional impact of bullying and cyberbullying on victims: a European cross-national study. Aggress. Behav. 38, 342–356. doi: 10.1002/ab.21440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Priest, N., Perry, R., Ferdinand, A., Paradies, Y., and Kelaher, M. (2014). Experiences of racism, racial/ethnic attitudes, motivated fairness and mental health outcomes among primary and secondary school students. J. Youth Adolesc. 43, 1672–1687. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0140-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rieffe, C., and Camodeca, M. (2016). Empathy in adolescence: relations with emotion awareness and social roles. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 34, 340–353. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivas-Drake, D., Syed, M., Umaña-Taylor, A., Markstrom, C., French, S., Schwartz, S. J., et al. (2014). Feeling good, happy, and proud: a meta-analysis of positive ethnic-racial affect and adjustment. Child Dev. 85, 77–102. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Hidalgo, A. J., Calmaestra, J., Casas, J. A., and Ortega-Ruiz, R. (2019). Ethnic-cultural bullying versus personal bullying: specificity and measurement of discriminatory aggression and victimization among adolescents. Front. Psychol. 10:46. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Hidalgo, A. J., Pantaleón, Y., and Alcívar, A. (2017). “La discriminación y la exclusión social: Tejer la convivencia intercultural” in Acoso escolar, ciberacoso y discriminación: Educar en diversidad y convivencia. eds. A. J. Rodríguez-Hidalgo and R. Ortega-Ruiz (Madrid: Los Libros de la Cararata), 81–95.

Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Hidalgo, A. J., Ortega-Ruiz, R., and Monks, C. P. (2015). Peer-victimisation in multi-cultural contexts: a structural model of the effects on self-esteem and emotions. Psicol. Educ. 21, 3–9. doi: 10.1016/j.pse.2015.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Hidalgo, A. J., Ortega-Ruiz, R., and Zych, I. (2014). Peer victimization and ethnic-cultural peer victimization: self-esteem and school relations between different cultural groups of students in Andalusia, Spain. J. Psychodidactics 19, 191–210. doi: 10.1387/RevPsicodidact.7909

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Hidalgo, A. J., Solera, E., and Calmaestra, J. (2018). Psychological predictors of cyberbullying according to ethnic-cultural origin in adolescents: a national study in Spain. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 49, 1506–1522. doi: 10.1177/0022022118795283

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, A. J., and Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychol. Bull. 132, 98–131. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.98

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, M. (1989). Society and the adolescent self-image. Rev. edn. (Middeltown, CT: Wesleyan University Press).

Google Scholar

Salmivalli, C. (2010). Bullying and the peer group: a review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 15, 112–120. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2009.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sapouna, M., and Wolke, D. (2013). Resilience to bullying victimization: the role of individual, family and peer characteristics. Child Abuse Negl. 37, 997–1006. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schroeder, B. A., Messina, A., Schroeder, D., Good, K., Barto, S., Saylor, J., et al. (2012). The implementation of a statewide bullying prevention program. Health Promot. Pract. 13, 489–495. doi: 10.1177/1524839910386887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sentse, M., Prinzie, P., and Salmivalli, C. (2017). Testing the direction of longitudinal paths between victimization, peer rejection, and different types of internalizing problems in adolescence. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 45, 1013–1023. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0216-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stavrinides, P., Georgiou, S., and Theofanous, V. (2010). Bullying and empathy: a short-term longitudinal investigation. Educ. Psychol. 30, 793–802. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2010.506004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsaousis, I. (2016). The relationship of self-esteem to bullying perpetration and peer victimization among schoolchildren and adolescents: a meta-analytic review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 31, 186–199. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2016.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ttofi, M. M., and Farrington, D. P. P. (2011). Effectiveness of school-based programs to reduce bullying: a systematic and meta-analytic review. J. Exp. Criminol. 7, 27–56. doi: 10.1007/s11292-010-9109-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Noorden, T. H. J., Haselager, G. J. T., Cillessen, A. H. N., and Bukowski, W. M. (2015). Empathy and involvement in bullying in children and adolescents: a systematic review. J. Youth Adolesc. 44, 637–657. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0135-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderbilt, D., and Augustyn, M. (2010). The effects of bullying. Paediatr. Child Health 20, 315–320. doi: 10.1016/j.paed.2010.03.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vera, E. M., Daskalova, P., Hill, L., Floro, M., Anderson, B., Roche, M., et al. (2017). Parental messages, school belonging, social skills, and personal control as predictors of bullying in ethnic minority adolescents. Sch. Ment. Heal. 9, 347–359. doi: 10.1007/s12310-017-9218-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vervoort, M. H. M., Scholte, R. H. J., and Overbeek, G. (2010). Bullying and victimization among adolescents: the role of ethnicity and ethnic composition of school class. J. Youth Adolesc. 39, 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9355-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viejo, C. (2014). ‘Dating violence’ y cortejo adolescente Un estudio sobre la violencia en las parejas sentimentales de los jóvenes andaluces. (Sevilla: Fundación Pública Andaluza Centro de Estudios Andaluces, Consejería de la Presidencia, Junta de Andalucía).

Google Scholar

Waasdorp, T. E., Baker, C. N., Paskewich, B. S., and Leff, S. S. (2013). The association between forms of aggression, leadership, and social status among urban youth. J. Youth Adolesc. 42, 263–274. doi: 10.1007/s10964-012-9837-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolke, D., and Lereya, S. T. (2015). Long-term effects of bullying. Arch. Dis. Child. 100, 879–885. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2014-306667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: bullying, ethnic-cultural discrimination, social skills, empathy, self-esteem

Citation: Rodríguez-Hidalgo AJ, Pantaleón Y and Calmaestra J (2019) Psychological Predictors of Bullying in Adolescents From Pluricultural Schools: A Transnational Study in Spain and Ecuador. Front. Psychol. 10:1383. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01383

Received: 18 February 2019; Accepted: 28 May 2019;
Published: 19 June 2019.

Edited by:

Rita Zukauskiene, Mykolas Romeris University, Lithuania

Reviewed by:

Evangelia Karagiannopoulou, University of Ioannina, Greece
Giulio Arcangeli, University of Florence, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Rodríguez-Hidalgo, Pantaleón and Calmaestra. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Juan Calmaestra, juan.calmaestra@uco.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.