Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 06 September 2019
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Bridging the Gap between Research and Policy in Fostering Social and Emotional Skills View all 8 articles

Relationship Between Child Perfectionism and Psychological Disorders

  • 1Department of Methodology of Behavioral Sciences, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
  • 2Mind, Brain and Behavioral Research Center, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
  • 3Department of Social Psychology, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
  • 4Department of Psychology, University of Oviedo, Oviedo, Spain
  • 5Independent Researcher, Oviedo, Spain

Objective: Perfectionism is one of the variables related to the correct emotional development or with the appearance of clinical symptomatology in childhood. A study has been designed to evaluate the differential effect that each dimension of perfectionism (external pressure, self-exigency, and negative self-evaluation) has in a Spanish children sample of general population for each of the following clinical aspects: irritability, worthlessness feelings, thinking problems, and psychophysiological symptoms.

Method: By a random cluster sampling, a total of 2,636 children from 8 to 12 years (M = 9.9, SD = 1.2; 51.3% boys) took part in this research. A serial multiple mediators model was used to check the relation between external pressure over the clinical symptoms through self-exigency and negative-self-evaluation.

Results: The results have shown a predictive effect of external pressure over a great variety of clinical symptomatology (irritability, worthlessness, thinking problems, and psychophysiological symptoms), a relation mediated by self-exigency and negative self-evaluation. These relations suggest that external pressure and negative self-evaluation are maladaptive dimensions as they predict the appearance of symptomatology, being the level of self-exigency a protective dimension and favoring the child’s positive development.

Conclusions: In consequence, these results point to the importance of the study of these variables that can generate difficulties in childhood in order to improve children’s quality of life and their correct development.

Perfectionism is a construct that has received growing attention during the last decade (Ayearst et al., 2012; Lloyd et al., 2014; Sherry et al., 2014; Hong et al., 2016; Gäde et al., 2017; Schmidt et al., 2018; Bouguettaya et al., 2019; Curran and Hill, 2019). It is defined as a disposition of the personality that is characterized by the search of faultlessness and the establishment of very high levels of performance, together with excessively critical self-evaluations (Frost et al., 1990; Hewitt and Flett, 1991). The perfectionist person self-assesses herself or himself not only through the achievement or success that is obtained after the implementation of the task but also, and as a very relevant factor, through others’ acceptance and approval (Flett and Hewitt, 2002; DiBartolo and Varner, 2012). Intrapersonal as well as interpersonal aspects are fundamental in order to understand perfectionism in its full extent (Hewitt et al., 2003).

With the aim to evaluate this construct, Frost et al. (1990) developed the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (FMPS) that includes the following dimensions: concern over mistakes, personal standards, parental expectations, parental criticism, and doubts about actions and organization; even though a high level of personal standards is considered to be the central element of perfectionism. Besides, Hewitt and Flett (1991) developed the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (HMPS) assuming three dimensions: self-oriented perfectionism, perfectionism oriented to others, and socially prescribed perfectionism.

The variety of dimensions that the instruments show is more apparent than real (Enns and Cox, 2002). Empirically, the different dimensions that have been proposed are supported by two great underlying dimensions: a positive dimension (or perfectionistic strivings), formed by the levels of personal standards, organization, self-oriented perfectionism, and perfectionism oriented to others; and a negative dimension (or perfectionistic concern) formed by concern over mistakes, doubts about actions, parental criticism, parental expectations, and socially prescribed perfectionism (Frost et al., 1993; Stöeber and Otto, 2006).

The majority of research into how the dimensions of perfectionism affect the psychological field has used adolescent and adult samples (Essau et al., 2008; Flett et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2014). This, produce a lack in the research about children, either about how the perfectionism dimensions influence the emotional disorders (e.g., Hewitt et al., 2002; Rice et al., 2007; Flett and Hewitt, 2012), or in the cognitive variables (DiBartolo and Varner, 2012).

From the multidimensional conception that this study assumes (Lozano et al., 2012), it is considered that the positive dimension of child perfectionism is formed by Self-Exigency (SE), and the negative one by External Pressure (EP), and Negative Self Evaluation (NSE). According to Bandura’s theory (1986), perfectionist traits will be developed through interactions between a child’s characteristics and his/her social environment (primarily family and school). This environment exerts perfectionist pressures (EP in the model) via social expectations of perfection and criticism when these expectations are not met. In these conditions, children put high standards of excellence on themselves (SE, according to the model) due to adults’ behavioral models and the selective reinforcement that these models exert when children achieve levels of excellence (Cole et al., 2001). When there are differences between a child’s task implementation level and his/her standards (Choy and McInerney, 2006) or when the family or school environment is judgmental, unpredictable or hostile (Herman and Ostrander, 2007), the child may develop NSE. Thus, perfectionism pressures of the external environment favor the appearance of other aspects of perfectionism (Morris and Lomax, 2014).

Studies about how the different dimensions of perfectionism affect the psychological balance of children show that higher sensitivity to making mistakes is associated with a decline in the levels of happiness and satisfaction and with a higher emotional instability (Rice and Preusser, 2002; Loades et al., 2019). Self-oriented perfectionism (SE in this study) and socially prescribed perfectionism (EP in this study) are also associated with high levels of anxiety and depression (Hewitt et al., 2002; Stornelli et al., 2009). However, other studies point that EP is positive and significantly associated with high levels of anxiety and depression, but SE is protective against emotional disorders when there is acceptance of the error (Lozano et al., 2015).

Due to the lack of studies about the effect that perfectionism has on child emotional aspects, this study attempts to analyze the relationship between perfectionism and child symptomatology that may hinder a balanced development, such as irritability, feelings of worthlessness, thinking problems, and psychophysiological symptoms. Thus, in line with adolescent and adult population situation (Stöeber and Otto, 2006), it is hypothesized that an increase in the dimensions that shape the negative field of child perfectionism (EP and NSE) will, therefore, favor an increase in the symptomatology of irritability, feelings of worthlessness, thinking problems, and psychophysiological symptoms. In contrast, it is expected that the positive field of perfectionism (SE) will be a protective element against the symptomatology that is studied.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A random cluster sample by the primary education schools of the Principality of Asturias (Spain) was performed. The sample consisted of 2,636 children between the ages of 8 and 12 years (M = 9.9, SD = 1.2). 51.3% were boys; 22.8% were in third grade, 25.4% were in fourth, 26.2% were in fifth, and 25.6% in sixth grade.

The cases where missing values were observed in the answers to the different questionnaires have been deleted (Fernández-Alonso et al., 2012). Table 1 shows the descriptives of the sample for the study of the different clinical variables in general and by grade, together with the maximum error made in the estimations.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the clinical variables and maximum error of estimate.

Instruments

The following assessment instruments have been applied:

The Childhood Perfectionism Inventory (IPI, in Spanish; Lozano et al., 2012). This questionnaire of 25 items evaluates the following dimensions: external pressure (EP; α = 0.90): The child perceives that her or his closest environment demands perfect behavior of her or him (e.g., “I must do things better than anyone else for others to value me”). Self-exigency (SE; α = 0.82): assesses the perfectionist attitude with which the child faces tasks (e.g., “I try to be the best in everything I do”). Negative self-evaluation (NSE; α = 0.90): evaluates the presence of negative self-judgments when the execution is not as excellent as it is wanted (e.g., “When I do not do things as well as I want, I feel like I am good for nothing”).

The Educational-Clinical Questionnaire (CECAD, in Spanish; Lozano et al., 2011). It assesses the following clinical aspects: worthlessness (α = 0.91): The perception that every child has of his or her worth and capacity to face daily tasks (e.g., “I think I am good for nothing”). Irritability (α = 0.87): evaluates the perceived capacity to get angry and the inner feeling of anger in everyday situations (e.g., “Anything irritates me very much”). Thinking problems (α = 0.83): It is valued if the person feels inundated by intrusive thoughts; if there is a tendency to value the things from the most negative perspective and if there is fear to lose control over the things that may happen (e.g., “I tend to think the worst”). Psychophysiological symptoms (α = 0.88): assess the level of physiological activation that accompanies anxiety: breathing problems, difficulties to maintain sleep, shaky hands, and palpitations (e.g., “I feel a pressure in my chest that leaves me out of breath”).

All the questionnaires have a format of 5-point Likert scale as this is the number that maximizes their psychometric properties (Lozano et al., 2008). These questionnaires have no reversed items in order to avoid any possible bias due to the reading skills of the sample (Suárez-Álvarez et al., 2018). In all dimensions, a high score is associated with a greater level in the variable.

Procedure

The different questionnaires were applied by two psychologists who are experts in the use of questionnaires with children. The questionnaires were provided in a single booklet, which was given to children in the classroom where they attend class without ever exceeding 1 h. In a complementary way, data about sociodemographic aspects were also collected. In all the cases, the questionnaires were filled anonymously, and participation in the study was entirely voluntary.

Before administering the questionnaires, consent from all the children’s parents was requested to allow the children to participate in the research.

Data Analysis

To test the aforementioned hypotheses, a serial multiple mediators model was used, as represented in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of the proposed model with two serial mediators. Note: EP, External Pressure; SE, Self Exigency; NSE, Negative Self-Evaluation; CV, Clinical Variables (Worthlessness, Irritability, Thinking problems, and Psychophysiological symptoms).

These models attempt to predict the symptomatology score of the clinical aspects that are studied by the assessment of one direct (c′) and three indirect effects (I1: EP → SE → Clinical Variables [a1b1]; I2: EP → SE → NSE → Clinical Variables [a1d21b2]; I3: EP → NSE → Clinical Variables [a2b2]). The sum of the direct effect with the three indirect effects is the total effect, which is represented as “c” in Figure 1.

Given that the proposed model assumes a linear relation between two mediators, the partial correlation between both mediators was calculated while controlling the effect of EP. Therefore, if this correlation were significant (CL = 95%), the use of this model would be justified, as both mediators were related even after adjusting for the effect of EP (Hayes, 2013).

Using the PROCESS software (Hayes, 2013) implemented on SPSS 20.0, 95% bootstrap bias-corrected confidence intervals (BCI) were generated for the direct and conditional effects on the basis of 10,000 bootstrap samples. Effects are statistically significant when 0 is not included in the bootstrap interval. Following Cumming’s recommendations (Cumming, 2014), intervals for all the estimated parameters are reported.

Results

First, the partial correlation between the mediators was calculated while controlling for the effect of EP. The value that was obtained was rSE.NSE.EP = 0.292, 95% CI [0.257, −0.327], p < 0.001. Given that the partial correlation differed from 0, the use of a model with serial mediators is justified.

In Tables 25 below, the values of the coefficient of regression are shown together with the total and indirect effects that EP has on the different clinical variables that are studied (worthlessness, irritability, thinking problems, and psychophysiological symptoms).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Estimates of worthlessness mediation.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Estimates of irritability mediation.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Estimates of thinking problems mediation.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Estimates for psychophysiological symptoms mediation.

As can be seen in the tables (Tables 25), all the coefficients (from a1 to b2) are statistically significant, so there is a relation between the predicting variable and what it predicts, except for the relation of SE with irritability and thinking problems (coefficient b1 in Tables 3, 4, respectively).

When examining the effect of each unhealthy perfectionism variable over each of the clinical aspects, it was observed that the relations of the EP as well as of the NSE favor the appearance of the symptoms since the value of the coefficients is positive. In the same line, it can be observed that the total effect of EP over each of the clinical variables is significantly positive. This value consists of the direct effect of EP (c′) as well as the sum of all the indirect effects in the model.

As almost all the indirect effects are statistically significant, these results represent evidence in favor of the mediation effect of SE and NSE over the effect of EP over the clinical variables.

It is also observed that the indirect effects EP → SE → NSE → Clinical Variable and EP → NSE → Clinical Variable are positive, which means that they provoke an increase in the clinical symptomatology.

Similarly, in relation with the second hypothesis, it is observed that the effect of SE over worthlessness (b1 = −0.129; 95% CI [−0.160, −0.099]) and psychophysiological symptoms (b1 = −0.099; 95% CI [−0.157, −0.041]) are negative, the same as the indirect effect EP → SE → Clinical Variable. The different coefficients show that the higher the EP, the higher SE is (given that a1 is positive), and this increase in SE is associated with a decrease (given that b1 is negative) of the clinical symptomatology which points to the fact that it behaves as a protective dimension against these symptomatologies. When we compare the size of the indirect effects by pairs (see Table 6), it is observed that for each of the clinical variables, the biggest of the indirect effects is that generated by EP → NSE → Clinical Variable, followed by EP → SE → NSE → Clinical Variable. The smallest protective effect generated by EP being the smallest the protective effect generated by EP → SE → Clinical Variable.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Comparison of the magnitude of indirect effects of SE and negative self-evaluation.

Discussion and Conclusions

This study attempts to assess, in a general population sample with ages from 8 to 12 years, the relation that exists between child perfectionism dimensions and the intrapersonal problems of Worthlessness, Irritability, Thinking problems and Psychophysiological symptoms.

As a global conclusion, it has been shown that perfectionism is related to the occurrence of the psychological problems studied similarly to what it does with anxiety and child depression (Hewitt et al., 2002; McCreary et al., 2004; Stornelli et al., 2009; Lozano et al., 2015).

The results reaffirm the first hypothesis that EP and NSE are unhealthy perfectionism dimensions since they are significantly associated with the increase of the clinical symptomatology. Moreover, EP favors the development of an NSE (ideas of inferiority when compared with their peers, doubts if they have performed well in the tasks, as well as the non-acceptance of errors). It is also relevant to observe that the most important effect over the internalized problems that are studied is the one exerted by the interaction of the two dimensions that shape unhealthy perfectionism (EP and NSE).

These results are consistent with social learning models (Bandura, 1986) as well as with the cognitive theory of Beck et al. (1979). First, children learn in their relationship with their parents and teachers that, in order to be valued, they must meet the high expectations of achievement that are demanded from them. Therefore, their effort is permanently directed to reach that level of excellence in order to be approved and accepted. In this way, a cognitive pattern is being formed—perfectionism—that children use with regularity to judge everything that is related to the tasks they perform. When they make mistakes that are not accepted in their environment, this cognitive scheme is activated, favoring distorted judgments of reality that, in turn, triggers the occurrence of emotional suffering. Among the cognitive distortions that may appear when performing a task that is well but not perfectly executed, it is possible to find dichotomous thinking (“the task is not perfectly done then I am good for nothing”), magnification (“I have made a mistake, they are not going to love me”), “should” enunciations (“I should do it better”), incorrect labeling (“I have made a mistake, I am a loser”), etc. Therefore, the perfectionism scheme in children turns to be an element of cognitive vulnerability. According to the diathesis-stress model by Clark and Beck (2010), this vulnerability could emerge in situations in which children’s vital interests of approval, acceptance, independence, and competence are at potential risk, thus favoring the occurrence of the internalized symptomatology that is studied (e.g., Rice et al., 2015). If children feel incompetent to handle any demanding situation (diathesis), it can be interpreted as dangerous (stress), thus leading to self-perceptions of worthlessness and incompetence (i.e., I think I do everything wrong), irritability (i.e., I feel anger inside) and therefore increasing the thinking problems (i.e., “disturbing thoughts come to my mind, even if I don’t want”) as well as the corresponding physiological symptoms (e.g., “I feel a pressure in my chest that leaves me out of breath”).

Consistently with what Stöeber and Otto (2006) argue about perfectionist strivings, it is confirmed that SE is a protective variable that has a positive direct effect (when it increases, the symptomatology of worthlessness and psychophysical symptoms decrease) as well as an indirect effect (when it has a mediating effect between EP and NSE, it reduces the negative effect that this interaction causes in all the variables that are studied). A child with high SE that is presented with a task she or he feels capable of doing well without fear of making mistakes, with the certainty that she or he is going to be accepted and loved even in the case of failing at first, does not feel vulnerable when facing such a challenge, as there is no threat for the child’s approval and acceptance, and ultimately, the studied psychological problems do not appear.

In conclusion, this study does not support the theoretical position where perfectionism is always regarded as maladaptive and an indicator of a psychological maladjustment (Flett and Hewitt, 2006), since it can also be adaptive and healthy as it has also been shown in previous studies (Stöeber and Otto, 2006; Owens and Slade, 2008; Lozano et al., 2015).

These results have multiple practical consequences not only from a clinical perspective but also educative. Both approaches have to direct their efforts to develop a high intrapersonal intelligence, resilient children, able to face problems and not avoid or escape from them due to fear of failure. It is necessary to plan cooperative, nurturing, and not exclusively competitive contexts where adults express in a direct way their positive expectations of the child’s capacity and achievements (Kenney-Benson and Pomerantz, 2005; Hutchinson and Yates, 2008), together with a non-judgmental attitude toward errors, in order to favor the development of high self-esteem (McArdle and Duda, 2004) and to avoid worries or negative beliefs about themselves which can develop anxiety disorders in adults (Esbjørn et al., 2015).

This study also has limitations that could be transformed into future research directions. It would be relevant to supplement children’s perfectionism self-reports with teachers’ and parents’ opinions in order to examine if parent’s attributions could predict child perfectionism problems as have been found in relation with others disorders (e.g., Williamson and Johnston, 2015) and to use clinical and not only general population to generalize these results in child population too. Just as there is a large number of retrospective studies whose aim is to determine how parenting styles affect the development of perfectionism and emotional disorders (e.g., Yoon and Lau, 2008; Speirs Neumeister et al., 2009), it would be important to conduct them longitudinally as well, with the aim of determining how child perfectionism may affect the development of emotional disorders in adulthood.

Data Availability

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of University of Granada, ethic committee with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the University of Granada, ethic committee.

Author Contributions

LL and AL contributed conception and design of the study. IP and EG-C organized the database. LL and IV-S performed the statistical analysis and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness [Ref. PSI2017-85724-P and Ref. PSI2017- 83966-R (MINECO/AEI/FEDER/UE)].

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ayearst, L., Flett, G. L., and Hewitt, P. L. (2012). Where is multidimensional perfectionism in DSM-5? A question posed to the DSM-5 personality and personality disorders work group. Personal. Disord. Theory Res. Treat. 3, 458–469. doi: 10.1037/a0026354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., and Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Bouguettaya, A., Cruwys, T., Moulding, R., King, R., and Bliuc, A. M. (2019). Evidence that frame of reference effects can reduce socially prescribed perfectionism. Front. Psychol. 9:2703. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02703

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choy, G., and McInerney, V. (2006). Multidimensions of perfectionism and self-concept in school-aged children. Paper presented at the Australian Association of Research in Education Conference. Adelaide, Australia. Available at: https://www.aare.edu.au/data/publications/2006/cho06178.pdf (Accessed April 20, 2019).

Google Scholar

Clark, D. A., and Beck, A. T. (2010). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Cole, D. A., Jacquez, F. M., and Maschman, T. L. (2001). Social origins of depressive cognitions: a longitudinal study of self-perceived competence in children. Cogn. Ther. Res. 25, 377–395. doi: 10.1023/A:1005582419077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cumming, G. (2014). The new statistics: why and how. Psychol. Sci. 25, 7–29. doi: 10.1177/0956797613504966

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curran, T., and Hill, A. P. (2019). Perfectionism is increasing overtime: a meta-analysis of birth cohort differences from 1989 to 2016. Psychol. Bull. 145, 410–429. doi: 10.1037/bul0000138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DiBartolo, P. M., and Varner, S. P. (2012). How children’s cognitive and affective responses to a novel task relate to dimensions of perfectionism. J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 30, 53–61. doi: 10.1007/s10942-011-0130-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enns, M. W., and Cox, B. J. (2002). “The nature and assessment of perfectionism: a critical analysis” in Perfectionism: Theory, research, and treatment. eds. G. L. Flett and P. L. Hewitt (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 33–62.

Google Scholar

Esbjørn, B. H., Lønfeldt, N. N., Nielsen, S. K., Reinholdt-Dunne, M. L., Sømhovd, M. J., and Cartwright-Hatton, S. (2015). Meta-worry, worry, and anxiety in children and adolescents: relationships and interactions. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 44, 145–156. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.873980

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Essau, C. A., Leung, P. W. L., Conradt, J., Cheng, H., and Wong, J. (2008). Fear symptoms in Chinese and German adolescents: their relationship with early learning experiences, perfectionism, and learning motivation. Depress. Anxiety 25, 801–810. doi: 10.1002/da.20334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Alonso, R., Suárez-Álvarez, J., and Muñiz, J. (2012). Imputación de datos perdidos en las evaluaciones diagnósticas educativas [Imputation methods for missing data in educational diagnostic evaluation]. Psicothema 24, 167–175.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

G. L. Flett and P. L. Hewitt (eds.) (2002). “Perfectionism and maladjustment: an overview of theoretical, definitional, and treatment issues” in Perfectionism: Theory, research, and treatment. (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 5–13.

Google Scholar

Flett, G. L., and Hewitt, P. (2006). Positive versus negative perfectionism in psychopathology: a comment on Slade and Owens’s dual process model. Behav. Modif. 30, 472–495. doi: 10.1177/0145445506288026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flett, G. L., and Hewitt, P. (2012). Perfectionism and cognitive factors in distress and dysfunction in children and adolescents: introduction to the special issue. J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 30, 53–61. doi: 10.1007/s10942-011-0134-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flett, G. L., Panico, T., and Hewitt, P. L. (2011). Perfectionism, type A behavior and self-efficacy in depression and health symptoms among adolescents. Curr. Psychol. 30, 105–116. doi: 10.1007/s12144-011-9103-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frost, R. O., Heimberg, R. G., Holt, C. S., Mattia, J. I., and Neubauer, A. L. (1993). A comparison of two measures of perfectionism. Personal. Individ. Differ. 14, 119–126. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90181-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frost, R. O., Marten, P. A., Lahart, C. M., and Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfectionism. Cogn. Ther. Res. 14, 449–468.

Google Scholar

Gäde, J. C., Schermelleh-Engel, K., and Klein, A. G. (2017). Disentangling the common variance of perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns: a bifactor model of perfectionism. Front. Psychol. 8:160. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Herman, K. C., and Ostrander, R. O. (2007). The effects of attention problems on depression: developmental, cognitive, and academic pathways. Sch. Psychol. Q. 22, 483–510. doi: 10.1037/1045-3830.22.4.483

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hewitt, P. L., Caelian, C. F., Flett, G. L., Sherry, S. B., Collins, L., and Flynn, C. A. (2002). Perfectionism in children: associations with depression, anxiety, and anger. Personal. Individ. Differ. 32, 1049–1061. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00109-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hewitt, P. L., and Flett, G. L. (1991). Dimensions of perfectionism in unipolar depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 100, 98–101. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.1.98

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., Besser, A., Sherry, S. B., and McGee, B. (2003). Perfectionism is multidimensional: a reply to Shafran, Cooper, and Fairburn. Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 1221–1236. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00021-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, R. Y., Lee, S. S. M., Chng, R. Y., Zhou, Y., Tsai, F.-F., and Tan, S. H. (2016). Developmental trajectories of maladaptive perfectionism in middle childhood. J. Pers. 85, 409–422. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutchinson, A., and Yates, G. (2008). Maternal goal factors in adaptive and maladaptive childhood perfectionism. Educ. Psychol. 28, 795–808. doi: 10.1080/01443410802320402

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenney-Benson, G. A., and Pomerantz, E. M. (2005). The role of mothers’ use of control in children’s perfectionism: implications for the development of children’s depressive symptoms. J. Pers. 73, 23–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00303.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lloyd, S., Schmidt, U., Khondoker, M., and Tchanturia, K. (2014). Can psychological interventions reduce perfectionism? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 43, 705–731. doi: 10.1017/S1352465814000162

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loades, M. E., Rimes, K. A., Lievesley, K., Ali, S., and Chalder, T. (2019). Perfectionism and beliefs about emotions in adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome and their parents: a preliminary investigation in a case control study nested within a cohort. Psychol. Health 34, 850–866. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2019.1579331

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lozano, L., García-Cueto, E., and Lozano, L. M. (2011). Cuestionario Educativo Clínico: Ansiedad y Depresión. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

Google Scholar

Lozano, L. M., García-Cueto, E., Martín, M., and Lozano, L. (2012). Development and validation of the childhood perfectionism inventory (I.P.I.). Psicothema 24, 149–155.

Google Scholar

Lozano, L. M., García-Cueto, E., and Muñiz, J. (2008). Effect of the number of response categories on the reliability and validity of rating scales. Methodology 4, 73–79. doi: 10.1027/1614-2241.4.2.73

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lozano, L. M., Valor-Segura, I., and Lozano, L. (2015). Could a perfectionism context produce unhappy children? Personal. Individ. Differ. 80, 12–17. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.01.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McArdle, S., and Duda, J. (2004). Exploring social-contextual correlates of perfectionism in adolescents: a multivariate perspective. Cogn. Ther. Res. 28, 765–788. doi: 10.1007/s10608-004-0665-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCreary, B. T., Joiner, T. E., Schmidt, N. B., and Ialongo, N. S. (2004). The structure and correlates of perfectionism in African American children. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 33, 313–324. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3302_13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, L., and Lomax, C. (2014). Assessment, development, and treatment of childhood perfectionism: a systematic review. Child Adolesc. Mental Health 19, 225–234. doi: 10.1111/camh.12067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owens, R. G., and Slade, P. D. (2008). So perfect it’s positively harmful? Reflections on the adaptiveness and maladaptiveness of positive and negative perfectionism. Behav. Modif. 43, 472–495. doi: 10.1177/0145445508319667

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rice, K. G., Leever, B. A., Noggle, C. A., and Lapsley, D. K. (2007). Perfectionism and depression symptoms in early adolescence. Psychol. Sch. 44, 139–156. doi: 10.1002/pits.20212

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rice, K. G., and Preusser, K. J. (2002). The adaptive/maladaptive perfectionism scale. Meas. Eval. Couns. Dev. 34, 210–222.

Google Scholar

Rice, K. G., Ray, M. E., Davis, D. E., DeBlaere, C., and Ashby, J. S. (2015). Perfectionism and longitudinal patterns of stress for STEM majors: implications for academic performance. J. Couns. Psychol. 62, 718–731. doi: 10.1037/cou0000097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, R. E., Courvoisier, D. S., Cullati, S., Kraehenmann, R., and Van del Linden, M. (2018). Too imperfect to fall asleep: perfectionism, pre-sleep counterfactual processing, and insomnia. Front. Psychol. 9:1288. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01288

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherry, S. B., Gralnick, T. M., Hewitt, P. L., Sherry, D. L., and Flett, G. L. (2014). Perfectionism and narcissism: testing unique relationships and gender differences. Personal. Individ. Differ. 61–62, 52–56. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, M. M., Saklofske, D. H., Yan, G., and Sherry, S. B. (2014). Perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns interact to predict negative emotionality: support for the tripartite model of perfectionism in Canadian and Chinese university students. Personal. Individ. Differ. 81, 141–147. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speirs Neumeister, K. L., Williams, K. K., and Cross, T. L. (2009). Gifted high-school students’ perspectives on the development of perfectionism. Roeper Rev. 31, 198–206. doi: 10.1080/02783190903177564

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stöeber, J., and Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: approaches, evidence, challenges. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10, 295–319. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stornelli, D., Flett, G. L., and Hewitt, P. L. (2009). Perfectionism, achievement, and affect in children: a comparison of students from gifted, arts, and regular programs. Can. J. Sch. Psychol. 24, 267–283. doi: 10.1177/0829573509342392

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suárez-Álvarez, J., Pedrosa, I., Lozano, L. M., García-Cueto, E., Cuesta, M., and Muñiz, J. (2018). Using reversed items in Likert scales: a questionable practice. Psicothema 30, 149–158. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2018.33

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williamson, D., and Johnston, C. (2015). Maternal and paternal attributions in the prediction of boys’ behavior problems across time. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 44, 668–675. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.862803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoon, J., and Lau, A. (2008). Maladaptive perfectionism and depressive symptoms among Asian American college students: contributions of interdependence and parental relations. Cult. Divers. Ethn. Minor. Psychol. 14, 92–101. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.14.2.92

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: perfectionism, clinical symptomatology, childhood, mediation analyses, childhood perfectionism inventory

Citation: Lozano LM, Valor-Segura I, García-Cueto E, Pedrosa I, Llanos A and Lozano L (2019) Relationship Between Child Perfectionism and Psychological Disorders. Front. Psychol. 10:1855. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01855

Received: 28 April 2019; Accepted: 29 July 2019;
Published: 06 September 2019.

Edited by:

Eduardo Fonseca-Pedrero, University of La Rioja, Spain

Reviewed by:

Francisco J. Abad, Autonomous University of Madrid, Spain
Jorge L. Puga, Catholic University San Antonio of Murcia, Spain

Copyright © 2019 Lozano, Valor-Segura, García-Cueto, Pedrosa, Llanos and Lozano. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Inmaculada Valor-Segura, ivalor@ugr.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.