Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 12 September 2019
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Improving Wellbeing in Patients with Chronic Conditions: Theory, Evidence, and Opportunities View all 34 articles

Paranoid Thinking and Wellbeing. The Role of Doubt in Pharmacological and Metacognitive Therapies

\r\nLeonor Asensio-AguerriLeonor Asensio-Aguerri1Luis Beato-Fernndez,Luis Beato-Fernández1,2Maria StavrakiMaria Stavraki2Teresa Rodríguez-Cano,Teresa Rodríguez-Cano1,2Miriam BajoMiriam Bajo2Darío Díaz*Darío Díaz2*
  • 1Mental Health Unit, Hospital General Universitario de Ciudad Real, Ciudad Real, Spain
  • 2Ciudad Real Medical School, Universidad de Castilla – La Mancha, Ciudad Real, Spain

Introduction: Pathological confidence in one’s thoughts is a key mechanism of chronic paranoid thinking. For this reason, many of the current therapies focus on trying to reduce it. In fact, the way some antipsychotics (e.g., haloperidol) work seems to be through the induction of doubt. Because of the impact of these pathological thoughts on positive health, studying the well-being of people who experience paranoid thoughts is fundamental. The first objective of this research is to apply the Complete State Model of Health (CSMH) to a sample of patients characterized by the presence of paranoid thinking. Our second objective is to evaluate the impact of therapies based on reducing pathological confidence on patients’ well-being.

Methods: Sixty participants with SCID-5 confirmed DSM-5 diagnosis related with paranoid thinking and without mood symptoms were recruited. In order to test the existence of a two continua model of mental health (CSMH), we conducted a parallel analysis and an exploratory factor analysis. To test our hypothesis regarding the partially mediating role of doubt between paranoid thinking and patients’ well-being, we conducted a biased corrected bootstrapping procedure.

Results: As expected, two different unipolar dimensions emerged from the measures used to assess paranoid thinking and positive health (two continua model of mental health). When patients received metacognitive and pharmacological treatment, more paranoid thinking led to more doubt in all thoughts, which in turn affected well-being. The analyses carried out confirmed the partial mediating role of doubt.

Conclusion: Despite the efficacy shown by both metacognitive therapies and antipsychotics, it seems that they not only reduce pathological confidence, but can also affect other thoughts not linked to delirium. This effect of generalization of doubt in all thoughts negatively affected patients’ well-being and quality of life.

Introduction

Paranoid delusional thinking is one of the central symptoms of psychosis. It can appear in a wide spectrum of disorders from individuals who do not have pathologies (Penn et al., 1997), to people with delusional disorder, brief psychotic disorder, schizophreniform disorder, schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, psychotic disorder induced by substances or drugs or personality disorders (e.g., paranoid personality disorder), among others (Garety et al., 1999). These delusional thoughts have traditionally been considered as false beliefs that are hard to modify, expressed with absolute conviction and not amenable to reason (Freeman et al., 2013). Therefore, in the study of paranoid thoughts, primary cognition must be considered, since irrational beliefs may be the result of a negative evaluation of situations, relationships with others and their interpretations (Freeman and Garety, 1999; Freeman et al., 2002; Morrison et al., 2015). Metacognition should also been taken into account, since delusional thinking is linked to second-order cognition (thoughts about one’s thoughts) (Moritz et al., 2016). That is, delusions are also related to the pathological confidence linked to incorrigibility, and to the collection of less information before making a decision or “jumping to conclusions” (Garety et al., 1999; Freeman et al., 2002; Moritz and Woodward, 2005). This confidence in thoughts is especially important, given that, regardless of the content of what we think, trust (or doubt) guides the use of this content (Self-Validation Theory) (Briñol and Petty, 2003).

Since pathological confidence in one’s thoughts is a key mechanism of delusional thinking, many of the current therapies focus on trying to reduce it. One of the most recent therapies is developed by Moritz and Woodward (2007) and Moritz et al. (2013), called MyMCT (previously MCT). This therapy is based on making patients aware of the cognitive distortions linked to delusional thinking (e.g., “jump to conclusions” or “overconfidence” in errors). Subsequently, they are taught alternative coping strategies and information processing. Therefore, one of the objectives of this therapy is to reduce confidence in pathological thoughts, in order to alter delusional beliefs (Moritz et al., 2014a; Eichner and Berna, 2016). However, not only metacognitive therapies influence the processes of secondary cognition linked to paranoid thinking. From a cognitive point of view, the induction of doubt is one of the central cognitive effects of antipsychotics (e.g., Haloperidol) (Freeman et al., 2002). The antipsychotics also cause patients to detach from their thoughts, which allow them to treat thoughts as objects and diminish confidence in them (Kapur et al., 2005).

Therefore, generating doubt to reduce pathological confidence seems to be a good option in the treatment of chronic paranoid thinking. This reduction in confidence can occur through affective or cognitive (in)validation. Affective validation occurs when people use their thoughts because they feel good about them or like them (e.g., Briñol et al., 2007; Huntsinger et al., 2014). Therefore, dampening positive emotions reduces thought reliance. Cognitive validation takes place when people use their thoughts because they believe they are valid or correct (see Briñol et al., 2018 for a description of both types of validation).

The fundamental problem is that the doubt generated (both by metacognitive therapies and by antipsychotics) could not only be limited to delusional thoughts (reducing pathological confidence), it could also be extended to cognition in general (generating an extreme doubt). This would be especially relevant given that trust in thoughts is a fundamental variable for people’s positive psychological functioning and well-being (Taylor and Brown, 1988). In addition, paranoid thinking greatly affects patients’ quality of life (Karow and Pajonk, 2006).

For these reasons, studying the well-being of people who experience chronic paranoid thoughts is fundamental, regardless of their psychopathological evaluation (Chan et al., 2018). In fact, the presence of health not only requires absence of disease, but also having a state of complete physical, mental and social well-being (World Health Organization, 1946). To evaluate this state Corey Keyes developed the Complete State Model of Health (CSMH) (Keyes, 2005). According to this model, health is not a state characterized solely by the absence of disease, but also by the presence of indicators of hedonic well-being (hedonia) and eudaimonic well-being (positive functioning). The application of this model for the evaluation of patients with paranoid thinking has interesting consequences given that the CSMH defends that health and disease are not two poles of a single dimension, but two unipolar dimensions correlated with each other (two continua model of mental health). Therefore, the presence of pathology does not imply the absence of positive health. Although the CSMH has been widely applied and tested in different populations (Keyes, 2006, 2009; Bariola et al., 2017; de Vos et al., 2018), the existence of two different axes (positive mental health and mental illness) should be examined for each disorder or psychopathological process (e.g., McGaffin et al., 2015; Díaz et al., 2018) since there are cases where the existence of two axes is not met (e.g., Díaz et al., 2007).

Although it is especially interesting to apply the CSMH in the study of disorders related to paranoid thinking, to the best of our knowledge there are no studies that have proven the existence of two axes in these disorders. There is just one study in which the CSMH has been applied in the study of schizophrenia spectrum disorders (Chan et al., 2018), but in that study the existence of two-dimensions related to mental illness and positive mental health was not examined. For these reasons, the first objective of the present research is to apply the CSMH to a sample of patients with paranoid thinking to explore the two different unipolar dimensions: paranoid thinking and positive mental health (i.e., well-being indicators). In addition, we expect that paranoid thinking and positive mental health will be correlated.

Finally, our second objective is to study how metacognitive and pharmacological therapies influence confidence in thoughts of people with paranoid thinking. Based on previous research (Moritz et al., 2014a; Eichner and Berna, 2016) our next hypothesis is that therapies will reduce pathological confidence. However, we also expect that these therapies will generate extreme doubt in all thoughts, and not only the ones associated with delirium. Given that confidence is a key element of positive functioning, this adverse metacognitive effect of generalization of doubt would affect patients’ positive health (i.e., well-being). As a consequence, we expect doubt to partially mediate the effect of paranoid thinking on patients’ well-being.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Sixty participants (25 females and 35 males) with SCID-5 confirmed DSM-5 diagnosis related with paranoid thinking and without mood symptoms (i.e., Schizophrenia, Brief Psychotic Disorder, Delusional Disorder or Substance/Medication-Induced Psychotic Disorder) were recruited in the HGUCR. Recruitment was carried out between September 2016 and August 2017. Patients were hospitalized and were in a post-acute or stable phase of their disorders. Patients with a comorbid diagnosis of other mental disorders at the time the study was conducted were excluded (e.g., Major Depressive Disorder). In addition, participants were excluded if they could not complete questionnaires written in Spanish. Participants were recruited via letter of invitation explaining the project and the voluntary nature of participation. They were 25 women (42%) and 35 men (58%) between 18 and 76 years old. The mean age was 41.12 years (SD = 14.78). The maximum educational level reached to 57% of primary education, 20% higher no university education, 20% hold a university degree and 3% a PhD. Twenty patients had Schizophrenia, twenty Brief Psychotic Disorder, fifteen Delusional Disorder, and five Substance/Medication-Induced Psychotic Disorder. All participants were already prescribed atypical antipsychotic medication at baseline. Participants received a metacognitive therapy focused in overconfidence based on Well’s metacognitive therapy, which assumes that the cause of disorder is located at the metacognitive level not at the level of cognitive content (Wells and Matthews, 1996). The adaptation of the Wells’ metacognitive model of GAD (Wells, 1995) proposed by Morrison et al. (Morrison et al., 2014) was used. The exact duration of sessions varied, as a flexible use of the manuals is advocated, based on the individual case formulation (e.g., Normann et al., 2014; see Hutton et al., 2014, for a complete description of the therapy). On average, patients received 562 min of metacognitive therapy (SD = 51 min).

Procedure

The study was approved by the ethics committee of the “Universidad de Castilla – La Mancha” (UCLM) and the HGUCR (Comité Ético de Investigación Clínica HGUCR-UCLM). All participants were informed that all collected information was confidential and anonymous, and signed an informed consent. To avoid possible order effects of the two sets of scales, half of the participants first completed the PIQ, and the PADS, and then the Satisfaction with Life Scale, the Positive Affect Scale and the Psychological Well-being Scales. The other half first completed the well-being scales and, then, proceeded to complete the PIQ and PADS.

Measures

Paranoid Thinking

Participants filled the Spanish version (Fonseca-Pedrero et al., 2009) of the PIQ (McKay et al., 2006), a 10-item tool that was constructed according to comprehensive definitional considerations and criteria set out by Freeman and Garety (2000). The original PIQ and the Spanish version of the questionnaire have previously used with clinical and non-clinical samples, showing in both cases excellent reliability and validity as a brief measurement of paranoid thinking (i.e., persecutory ideation; Fonseca-Pedrero et al., 2009). Participants answered using a 5-point Likert scale response format ranging from 0 (very untrue) to 4 (very true). In addition, the Spanish version (Valiente et al., 2011) of the PADS (Melo et al., 2009); was used. This scale is a brief measure to assess both the severity of paranoid thinking (PADS-P) and the perceived deservedness of persecution (PADS-D) and is suitable for both clinical and non-clinical populations. The two subscales possess good internal consistency and factorial validity (e.g., Valiente et al., 2011). Participants were asked to rate each item on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 (certainly false) to 4 (certainly true).

Positive Health

According to the CSMH, to measure hedonia indicators, participants responded to the Satisfaction With Life Scale (Diener et al., 1985) and Positive Affect Scale. The Satisfaction With Life Scale (Spanish version; Cabañero et al., 2004) includes five items with adequate psychometric properties (e.g., Rodríguez-Carvajal et al., 2010). Participants responded to the items using a Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Moreover, they completed the Positive Affect Scale (Spanish version; Díaz et al., 2007) indicating how frequently, during the last month, they have felt: calm and peaceful, extremely happy, in good spirits, cheerful, satisfied, and full of life. Participants answered the items using a response format ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (all the time). We also measured positive function indicators using the Psychological Well-being Scales (Ryff, 1989; Díaz et al., 2006). This instrument includes six dimensions of positive functioning (i.e., autonomy, self-acceptance, positive relations, environmental mastery, purpose in life and personal growth). Participants responded using a 6-point Likert scale anchored with 1 = strongly disagree and 6 = strongly agree. The structure of six dimensions (with or without a general factor) has been demonstrated through confirmatory factor analysis in previous research (Díaz et al., 2006, 2015; Van Dierendonck et al., 2008). Following Keyes (2005) and Díaz et al. (2018), to obtain a categorical measure of the presence (flourishing) or absence (languishing) of positive mental health, we considered the presence of each indicator when participant’s score in the scale was equal to or greater than the mean of the general Spanish population according to an aggregate data set composed by three samples (Blanco and Díaz, 2005; Díaz et al., 2006, 2007). The cut-off points for the categorical mental health indicators were: satisfaction with life = 3.28; positive affect = 3.36; autonomy = 4.24; self-acceptance = 4.31; positive relations = 4.58; environmental mastery = 4.32, purpose in life = 4.48; personal growth = 4.56. The presence of positive mental health required both criteria of hedonia and positive functioning. Moreover, each patient took part in a semi-structured interview based on the indicators proposed by Keyes (2005). The professionals who interviewed the patients codified the presence or absence of each indicator. Next, the presence or absence of positive health was tested based on the same criteria (CSMH categorical diagnosis; see Supplementary Table S1). The evaluations of well-being scales and semi-structured interviews converged for 58 patients (96.7%). In the two divergences found, we used the result of the semi-structured interview.

Doubt in General Thoughts

To assess participants’ subjective feeling of doubt in general thoughts two items anchored at not at all (1) and extremely (9) asking how much doubt they had in their general thoughts and how invalid they considered their general thoughts, were administered. Ratings were highly intercorrelated (r = 0.87), so they were averaged to create a composite doubt index. Higher values on this index indicated more doubt in general thoughts.

Data Analysis

To test the two continua model of mental health (two different dimensions emerge from the measures used to assess positive mental health and paranoid thinking), we conducted an EFA. A critical methodological decision concerning EFA is the number of factors to retain. According to previous literature, we employed PA, which is one of the most accurate factor retention methods (e.g., Glorfeld, 1995; Hayton et al., 2004). Specifically, we calculated the mean and the 95th percentile for each of the eigenvalues of 100 randomly generated data sets using a SPSS syntax (O’connor, 2000). Next, we compared these random data eigenvalues to real data eigenvalues obtained from a PCA. In the PCA we extracted a number of factors equal to the number of scales entered into the analysis. Finally, we conducted an EFA based on the criterion established by the PA regarding the number of factors to be extracted. We employed principal axis as factor extraction method (Fabrigar et al., 1999) and direct oblimin as rotation method, since the emerged dimensions were expected to be correlated. We computed factor scores following the procedure proposed by Grice (2001). To study the relationship between paranoid thinking and well-being indicators, we used Pearson’s correlations, a contingency table, and a Fisher’s exact test. Finally, to test our hypothesis regarding the mediating role of doubt between paranoid thinking and patients wellbeing, we conducted a biased corrected bootstrapping procedure with 10,000 bootstrap re-samples using Hayes PROCESS macro (model 4; see Figure 2). PROCESS is a computational procedure for SPSS and SAS that implements moderation or mediation analysis as well as their combination in an integrated conditional process model (Shrout and Bolger, 2002; Preacher and Hayes, 2004; Hayes, 2013; Bajo et al., 2018). Some factors can produce spurious associations, particularly in a non-experimental study such as the present one. Therefore, demographic data, including sex, age, and education level, and DSM-5 diagnosis were introduced as covariates in mediation analysis.

Results

Descriptive statistics (means and standard deviations) and Cronbach alpha coefficients (α) of paranoid thinking, well-being scales and doubt index are displayed in Table 1. As we can see in Table 2, the results of PA indicate that only the first and second eigenvalues of the real dataset exceeded random values. Scree Plot (Figure 1) confirms these results. Based on these findings, an EFA was conducted. As shown in Table 3, all the well-being scales essentially loaded on the first factor, explaining 46% of variance. On the other hand, all paranoid thinking scales loaded on the second factor, explaining 14% of the variance. These results support the two continua model of mental health. That is, mental health has two different unipolar dimensions: Positive mental health (i.e., Satisfaction With Life Scale, Positive Affect Scale and Psychological Well-being Scales; factor 1) and paranoid thinking (i.e., PIQ, PADS-P and PADS-D; factor 2). Correlation between the two factors was -0.46, which is an indicator of the strong existing relationship between paranoid thinking and positive health. To explore in more detail this relationship, we calculated the Pearson’s correlations of well-being scales and paranoid thinking scales (Table 4). The psychological well-being scales were all significantly correlated with PIQ, PADS-P and PADS-D (with the exception of the correlation between Personal Growth and PIQ). Precisely, the autonomy scale showed stronger relation to paranoid thinking than the rest of the scales. However, the subjective well-being scales showed a weak relationship with paranoid thinking. Only the relationship between life satisfaction and PIQ was significant.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Means (M), standard deviations (SD), and Cronbach alpha coefficients (α) of paranoid thinking, well-being measures, and doubt index.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Parallel analysis.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scree plot.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Exploratory factor analysis of paranoid thinking scales and well-being measures.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Pearson’s correlations and 95% confidence intervals of paranoid thinking scales and well-being measures.

Having the existence of the two-dimensionality hypothesis confirmed, we expected that some patients with paranoid thinking could be assessed as healthy by meeting the criteria for the presence of positive health (categorical diagnostic approach). The categorical diagnosis requires the criteria of hedonia (a high level on positive affect and/or informed life satisfaction) and positive function (a high level on three or more of the psychological well-being indicators) to be met. To test this idea, we computed a contingency table of positive mental health (presence-absence) X disorder (Schizophrenia, Brief Psychotic Disorder, Delusional Disorder and Substance/Medication-Induced Psychotic Disorder) (Table 5). Notably, 19 patients (31.7%) were mentally healthy.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Contingency table of mental disorder diagnosis and positive health.

Finally, applying the two continua model of mental health and according to our hypothesis (cognitive and pharmacological therapies reduce pathological confidence in all kind of thoughts, not only in pathological ones), we predicted that: (1) more paranoid thinking implies more external doubt (induced by longer metacognitive therapy sessions or more antipsychotics doses) in general thoughts; (2) more paranoid thinking implies less well-being; (3) more doubt implies less well-being; (4) doubt partially mediates the relationship between paranoid thinking and well-being. First, to check that metacognitive therapy was related to participants’ doubt in their own thoughts we correlated the total duration of therapy with the doubt index. Pearson correlation was significant (r = 0.58, p < 0.01), therefore longer duration implied greater doubt. Next, to test our hypothesis we conducted a biased corrected bootstrapping procedure using Hayes PROCESS macro (model 4) (Shrout and Bolger, 2002; Preacher and Hayes, 2004). This approach includes procedures that compute a 95% CI around the indirect effect and mediation is supported if this CI does not include zero. Paranoid thinking was the independent variable, well-being was the dependent variable, and doubt was the mediating variable (see Figure 2). As predicted, the data revealed that the 95% CI of the indirect effect (i.e., the path through the mediator) did not include zero (Indirect Effect a x b = 0.15, CI 95% = from -0.34 to -0.05), thus mediation by doubt is supported (Bajo et al., 2018).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Doubt in one’s thoughts as a mediator between Paranoid Thinking and Well-being. Figure in the parenthesis (i.e., –0.28) is the direct effect of Paranoid Thinking on Well-being while accounting for the effect through the indirect path (p < 0.05).

Discussion

Primary social cognition (e.g., the content or direction of thoughts) has been widely studied in paranoid thinking. In fact, there are different theories that have tried to explain the importance of social cognition in the origin and maintenance of this type of thinking (Penn et al., 1997; Freeman et al., 2013). However, the study of secondary cognition or metacognition has received less attention, although research in this concept has grown exponentially in the last decade. The interest in the study of metacognitive processes linked to paranoid thinking is basically related to pathological confidence that characterizes this type of thinking. This confidence is essential to understand paranoid delusions given that, regardless of the content of what we think, trust (or doubt) directs the use we make of this content (Self-Validation Theory) (Briñol and Petty, 2003). Therefore, increased confidence in a thought leads to more use of this though. In addition, pathological confidence causes thoughts to be rigid, and makes it very difficult to modify them (e.g., through the use of cognitive therapies, CBT) (Moritz et al., 2014a). As a result, different metacognitive interventions have been developed in recent years (e.g., Moritz et al., 2014a; Morrison et al., 2014). Despite the controversy over whether these therapies act at a cognitive or metacognitive level (e.g., Andreou et al., 2018; Capobianco and Wells, 2018), they focus on the generation of doubt in thoughts related to paranoid delirium (Moritz and Lysaker, 2018). Moreover, the induction of doubt is also one of the effects of antipsychotic drugs. For this reason, the main objective of this research was to analyze the effect of doubt (vs. confidence) caused by this type of therapies (i.e., metacognitive and pharmacological based on antipsychotics) on patients’ well-being and quality of life.

In order to analyze patients’ well-being, we applied the CSMH to the study of paranoid thinking. First, and according to our hypothesis, we verified one of the main axioms of the CSMH, the existence of two different axes: mental illness (paranoid thinking) and positive mental health (well-being indicators). Both Horn PA and EFA indicated the presence of two oblique factors. Although previous research applied CSMH in the study of schizophrenia spectrum disorders (Taylor and Brown, 1988), the existence of two-dimensions related to mental illness and positive mental health has not been proven. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first time that the existence of this axiom is confirmed. Despite this bi-dimensionality (mental illness – positive mental health), both factors were highly correlated. In this sense, the strongest relations were between psychological well-being and paranoid thinking (PADSP, PADSD, and PIQ). In particular, autonomy showed the greatest association with paranoid thinking. Furthermore, this indicator of psychological well-being showed a high factorial load (>0.60) in factor 2 (Paranoid Thinking). These results are consistent with previous research that indicates the strong impact of schizophrenia spectrum disorders on patients’ autonomy and quality of life (Prouteau et al., 2005; Carrión et al., 2013).

The existence of two different axes (mental illness – positive mental health) has interesting consequences. The first is related to the idea that the absence (or decrease) of paranoid thinking is not equivalent to the presence of health. Therefore, it is important to continue working to improve well-being and quality of life of patients with paranoid thinking, independently of the reduction of symptoms (Collins et al., 1991; Norman et al., 2000). From another perspective, a second consequence is that the presence of paranoid thinking does not necessarily imply the absence of positive health (i.e., subjective well-being and psychological well-being). In fact, according to our results, 19 out of 60 patients showed presence of positive health (flourishing) despite being diagnosed with disorders related to paranoid thinking. Therefore, patients with clinical symptoms of paranoid thinking may inform satisfactory levels of well-being and quality of life. A possible explanation of the high number of patients with presence of positive mental health is related to our sample, given that all patients were in a post-acute or stable phase of the disorders. Even so, other research has also found similar results (Chan et al., 2018).

Once the main axiom of the CSMH was verified, our main purpose was to evaluate the impact that metacognitive and pharmacological therapies based on antipsychotics have on patients’ well-being. Our results suggest that when patients received metacognitive and pharmacological treatment, more paranoid thinking implied more doubt in general thoughts. This relationship probably appeared because patients with more paranoid thinking required longer sessions of metacognitive therapy and higher doses of prescribed antipsychotic. These results seem to suggest that although the main metacognitive therapies focus on reducing pathological confidence, they could be affecting thoughts regardless of whether they were related to delirium or not. The same might happen with antipsychotics, that is, their effect on generating doubt may not be specific to pathological content. Consequently, the generalization of doubt in all thoughts could negatively affect patients’ well-being. This result should not be surprising because confidence in thoughts is a fundamental variable of positive functioning (Huppert and So, 2013). Indeed, doubt that patients reported in their thoughts (generated by the metacognitive and pharmacological treatment) partially mediated the effect of paranoid thinking on well-being. Therefore, despite the efficacy shown by antipsychotics (first or second generation; Leucht et al., 2009) and metacognitive therapies (Moritz et al., 2014a), they can produce a generalization of doubt which in turn can negatively affect patients’ well-being and quality of life, at least in the beginning of the treatment. Previous research (Moritz et al., 2014b) indicated that in the long term there could be a “sleeper” effect of metacognitive therapy that would increase patients’ self-esteem and quality of life. Future studies could explore if this “sleeper” effect is related to the recovery of confidence in thoughts not related to delirium. In this sense, to improve patients’ well-being and quality of life, it seems interesting to design interventions to increase confidence in thoughts not related to delirium.

Despite the contribution described above, the present work has several limitations. The first one is related to the sample size. We determined the sample size taking into account the difficulty to access hospitalized patients with paranoid thinking who would agree to collaborate and fill out the informed consent. In fact, most studies related to paranoid thinking and schizophrenia tend to have relatively small samples (Kay et al., 1987; Briggs et al., 2008; Dias et al., 2013; Eisenacher and Zink, 2017). Although sample size may have affected some of our analyses, it did not compromise the obtained conclusions. Regarding the factorial analyses carried out, although the ratio N:p is relatively low (5.5), the communalities are greater than 0.50 (with the exceptions of PIQ = 0.45, Satisfaction = 0.30, Positive Affection = 0.35, and Positive Relationships = 0.35). These results indicate an acceptable factor recovery (MacCallum et al., 1999; Hogarty et al., 2005). In addition, with respect to external validity, different studies have found similar results showing the emergence of two different but related factors of positive health measures (i.e., well-being) and pathology (Keyes, 2005; Bajo et al., 2018). Concerning the contingency table between diagnosis of mental disorders and positive health, we used a Fisher’s exact test precisely because we expected low values in the cells of the contingency table. Regarding the correlation analyses, we have included CIs to measure the impact of the sample size on the results (Altman and Gardner, 1988). Finally, in relation to the mediation analysis, the smaller the sample size, the lower are the probabilities of finding a statistically significant mediation (Shrout and Bolger, 2002). Since our research reveal the expected mediation effect, this suggests that it is unlikely our study lacked sufficient power to support the null hypothesis. Another limitation of our study is that we did not use social well-being measures to evaluate positive mental health. Given the special characteristics of our sample, we decided not to evaluate this aspect, since we did not want cognitive fatigue to affect the results. Lastly, a final limitation of our study is that patients received simultaneously pharmacological treatment and metacognitive therapy. As a consequence, we could not study the effect of the generalization of doubt in isolation for each treatment. Probably, the effect of generalization of doubt is produced in both treatments in an isolated way since the cognitive effect of the antipsychotics’ confidence reduction does not seem to be specific for the paranoid thoughts, and there is a high probability that metacognitive therapy also affects thoughts unrelated to delusions (Moritz et al., 2014a, 2016). In fact, we found a significant relationship between the duration of metacognitive treatment and doubt in general thoughts. Future investigations should further explore this question.

Conclusion

In this study we verified, for the first time, one of the main axioms of the CSMH: positive mental health and paranoid thinking are not two poles of a single dimension, but two unipolar dimensions correlated with each other. Therefore, the absence (or reduction) of paranoid thinking does not imply the presence of health. As a consequence, it is important to continue working to improve the well-being and quality of life of patients with paranoid thinking, independently of the reduction of symptoms. In addition, the presence of paranoid thinking does not necessarily imply the absence of positive health. That is, people with paranoid thinking can experience satisfactory levels of well-being. Once the main axiom of CSMH was verified, our main purpose was to evaluate the impact that metacognitive and pharmacological therapies based on antipsychotics have on patients’ well-being. Despite the efficacy shown by metacognitive therapies (Moritz et al., 2014a) and antipsychotics (first or second generation; Leucht et al., 2009), it seems that they not only reduce pathological confidence, but can also affect other thoughts not linked to delirium. This effect of generalization of doubt in all thoughts must be controlled, given that it negatively affects patient’s well-being and quality of life.

Data Availability

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Ethics Statement

This study was part of a research project funded by the Spanish Ministry of Education and Science, and was approved by the ethics committee of the “Universidad de Castilla – La Mancha” (UCLM) and the HGUCR (“Comité Ético de Investigación Clínica, HGUCR – UCLM”). All participants completed an informed consent form, assuring them that all information they provided would remain confidential and anonymous.

Author Contributions

DD and LA-A conceived the study design. LA-A collected the data. DD, LA-A, and LB-F drafted the manuscript. MB, MS, and LA-A performed the data analysis. MS, MB, and TR-C contributed to the critical revisions of the manuscript. All authors discussed the results, implications, and literature, and approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Funding

This research was supported by the Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness – Government of Spain (PSI2017-83303-C2-2-P).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02099/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

CBT, cognitive-behavioral therapy; CI, confidence interval; CSMH, Complete State Model of Health; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; EFA, exploratory factor analysis; HGUCR, Hospital General Universitario de Ciudad Real; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; MCT and MyMCT, metacognitive training; PA, parallel analysis; PADS, Persecution and Deservedness Scale; PCA, principal component analysis; PIQ, Persecutory Ideation Questionnaire; SCID-5, Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5.

References

Altman, D. G., and Gardner, M. J. (1988). Calculating confidence intervals for regression and correlation. Br. Med. J. 296, 1238–1242. doi: 10.1136/bmj.296.6631.1238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andreou, C., Balzan, R. P., Jelinek, L., and Moritz, S. (2018). Letter to the editor: metacognitive training and metacognitive therapy. A reply to Lora Capobianco and Adrian Wells. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 59, 162–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bajo, M., Blanco, A., Stavraki, M., Gandarillas, B., Cancela, A., Requero, B., et al. (2018). Post-traumatic cognitions and quality of life in terrorism victims: the role of well-being in indirect versus direct exposure. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 16:96. doi: 10.1186/s12955-018-0923-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bariola, E., Lyons, A., and Lucke, J. (2017). Flourishing among sexual minority individuals: application of the dual continuum model of mental health in a sample of lesbians and gay men. Psychol. Sex. Orientat. Gend. Divers. 4, 43–53. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanco, A., and Díaz, D. (2005). Social well-being: theoretical structure and measurement. Psicotherma 17, 582–589.

Google Scholar

Briggs, A., Wild, D., Lees, M., Reaney, M., Dursun, S., Parry, D., et al. (2008). Impact of schizophrenia and schizophrenia treatment-related adverse events on quality of life: direct utility elicitation. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 6:105. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-6-105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briñol, P., Petty, R., and Barden, J. (2007). Happiness versus sadness as a determinant of thought confidence in persuasion: a self-validation analysis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 711–727. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briñol, P., and Petty, R. E. (2003). Over head movements and persuasion: a self-validation analysis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 1123–1139. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., Stavraki, M., Lamprinakos, G., Wagner, B., and Díaz, D. (2018). Affective and cognitive validation of thoughts: an appraisal perspective on anger, disgust, surprise, and awe. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 114, 693–718. doi: 10.1037/pspa0000118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabañero, M. J., Richard, M., Cabrero, J., Orts, M. I., Reig, A., and Tosal, B. (2004). Fiabilidad y validez de una Escala de Satisfacción con la Vida de Diener en una muestra de mujeres embarazadas y puérperas. Psicothema 16, 448–455.

Google Scholar

Capobianco, L., and Wells, A. (2018). Letter to the editor: metacognitive therapy or metacognitive training: what’s in a name? J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 59:161. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2017.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrión, R. E., McLaughlin, D., Goldberg, T. E., Auther, A. M., Olsen, R. H., Olvet, D. M., et al. (2013). Prediction of functional outcome in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 1133–1142. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.1909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, R., Mak, W., Chio, F., and Tong, A. (2018). Flourishing with psychosis: a prospective examination on the interactions between clinical, functional, and personal recovery processes on well-being among individuals with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr. Bull. 44, 778–786. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, E. J., Hogan, T. P., and Desai, H. (1991). Measurement of therapeutic response in schizophrenia. A critical survey. Schizophr. Res. 5, 249–253. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(91)90083-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vos, J. A., Radstaak, M., Bohlmeijer, E. T., and Westerhof, G. J. (2018). Having an eating disorder and still being able to flourish? Examination of pathological symptoms and well-being as two continua of mental health in a clinical sample. Front. Psychol. 9:2145. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dias, E. C., Bickel, S., Epstein, M. L., Sehatpour, P., and Javitt, D. C. (2013). Abnormal task modulation of oscillatory neural activity in schizophrenia. Front. Psychol. 4:540. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz, D., Blanco, A., Horcajo, J., and Valle, C. (2007). Depression and the complete state model of health. Psicothema 19, 286–294.

Google Scholar

Díaz, D., Rodríguez-Carvajal, R., Blanco, A., Moreno-Jiménez, B., Gallardo, I., Valle, C., et al. (2006). Spanish adaptation of the psychological well-being scales (PWBS). Psicothema 18, 572–577.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Díaz, D., Stavraki, M., Blanco, A., and Bajo, M. (2018). 11-M victims 3 years after madrid terrorist attacks: looking for health beyond Trauma. J. Happiness Stud. 19, 663–675. doi: 10.1007/s10902-016-9842-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz, D., Stavraki, M., Blanco, A., and Gandarillas, B. (2015). The eudaimonic component of satisfaction with life and psychological well-being in Spanish cultures. Psicothema 27, 247–253. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2015.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Emmons, R., Larsen, R., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Eichner, C., and Berna, F. (2016). Acceptance and efficacy of metacognitive training (MCT) on positive symptoms and delusions in patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis taking into account important moderators. Schizophr. Bull. 42, 952–962. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenacher, S., and Zink, M. (2017). The importance of metamemory functioning to the pathogenesis of psychosis. Front. Psychol. 8:304. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabrigar, L., Maccallum, R., Wegener, D., and Haggard, P. (1999). Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychol. Methods 4, 272–299. doi: 10.1037//1082-989x.4.3.272

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Paíno-Piñeiro, M., Lemos-Giráldez, S., Villazón-García, U., and Muñiz, J. (2009). Validation of the schizotypal personality questionnaire—brief form in adolescents. Schizophr. Res. 111, 53–60. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, D., Dunn, G., Fowler, D., Bebbington, P., Kuipers, E., Emsley, R., et al. (2013). Current paranoid thinking in patients with delusions: the presence of cognitive-affective biases. Schizophr. Bull. 39, 1281–1287. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, D., and Garety, P. A. (1999). Worry, worry processes and dimensions of delusions: an exploratory investigation of a role for anxiety processes in the maintenance of delusional distress. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 27, 47–62.

Google Scholar

Freeman, D., and Garety, P. A. (2000). Comments on the content of persecutory delusions: does the definition need clarification? Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 39, 407–414. doi: 10.1348/014466500163400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, D., Garety, P. A., Kuipers, E., Fowler, D., and Bebbington, P. E. (2002). A cognitive model of persecutory delusions. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 41, 331–347.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Garety, P., Philippa, A., and Freeman, D. (1999). Cognitive approaches to delusions: a critical review of theories and evidence. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 38, 113–154. doi: 10.1348/014466599162700

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glorfeld, L. (1995). An improvement on Horn’s parallel analysis methodology for selecting the correct number of factors to retain. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 55, 377–393. doi: 10.1177/0013164495055003002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grice, J. (2001). Computing and evaluating factor scores. Psychol. Methods 6, 430–450. doi: 10.1037//1082-989x.6.4.430-450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Hayton, J., Allen, D., and Scarpello, V. (2004). Factor retention decisions in exploratory factor analysis: a tutorial on parallel analysis. Organ Res. Methods 7, 191–205. doi: 10.1177/1094428104263675

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogarty, K. Y., Hines, C. V., Kromrey, J. D., Ferron, J. M., and Mumford, K. R. (2005). The quality of factor solutions in exploratory factor analysis: the influence of sample size, communality, and overdetermination. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 65, 202–226. doi: 10.3758/s13428-014-0486-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huntsinger, J., Isbell, L., and Clore, G. L. (2014). The affective control of thought: malleable, not fixed. Psychol. Rev. 121, 600–618. doi: 10.1037/a0037669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huppert, F., and So, T. (2013). Flourishing across Europe: application of a new conceptual framework for defining well-being. Soc. Indic. Res. 110, 837–861. doi: 10.1007/s11205-011-9966-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutton, P., Morrison, A., Wardle, M., and Wells, A. (2014). Metacognitive therapy in treatment-resistant psychosis: a multiple-baseline study. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 42, 166–185. doi: 10.1017/S1352465812001026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kapur, S., Mizrahi, R., and Li, M. (2005). From dopamine to salience to psychosis—linking biology, pharmacology and phenomenology of psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 79, 59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karow, A., and Pajonk, F. (2006). Insight and quality of life in schizophrenia: recent findings and treatment implications. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 19, 637–641. doi: 10.1097/01.yco.0000245754.21621.c9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A., and Opler, L. A. (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 13, 261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keyes, C. (2005). Mental illness and/or mental health? Investigating anxioms of the complete state model of health. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 73, 539–548. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.73.3.539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keyes, C. (2006). Mental health in adolescence: is America’s youth flourishing? Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 76, 395–402. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.76.3.395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keyes, C. (2009). The Black-White paradox in health: flourishing in the face of social inequality and discrimination. J. Pers. 77, 1677–1706. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00597.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leucht, S., Corves, C., Arbter, D., Engel, R. R., Li, C., and Davis, J. M. (2009). Second-generation versus first-generation antipsychotic drugs for schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Lancet 373, 31–41. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61764-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacCallum, R. C., Widaman, K. F., Zhang, S., and Hong, S. (1999). Sample size in factor analysis. Psychol. Methods 4, 84–99. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.4.1.84

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGaffin, B., Deane, F., Kelly, P., and Ciarrochi, J. (2015). Flourishing, languishing and moderate mental health: prevalence and change in mental health during recovery from drug and alcohol problems. Addict. Res. Theory 23, 351–360. doi: 10.3109/16066359.2015.1019346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKay, R., Langdon, R., and Coltheart, M. (2006). The persecutory ideation questionnaire. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 194, 628–631. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000231441.48007.a5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melo, S., Corcoran, R., Shryane, N., and Bentall, R. P. (2009). The persecution and deservedness scale. Psychol. Psychother. Theory Res. Pract. 82, 247–260. doi: 10.1348/147608308x398337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., Andreou, C., Schneider, B. C., Wittekind, C. E., Menon, M., Balzan, R. P., et al. (2014a). Sowing the seeds of doubt: a narrative review on metacognitive training in schizophrenia. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 34, 358–366. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2014.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., Veckenstedt, R., Andreou, C., Bohn, F., Hottenrott, B., Leighton, L., et al. (2014b). Sustained and “Sleeper” effects of group metacognitive training for schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 1103–1111.

Google Scholar

Moritz, S., Kolbeck, K., and Andreou, C. (2016). Antipsychotics decrease response confidence. J. Psychopharmacol. 30, 831–833. doi: 10.1177/0269881116650404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., and Lysaker, P. H. (2018). Metacognition – what did James H. Flavell really say and the implications for the conceptualization and design of metacognitive interventions. Schizophr. Res. 201, 20–26. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2018.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., Veckenstedt, R., Bohn, F., Hottenrott, B., Scheu, F., Randjbar, S., et al. (2013). Complementary group metacognitive training (MCT) reduces delusional ideation in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 151, 61–69. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., and Woodward, T. (2007). Metacognitive training in schizophrenia: from basic research to knowledge translation and intervention. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 20, 619–625. doi: 10.1097/yco.0b013e3282f0b8ed

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., and Woodward, T. S. (2005). Jumping to conclusions in delusional and non-delusional schizophrenic patients. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 44, 193–207. doi: 10.1348/014466505x35678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, A., Pyle, M., Chapman, N., French, P., Parker, S., and Wells, A. (2014). Metacognitive therapy in people with a schizophrenia spectrum diagnosis and medication resistant symptoms: a feasibility study. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 45, 280–284. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, A. P., Shryane, N., Fowler, D., Birchwood, M., Gumley, A. I., Taylor, H. E., et al. (2015). Negative cognition, affect, metacognition and dimensions of paranoia in people at ultra-high risk of psychosis: a multi-level modelling analysis. Psychol. Med. 45, 2675–2678.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Norman, R. M., Malla, A. K., McLean, T., Voruganti, L. P., Cortese, L., McIntosh, E., et al. (2000). The relationship of symptoms and level of functioning in schizophrenia to general wellbeing and the quality of life scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 102, 303–309. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2000.102004303.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Normann, N., van Emmerik, A., and Morina, N. (2014). The efficacy of metacognitive therapy for anxiety and depression: a meta-analytic review. Depress. Anxiety 31, 402–411. doi: 10.1002/da.22273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’connor, B. P. (2000). SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of components using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP test. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 32, 396–402. doi: 10.3758/bf03200807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penn, D., Corrigan, P., Bentall, R., Racenstein, J., and Newman, L. (1997). Social cognition in schizophrenia. Psychol. Bull. 121, 114–132.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 36, 717–731. doi: 10.3758/bf03206553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prouteau, A., Verdoux, H., Briand, C., Lesage, A., Lalonde, P., Nicole, L., et al. (2005). Cognitive predictors of psychosocial functioning outcome in schizophrenia: a follow-up study of subjects participating in a rehabilitation program. Schizophr. Res. 77, 343–353. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Carvajal, R., Díaz, D., Moreno-Jiménez, B., Blanco, A., and van Dierendonck, D. (2010). Vitality and inner resources as relevant components of psychological well-being. Psicothema 22, 63–70.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological wellbeing. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1069–1081. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2010.503835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrout, P., and Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol. Methods 7, 422–445. doi: 10.1037//1082-989x.7.4.422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S., and Brown, J. (1988). Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental healt. Psychol. Bull. 103, 193–210. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valiente, C., Cantero, D., Vázquez, C., Sanchez, Á, Provencio, M., and Espinosa, R. (2011). Implicit and explicit self-esteem discrepancies in paranoia and depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 120, 691–699. doi: 10.1037/a0022856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dierendonck, D., Díaz, D., Rodríguez-Carvajal, R., Blanco, A., and Moreno-Jiménez, B. (2008). Ryff’s six-factor model of psychological well-being, a Spanish exploration. Soc. Indic. Res. 87, 473–479. doi: 10.1007/s11205-007-9174-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, A. (1995). Meta-cognition and worry: a cognitive model of generalised anxiety disorder. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 23, 301–320. doi: 10.1017/s1352465800015897

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, A., and Matthews, G. (1996). Modelling cognition in emotional disorder: the S-REF model. Behav. Res. Ther. 34, 881–888. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(96)00050-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (1946). Official Records of the World Health Organization, Vol. 2. Geneva: World Health Organization, 100.

Google Scholar

Keywords: well-being, quality of life, paranoid thinking, doubt, Complete State Model of Health

Citation: Asensio-Aguerri L, Beato-Fernández L, Stavraki M, Rodríguez-Cano T, Bajo M and Díaz D (2019) Paranoid Thinking and Wellbeing. The Role of Doubt in Pharmacological and Metacognitive Therapies. Front. Psychol. 10:2099. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02099

Received: 09 April 2019; Accepted: 29 August 2019;
Published: 12 September 2019.

Edited by:

Andrew Kemp, Swansea University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Nekane Basabe Barañano, University of the Basque Country, Spain
Manuel Cárdenas, University of Valparaíso, Chile

Copyright © 2019 Asensio-Aguerri, Beato-Fernández, Stavraki, Rodríguez-Cano, Bajo and Díaz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Darío Díaz, dario.diaz@uclm.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.