Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 01 November 2019
Sec. Health Psychology

Commentary: Complex Motor Learning and Police Training: Applied, Cognitive, and Clinical Perspectives

  • 1Department of Police Science, University of Applied Sciences for Public Administration and Management, Gelsenkirchen, Germany
  • 2Department of Training Pedagogy and Martial Research, German Sport University Cologne, Cologne, Germany

A Commentary on
Complex Motor Learning and Police Training: Applied, Cognitive, and Clinical Perspectives

by Di Nota, P. M., and Huhta, J.-M. (2019). Front. Psychol. 10:1797. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01797

Di Nota and Huhta (2019) recently provided an informative overview concerning complex motor learning in the context of police training. Reflecting the emphasis on complex motor learning in the context of police training, there are a number of pedagogical practices and coaching decisions which are informed by this paper. As Di Nota and Huhta (2019) explain, that “police trainers generally agree that basic skills training should begin with learning the fundamentals, or component “chunks,” in order to develop proficiency, comfort and safety with a given technique” (p. 10). As such, the authors suggest first learning basic skills and techniques up to a proficient level before further develop expertise “through exposure to increasingly complex and demanding situations” (p. 10) and applying them in performance settings. Concerning this assumption of a linear and modular approach to training (Moy et al., 2015), we raise two issues informed by the ecological dynamics perspective on skill development (Araújo and Davids, 2011; Seifert et al., 2019) that may extend some of the suggestions of Di Nota and Huhta (2019). First, the problem of isolation in complex motor learning and second, the problem of neglecting representative task design before the utilization of complex scenario trainings. It is understandable, that article length restrictions may have impacted on the authors in providing the necessary details that we feel warrants additional mention. Hence, it is not our intention to take away from the quality work presented but rather to support its desired aims.

Firstly, from an applied coaching perspective, the issue of isolating parts of movements in order to integrate them later on in a more complex movement or isolating the movement from the performance context has been observed in the police training domain on various occasions (Cushion, 2018; Staller et al., 2019) and has been met with criticism particularly with regards to transfer (Cushion, 2018; Körner and Staller, 2018; Staller and Körner, in press). The proposition of Di Nota and Huhta (2019) of “chunking” movements in order to “facilitate gaining competence in smaller, more manageable units of information” (p. 2) builds on several implicit assumptions in accordance with Information Processing Theory (Schmidt et al., 2018), that are relevant to the field of policing: (a) isolation of performance components effectively contribute to skill development and transfer to the field, (b) the existence of an ideal movement solution, (c) their repetition with low degrees of variation, and (d) the learning process as a coaching centered endeavor with emphasis on prescriptive instruction and feedback. These assumptions are heavily challenged by the ecological dynamics framework (Araújo et al., 2017; Seifert et al., 2017, 2019), which started to gain traction in the sport (Correia et al., 2018) and policing domain (Körner and Staller, 2018). According to this perspective, skill learning better refers to the process of adapting and attuning to the environment, instead of reproducing an “ideal” technique” out of context (Araújo and Davids, 2011). Adopting this perspective leads to pedagogical approaches that facilitate the emergence of greater functional relationship between the learner, conceptualized as adaptive and self-organized system, and the performance environment instead of pursuing a perfect technique (Körner and Staller, 2018; Renshaw and Chow, 2018). By highlighting the importance of performance-environment coupling, perceptual-action coupling and consideration of the non-linearity of the learning process and performance in the field (Seifert et al., 2019), the ecological dynamics framework offers a useful perspective on skill development and transfer especially with regards to designing practice activities in general (Pinder et al., 2011; Krause et al., 2017) and in the law enforcement domain particularly (Staller et al., 2017; Körner and Staller, 2018). Furthermore, concerning the assumption of an ideal movement as a basis of the linear approach to learning, data has shown, that due to ongoing changes within individual and environmental constraints there is no repetition within a repetition on an intraindividual basis (Schöllhorn, 1999; Schöllhorn et al., 2012) and that movement solutions to specific problems at hand vary between individuals (Dicks et al., 2017). This applies even to non-complex movements (Bernstein, 1967). In the context of police training, the functional role of intra- and interindividual variability has recently been pointed out (Körner and Staller, 2018) and is backed up by current data highlighting the functionality of adaptive behavioral solutions in police self-defense task (Körner et al., 2019). Allowing and promoting functionality and variability of individual solutions for problems at hand is as much a pedagogical challenge for the coach as providing descriptive feedback and an external focus of attention.

The second issue relates to the actual coaching practice within the context of police training. Even though Di Nota and Huhta (2019) complement the need for performance-environment and perceptual-action coupling in learning tasks by referring to scenario-based training as the “gold standard for complex motor learning for police” (p. 10), we would argue, that this approach is too short-sighted for effective skill development in police training. Data from training observations in police training (Cushion, 2018; Staller et al., 2019) showed, that trainings tasks—except for scenario-based training—regularly seem to lack representative task design leading to a lack of transfer in more complex tasks later on. These data indicate that representatively designed tasks are employed in police training, but not in the beginning of training, when practice tasks regularly involve partner interaction on a one-to-one basis. The concept of representative task design (Pinder et al., 2014; Staller et al., 2017) may provide police trainers and coaches with the tools to practice the needed skills in an integrated manner (incorporating perception-action and performance-environment coupling) beyond scenario-based training. Police trainers should generally focus on representatively-designed, high-quality interactions of training partners right from the start (Staller and Körner, 2018) instead of constraining for disintegrated motor learning in the frame of isolated practices out of context in the beginning and only subsequently focusing on representative task design in the context of scenario-based training. This may be achieved by framing partner drills (even in the early stages of training) as representative simulations with clear roles and responsibilities providing the learner with the opportunity to act upon information variables offering situationally and gradually varying demands (physical, cognitive-perceptual, and affective) that are representative of the performance context allowing for the acquisition, emergence and attunement of functional skill. Furthermore, these interactions may also serve as a base unit for the analysis for the efficiency of the training process (Araújo and Davids, 2018) when evaluating police training.

Author Contributions

MS and SK authors contributed equally to the ideas presented. MS wrote the first draft of the paper. Both authors contributed equally to editing the first draft to its final version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Araújo, D., and Davids, K. (2011). What exactly is acquired during skill acquisition? J. Conscious. Stud. 18, 7–23.

Google Scholar

Araújo, D., and Davids, K. (2018). The (sport) performer-environment system as the base unit in explanations of expert performance. J. Expertise 1, 144–154.

Google Scholar

Araújo, D., Hristovski, R., Seifert, L., Carvalho, J., and Davids, K. (2017). Ecological cognition: expert decision-making behaviour in sport. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 38, 1–25. doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2017.1349826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, N. A. (1967). The Co-ordination and Regulation of Movements. Oxford: Pergamon.

Google Scholar

Correia, V., Carvalho, J., Araújo, D., Pereira, E., and Davids, K. (2018). Principles of nonlinear pedagogy in sport practice. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 24, 117–132. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2018.1552673

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cushion, C. J. (2018). Exploring the delivery of officer safety training: a case study. Policing 5, 1–15. doi: 10.1093/police/pax095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Nota, P. M., and Huhta, J.-M. (2019). Complex motor learning and police training: applied, cognitive, and clinical perspectives. Front. Psychol. 10:1797. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dicks, M., Button, C., Davids, K., Chow, J. Y., and van der Kamp, J. (2017). Keeping an eye on noisy movements: on different approaches to perceptual-motor skill research and training. Sports Med. 47, 575–581. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0600-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Körner, S., and Staller, M. S. (2018). From system to pedagogy: towards a nonlinear pedagogy of self-defense training in the police and the civilian domain. Secur. J. 31, 645–659. doi: 10.1057/s41284-017-0122-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Körner, S., Staller, M. S., and Kecke, A. (2019). “Geht da mehr? – Vergleich linearer und nicht-linearer Trainingspädagogik im Einsatztraining von Polizist*innen [Is there more? - Comparison of linear and non-linear training pedagogy in the police use of force training],” in Teaching is Learning: Methods, Contents and Role Models in the Didactics of Martial Arts - 8th Annual Symposium of the dvs Komission “Kampfkunst und Kampfsport”, eds M. Meyer and M. S. Staller (Vechta), 21–22.

Google Scholar

Krause, L., Farrow, D., Reid, M., Buszard, T., and Pinder, R. A. (2017). Helping coaches apply the principles of representative learning design: validation of a tennis specific practice assessment tool. J. Sports Sci. 5, 1–10. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2017.1374684

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moy, B., Renshaw, I., Davids, K., and Brymer, E. (2015). Overcoming acculturation: physical education recruits' experiences of an alternative pedagogical approach to games teaching. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 21, 386–406. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2015.1017455

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinder, R. A., Davids, K., Renshaw, I., and Araújo, D. (2011). Representative learning design and functionality of research and practice in sport. J. Sport Excerc. Psychol. 33, 146–155. doi: 10.1123/jsep.33.1.146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinder, R. A., Renshaw, I., Headrick, J., and Davids, K. (2014). “Skill acquisition and representative task design,” in Complex Systems in Sport, eds R. Hristovski, D. Araújo, N. Balagué Serre, C. Button, and P. Passos (London: Routledge), 319–333. Available online at: http://eprints.qut.edu.au/63712/

Google Scholar

Renshaw, I., and Chow, J. Y. (2018). A constraint-led approach to sport and physical education pedagogy. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 18, 1–14. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2018.1552676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, R., Lee, T., Winstein, C., Wulf, G., and Zelaznik, H. (2018). Motor Control and Learning. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Schöllhorn, W. (1999). Individualität - ein vernachlässigter Parameter? [Individuality - A neglected parameter?]. Leistungssport 2, 7–12.

Google Scholar

Schöllhorn, W., Hegen, P., and Davids, K. (2012). The nonlinear nature of learning - A differential learning approach. Open Sports Sci. J. 5, 100–112. doi: 10.2174/1875399X01205010100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seifert, L., Araújo, D., Komar, J., and Davids, K. (2017). Understanding constraints on sport performance from the complexity sciences paradigm: an ecological dynamics framework. Hum. Mov. Sci. 56A, 178–180. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2017.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seifert, L., Papet, V., Strafford, B. W., Coughlan, E. K., and Davids, K. (2019). Skill transfer, expertise and talent development: an ecological dynamics perspective. Mov. Sport Sci. 19, 705–711. doi: 10.1051/sm/2019010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staller, M. S., and Körner, S. (2018). Your partner makes you better: the partner interaction paradigm in conflict training. Mov. J. Phys. Educ. Sport Sci. 11, 169.

Google Scholar

Staller, M. S., and Körner, S. (in press). Training für den Einsatz II: Plädoyer gegen das Training isolierter Komponenten im polizeilichen Einsatztraining [Training for the field II: Plea against the training of isolated components in police use offorce training]. Die Polizei.

Google Scholar

Staller, M. S., Körner, S., Heil, V., Klemmer, I., and Kecke, A. (2019). “Die Trainingspraxis im polizeilichen Einsatztraining: Eine Fallstudie [Training practice in police use of force training: A case study],” in Abstracts of the 7th Annual Conference of the Committee for Martial Arts Studies in the German Association of Sport “Experiencing, Training and Thinking the Body in Martial Arts and Martial Sports,” November 15-17, 2018, Ghent, Belgium, ed A. Niehaus, 32.

Google Scholar

Staller, M. S., Zaiser, B., and Körner, S. (2017). From realism to representativeness: changing terminology to investigate effectiveness in self-defence. Mart. Arts Stud. 4, 70–77. doi: 10.18573/j.2017.10187

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: police, police training, complex motor learning, coaching (performance), ecological dynamics

Citation: Staller MS and Körner S (2019) Commentary: Complex Motor Learning and Police Training: Applied, Cognitive, and Clinical Perspectives. Front. Psychol. 10:2444. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02444

Received: 05 September 2019; Accepted: 15 October 2019;
Published: 01 November 2019.

Edited by:

Hyemin Han, University of Alabama, United States

Reviewed by:

Peter Renden, The Hague University of Applied Sciences, Netherlands

Copyright © 2019 Staller and Körner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mario S. Staller, mario.staller@fhoev.nrw.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.