Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 13 May 2020
Sec. Cognition

Near-Death Experience Memories Include More Episodic Components Than Flashbulb Memories

  • 1Coma Science Group, GIGA-Consciousness, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
  • 2Centre du Cerveau2 (Centre Intégré Pluridisciplinaire de l’Étude du Cerveau, de la Cognition et de la Conscience), University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
  • 3GIGA-CRC In vivo Imaging, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium

Memories of near-death experiences (NDEs) are recalled as “realer” than memories of other real or imagined events. Given their rich phenomenology, emotionality and consequentiality, it was hypothesized that they could meet some aspects of the definition of flashbulb memories. We aimed to identify and compare the episodic and non-episodic information provided in verbal recollections of NDE, flashbulb, and control autobiographical memories. The phenomenological characteristics and centrality of the memories were also compared. Twenty-five participants who had lived a NDE in a life-threatening situation were interviewed and completed the Memory Characteristics Questionnaires as well as the Centrality of Event Scale for their NDE, a flashbulb and another autobiographical memory used as control. Overall, transcribed NDE verbal recollections included a higher overall amount of details and more internal/episodic information than control autobiographical and flashbulb memories. Moreover, flashbulb memories were associated to a lower intensity of feelings while remembering and a lower personal importance, and are less reactivated and less susceptible to be remembered from a first person perspective compared to NDE and control autobiographical memories. Finally, NDE memories are the most central memories to experiencers’ identity, followed by control autobiographical and then by flashbulb memories. These findings corroborate previous studies highlighting the impact and uniqueness of NDE memories.

Introduction

Near-death experiences (NDEs) have been defined as psychological events usually occurring when people are close to death or in situations of intense danger (Greyson, 2000b). These subjective experiences contain some typical and recurrent features such as the vision of a bright light, out-of-body experiences (OBEs) or coming to a border or a point of no return (Greyson, 1983; Martial et al., 2017a; Cassol et al., 2018). Long regarded as taboo and out of reach for science, the scientific study of the phenomenon emerged only four decades ago but recently led to an increasing number of empirical research (e.g., Van Lommel et al., 2001; Palmieri et al., 2014; Martial et al., 2018, 2019a,b). NDEs are of particular clinical relevance given their frequency of appearance (i.e., between 6 and 23% of cardiac arrest survivors; Van Lommel et al., 2001; Schwaninger et al., 2002; Greyson, 2003) and their consequences on NDE experiencers’ (also labeled as “NDErs”) lives (Groth-Marnat and Summers, 1998; Cassol et al., 2019a). Numerous aftereffects have been reported, such as the development of a higher spirituality, less materialistic values or a reduced fear of death (Noyes, 1980; Groth-Marnat and Summers, 1998; Knoblauch et al., 2001).

Previous studies have highlighted the uniqueness of NDE memories within autobiographical memory (i.e., memories about an individual’s life; Williams et al., 2008). NDE memories were found to contain more details (e.g., sensory, emotional, and self-referential) than memories of other real and imagined events, and memories of a period of coma or impaired consciousness following an acquired severe brain dysfunction without NDE (Thonnard et al., 2013). Thus, it was suggested that they could not be considered as imagined event memories. Other studies have found that richer NDEs (i.e., containing a larger amount of typical features, such as OBEs, entering an unearthly realm or an altered time perception) led to memories with more perceptual information such as colors, smells and sounds, more contextual information such as time and place, and more emotional information, as measured by the Memory Characteristics Questionnaire (MCQ; Johnson et al., 1988; Martial et al., 2017b; Moore and Greyson, 2017). At first sight, the important amount of phenomenological details contained in their recollections might seem counter-intuitive given that NDEs are assumed to occur during an altered state of consciousness and are classically associated with a critical or confused state (e.g., Van Lommel et al., 2001). In an effort to explain the special nature of NDE memories, different theories have been put forth. It was suggested that the significant emotionality associated with these mental events could enhance the quantity of sensory details stored in memory (Schaefer and Philippot, 2005). Besides, emotional events are more likely to be frequently rehearsed, therefore potentially increasing the amount of memorized characteristics (Thonnard et al., 2013).

Given their emotionality and consequentiality (i.e., the consequences of the event or aftermath; e.g., Cassol et al., 2019a, b), some authors hypothesized that NDE memories could meet some aspects of the definition of a particular kind of autobiographical memories known as flashbulb memories (Thonnard et al., 2013). Flashbulb memories refer to very vivid and long-lasting memories of the circumstances in which one learned about a shocking public event (Brown and Kulik, 1977). Studies on flashbulb memories have been focusing on landmark events such as the September 11 terrorist attacks or the death/assassination of famous people (Finkenauer et al., 1998; Curci et al., 2001; Tinti et al., 2009). A prominent feature of these memories is that they display high ratings of belief in accuracy/subjective certainty (Brown and Kulik, 1977; Kraha and Boals, 2014) and that people can usually remember some of their personal aspects for several decades (Berntsen and Thomsen, 2005), for example the place they were when they heard the news, who they were with, the clothes they were wearing, or the emotions felt at the time of the event (Luminet and Curci, 2009). Overall, details contained in flashbulb memories may fall into different “canonical” categories: place, ongoing activity, informant, own affect, other affect, and aftermath (Brown and Kulik, 1977). The level of consequentiality associated with flashbulb memories and the underlying memory processes, however, remain debated. Pioneers in the area have suggested that these memories do not imply the existence of a special memory mechanism and include the notion of consequentiality as a key element in the formation of these memories, along with novelty, surprise, emotionality and overt rehearsal [i.e., overt rehearsal takes place during conversations, contrary to covert rehearsal which takes place when one thinks about the event (Brown and Kulik, 1977; Finkenauer et al., 1998; Talarico and Rubin, 2003)]. However, results from other studies suggest different underlying memory processes for memories of learning about public news (such as flashbulb memories) and memories for private events (i.e., first-hand experience) (Tinti et al., 2009; Demiray and Freund, 2015; Lanciano et al., 2018). For example, first-hand experiences would show a higher consequentiality and life impact, and would have a protective function because their content is related to the individual’s personal identity and well-being (Pillemer, 2009).

Generally speaking, narratives of autobiographical memories are known to be multifaceted in content and to include both episodic (i.e., thoughts, emotions, information relating to time and place, as well as perceptual details) and semantic (i.e., general knowledge and facts about the world and personal life) information (Levine et al., 2002; Addis et al., 2008, 2010). Given that episodic and semantic memory are thought to rely on different neural processes (Moscovitch et al., 2005), Levine et al. (2002) have developed an Autobiographical Memory Interview (AI) which includes a reliable coding scheme enabling the distinction between episodic and non-episodic details comprised in verbalized autobiographical memories. In their AI coding scheme, episodic details (scored by using categories adapted from the MCQ) refer to the central event and its emotional, perceptual, temporal and spatial characteristics, and non-episodic details relate to repetitions, metacognitive statements, and information that does not pertain to the main event or that is not contextually bounded, such as general knowledge or facts. While the former seem essential for high-fidelity representation of personally experienced events, the latter rather contribute to the coherence and continuity of self-knowledge and identity through time (Levine et al., 2002). To date, the AI scoring scheme has been used in a wide range of studies related to memory processes in aging (e.g., Levine et al., 2002), psychiatric conditions (e.g., Moscovitch et al., 2018), neurodegeneration (e.g., Bastin et al., 2013) or lesion cases (e.g., Steinvorth et al., 2005). However, no study has yet investigated the episodic and semantic composition of NDE verbal recollections as well as the comparison with those of flashbulb memories. Therefore, the primary aim of this study was to (1) assess whether the episodic and non-episodic information comprised in verbal recollections of NDE, flashbulb and control autobiographical memories differ and (2) further compare their phenomenological characteristics using the sMCQ (a short version of the MCQ; Johnson et al., 1988; D’Argembeau and Van der Linden, 2008) and their consequentiality by means of the Centrality of Event Scale (CES; Berntsen and Rubin, 2006).

Materials and Methods

Participants

NDErs were recruited via the websites, appearances in local media and publications of the International Association for Near-Death Studies (IANDS France), the Coma Science Group (University of Liège, Belgium). Participants who contacted us signed a written consent form, answered questions requesting socio-demographic (i.e., gender and age at interview) as well as clinical information (i.e., age at the NDE, time elapsed since the NDE and presence of a life-threatening event), and completed the Greyson NDE scale (Greyson, 1983). This 16-item multiple-choice validated scale enables a standardized identification of a NDE (Greyson, 1983; Lange et al., 2004) as well as the quantification of its intensity (i.e., total score ranging from 0 to 32). For each of the 16 items, the scores are arranged on an ordinal scale ranging from 0 to 2 (i.e., 0 = not present, 1 = mildly or ambiguously present, and 2 = definitively present).

Ultimately, we included 25 participants whose experience had been lived in a life-threatening situation and who met the accepted criteria of a NDE (i.e., Greyson NDE scale’s total score ≥ 7). The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Liège.

Experimental Task

Screening Tasks

The first step of the face-to-face interview consisted of a screening assessment in order to detect and exclude memory biases related to negative mood as well as mild cognitive impairments.

Regarding negative mood, the 25 NDErs completed the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988). The PANAS consists of 20 emotion words, with 10 loading on the Positive Affect factor and 10 on the Negative Affect one. This scale assesses participants’ baseline mood levels. The positive subscale (PANAS+) includes words such as “alert,” “inspired,” or “enthusiastic,” whereas the negative subscale (PANAS−) includes words such as “distressed,” “upset,” or “guilty.” Participants rate the degree to which they endorse each item on a rating scale ranging from 1 = very slightly or not at all to 5 = extremely. Items are then summed to create a score for each factor. Higher scores represent greater endorsement of the construct.

Potential mild cognitive impairments were assessed using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA; Nasreddine et al., 2005). This scale includes 30 items assessing multiple cognitive domains: short-term memory (5 points); visuospatial abilities via clock drawing (3 points) and a cube copy task (1 point); executive functioning via an adaptation of Trail Making Test Part B (1 point), phonemic fluency (1 point) and verbal abstraction (2 points); attention, concentration and working memory via target detection (1 point), serial subtraction (3 points), digits forward (1 point) and digits backward (1 point); language via confrontation naming with low-familiarity animals (3 points) and repetition of complex sentences (2 points); and orientation to time and place (6 points). Items are summed to create a score and the clinical cut-off score is equal to 26.

Autobiographical Memory Interview (Levine et al., 2002)

Once participants were screened for mood and for memory impairments, the interviews were carried out based on the probing and scoring recommendations of the Autobiographical Memory Interview (AI) by Levine et al. (2002). We conducted a semi-structured interview in a quiet room for each participant. Specifically, participants were asked to freely describe in considerable detail three target memories dating (without restriction regarding the recall time), as far as possible, to the same period: (1) the memory of their NDE, (2) a flashbulb memory (they were provided with a definition and examples; i.e., memories of the circumstances in which one first learned of a very surprising and consequential or emotionally arousing event such as September 11 terrorist attacks), and (3) an autobiographical memory. Each event had to be temporally and contextually specific, that is, occurring over minutes or hours but not exceeding a day. The order in which the memories were to be recalled was randomized. According to the AI guidelines, two further levels of recall may be needed in case of a lack of key information: (1) general and (2) specific probing. General probes are non-specific cues aiming to encourage full description (e.g., “Can you tell me more about it?”). Specific probes cue precise aspects of the event in order to determine if participants remember more information (e.g., “Where did this event take place?”). Nevertheless, no specific probe was employed in the present study due to substantial recalls by our participants. Narratives were audio-recorded, transcribed and analyzed using the established manual scoring procedure, allowing to separate episodic details (i.e., description of the event, sensory or mental state details specific to the event; “internal details”) from non-episodic details (i.e., semantic or factual statements, or other details not specific to the event; “external details”) (Levine et al., 2002).

Questionnaires

Finally, after each memory recall, participants completed a short version of the Memory Characteristics Questionnaire (sMCQ; D’Argembeau and Van der Linden, 2008; adapted from Johnson et al., 1988) and the Centrality of Event Scale (CES; Berntsen and Rubin, 2006). The former assesses memory clarity, sensory details, self-referential and emotional information, reactivation frequency, and confidence in the memory by means of 16 items (each rated on a 1–7 point Likert scale). The latter is a 20-item instrument that measures how central an event is to a person’s identity and life story. All items are rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = totally disagree to 5 = totally agree. The three groups of items provide an assessment of whether an event has become a reference point for the generation of expectations to other events in the respondent’s life story, whether an event is considered as a central component of the person’s identity, and whether the event is considered as a turning point in one’s life story, respectively.

Analysis

Data analyses were carried out using Jamovi statistical software (1.1.9.0).

Study Sample

Greyson NDE scale total scores, age at NDE and age at interview are summarized as mean ± standard deviation (SD). Distributions being skewed, results at the PANAS and the MoCA are summarized as median (inter-quartile range; IQR), and differences for time since event (NDE vs. flashbulb vs. autobiographical) were analyzed using Friedman tests and expressed as median (IQR) and average rank.

Autobiographical Memory Interview

Transcribed recalls were segmented into “details,” or “segments.” A detail is defined as “a unique occurrence, observation, or thought, generally expressed as a grammatical clause” (Levine et al., 2002). A segment can be defined as a sentence or part of a sentence that conveys information. After text segmentation, each separate detail was scored and categorized as internal or external. Internal details pertain to the main event and are episodic in nature, whereas external details are not directly related to the main event and may correspond to semantic information. Internal details are subdivided into: (1) event details, which describe the unfolding of the story (e.g., happenings, persons involved, reactions/emotions of oneself or other people, one’s clothing, the weather); (2) the time (e.g., life epoch, day, month, year, season, and clock time); (3) the place (i.e., any information that involves localization in space, such as a city, a street, a building, a room); (4) perceptual details (i.e., visual, olfactory, tactile, gustatory, auditory information, proprioceptive, or nociceptive information); and (5) thoughts and emotions (relating to the mental state of the subject). Apart from semantic information, repetitions of information and other unrelated details (i.e., time, place, event, perceptual and emotional details that do not directly pertain to the main event) are classified as external. Two independent raters (HC and EB) analyzed each transcribed recall by scoring each detail. Scoring reliability was assessed using intra-class correlation (ICC) coefficients, showing that both raters scored the narratives in a highly reliable manner (ICCinternal details = 0.99; ICCexternal details = 0.98). Residual discrepancies between raters were discussed later on to reach a final classification for all details. Based on this final classification, all subtypes of details generated spontaneously were summed across memories.

For each type of memory, the measures of interest were the overall amount of (1) internal and (2) external details. First, we looked at potential associations between the number of reported details and demographic variables (i.e., age at NDE, age at event and time since event) using Pearson’s correlations, as well as between these details and the MoCA total scores using Spearman’s correlations given that they were not normally distributed. Second, we looked at the difference in the amount of internal and external details reported for each type of memory. Variables being normally distributed, differences between groups were assessed using two-ways repeated measures analysis of variance (rmANOVAs), with the type of details (interval vs. external) and the type of memory (NDE vs. flashbulb vs. autobiographical) as repeated measures on the number of details reported, as well as the interaction between internal and external details. In case of violation of sphericity, we used a Greenhouse–Geisser correction. Results were considered significant at p = 0.05. In case of significant results, we performed post hoc comparisons using Tukey HSD, setting the level of significance at 0.017 after Bonferroni correction. Regarding additional measures, we looked at the effect of the memory type on the amount of subtypes of internal and external memory details (i.e., event, perceptual, emotional and semantic details, as well as time, place and repetitions). Distributions being skewed, differences between groups were assessed using Friedman tests and results were considered significant at p = 0.004 after Bonferroni correction. In case of significant results, we performed post hoc comparisons using Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, setting the level of significance at 0.017 after Bonferroni correction.

Questionnaires

CES total scores were analyzed using an rmANOVA. Answers at the sMCQ being ordinal data measures, memories (NDE vs. flashbulb vs. autobiographical) were compared using a Friedman test, setting the level of significance at 0.003 after Bonferroni correction. In case of significant results, we performed post hoc comparisons using Tukey HSD tests for rmANOVAs and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests for Friedman tests, setting the level of significance at 0.017 after Bonferroni correction. Additionally, we examined associative strength between CES total scores and reported details (i.e., internal and external score) as well as sMCQ items using Spearman’s correlations.

Results

Study Sample

Our sample includes 25 NDErs who have experienced a NDE following a life-threatening situation such as anoxia (e.g., cardiac arrest, near-drowning; n = 8), trauma (e.g., motor vehicle accident, falls; n = 6) or other events (non-traumatic events such as complication during surgery; n = 11). Participants’ negative mood was not higher than normal (i.e., as assessed by the PANAS) and they are all above the cut-off score at the MoCA (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical information for NDErs (n = 25; 10 females).

All participants were able to recall a flashbulb memory and an autobiographical memory that happened around the same time as the NDE. Recalled memories did not differ regarding the number of years elapsed since the event (medianNDE = 27, IQR = 10–45, average rank = 2.24; medianFlashbulb = 17, IQR = 15–27, average rank = 1.68; medianAutobiographical = 26, IQR = 12–42, average rank = 2.08; [X2(2) = 4.622, p = 0.099, kendall coefficient = 0.092]). Recalled flashbulb memories were the following: the assassination of J. F. Kennedy (n = 1), the first steps on the moon (n = 5), the fall of the Berlin wall (n = 2), death of King Baudouin of Belgium (n = 3), death of Princess Diana (n = 1), September 11 terrorist attacks (n = 8), Liège shooting attack (n = 4), and Paris terrorist attacks (n = 1). Demographic and clinical information of the entire sample can be found in Table 1.

Autobiographical Memory Interview

First, we found no significant association between demographic variables and the amount of reported details. Precisely, the age at interview was not correlated with the amount of internal details comprised in NDE (r = −0.188, p = 0.367), flashbulb (r = −0.063, p = 0.764) and autobiographical (r = 0.175, p = 0.402) memories, as well as the amount of external details of NDE (r = 0.051, p = 0.808) flashbulb (r = −0.230, p = 0.270) and autobiographical memories (r = 0.055, p = 0.794). The age at event was not correlated with the amount of internal details comprised in NDE (r = −0.292, p = 0.156), flashbulb (r = 0.243, p = 0.242) and autobiographical memories (r = −0.077, p = 0.715), as well as the amount of external details of NDE (r = 0.051, p = 0.808) flashbulb (r = −0.126, p = 0.548) and autobiographical memories (r = 0.024, p = 0.910). Finally, the time since event was not correlated with the amount of internal details comprised in NDE (r = 0.174, p = 0.405), flashbulb (r = −0.297, p = 0.149) and autobiographical memories (r = 0.199, p = 0.340), as well as the amount of external details of NDE (r = 0.006, p = 0.977), flashbulb (r = −0.037, p = 0.861) and autobiographical memories (r = −0.283, p = 0.171).

Second, we found no significant association between the MoCA total scores and the amount of internal details comprised in NDE (rho = −0.334, p = 0.103), flashbulb (r = −0.276, p = 0.183), and autobiographical (rho = 0.061, p = 0.771), memories, as well as the amount of external details of NDE (r = −0.378, p = 0.062) flashbulb (r = 0.369, p = 0.188), and autobiographical memories (r = −0.199, p = 0.341).

Finally, the rmANOVA revealed a main effect of the type of detail [F(1,24) = 32.4, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.575], NDErs recalled a higher overall amount of internal details (M = 29.1, SD = 26) compared to external ones (M = 13.4, SD = 11.8). We also found a main effect of the type of memory [F(1.22,29.35) = 28.2, p > 0.001, ηp2 = 0.54]. The overall amount of details was higher for the NDE (M = 36.4, SD = 30.2) compared to the flashbulb (M = 12.5, SD = 8.47; t = 6.813, p = 0.001) and the autobiographical memory (M = 14.8, SD = 8.74; t = −6.151, p < 0.001), which did not differ from each other (t = 0.662, p = 0.786). Lastly, the interaction between the type of detail and the type of memory was also significant [F(1.42,34.10) = 19.3, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.445]. Specifically, the number of internal details reported for NDE memories (M = 53, SD = 32.7) is higher than the internal details reported for flashbulb (M = 16, SD = 8.90; t = 8.660, p < 0.001) and autobiographical memories (M = 18.2, SD = 5.29; t = −8.127, p < 0.001), and higher than the amount of external details reported for NDE (M = 19.8, SD = 14.6; t = 8.420, p < 0.001), flashbulb (M = 9.08, SD = 6.53; t = 9.402, p < 0.001) and autobiographical memories (M = 11.4, SD = 10.02; t = 8.897, p < 0.001). The other conditions did not significantly differ (all ps > 0.14).

Differences in episodic and non-episodic subcomponents of NDE, flashbulb and autobiographical memories can be found in Table 2. Results indicated a main effect of the memory type on the amount of event and perceptual internal details (see Table 2). Specifically, post hoc comparisons indicated that NDE memories contained significantly more internal event details than autobiographical (V = 12, p < 0.001) and flashbulb memories (V = 19, p < 0.001). Autobiographical and flashbulb memories did not differ (V = 80, p = 0.223). NDE memories also showed higher ratings for internal perceptual details as compared to autobiographical (V = 3.5, p < 0.001) and flashbulb memories (V = 246, p < 0.001). Once again, autobiographical and flashbulb memories did not differ (V = 54.5, p = 0.775). No other significant difference was observed. Although not significant when considering Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, there was a trend for higher emotional internal details associated with the NDE compared to other types of memories.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Episodic/internal and non-episodic/external subcomponents of NDE (n = 25), flashbulb (n = 25), and autobiographical memories (n = 25).

Questionnaires

Short Memory Characteristics Questionnaire

Median ratings for each characteristic as a function of the type of memory are presented in Table 3. Results indicated a between-memory difference in the (1) feeling of mentally reliving the event, (2) sensation of feeling the emotions felt during the event while remembering, (3) visual perspective taken while remembering, (4) emotions felt at the time of the event (i.e., valence), (5) personal importance attached to the event, and (6) reactivation frequency. Memories did not differ in the amount of sensory details, clarity (i.e., time, location and coherence), confidence in the memory as well as remembering one’s own actions/words/thoughts. Post hoc comparisons indicated that the feeling of mentally reliving the event is higher in autobiographical memories as compared to flashbulb memories (V = 187, p = 0.002). NDE memories did not differ from autobiographical (V = 32, p = 0.361) and flashbulb memories (V = 144, p = 0.049). The sensation of feeling the emotions felt during the event while remembering is higher in NDE and autobiographical memories compared to flashbulb memories (V = 192, p = 0.001 and V = 20, p < 0.001, respectively), but did not differ between NDE and autobiographical memories (V = 37.5, p = 0.109). Regarding visual perspective, flashbulb memories showed lower scores than NDE (V = 162.5, p < 0.001) and autobiographical memories (V = 4.5, p < 0.001) indicating that they were less susceptible to be remembered from a first-person perspective. Difference between NDE and autobiographical memories did not reach significance (V = 24, p = 0.438). Flashbulb memories also showed lower scores for the valence as compared to NDE (V = 207.5, p < 0.001) and autobiographical memories (V = 232, p = 0.001; NDE and autobiographical memories did not differ, V = 74.5, p = 0.171), indicating that the latter were more positive in average. NDE and autobiographical memories also scored higher than flashbulb memories in terms of personal importance (V = 250, p < 0.001 and V = 7.5, p < 0.001, respectively). On the other hand, difference between NDE and autobiographical memories did not reach significance (V = 30.5, p = 0.307). Finally, NDE and autobiographical memories were more frequently shared and reactivated than flashbulb memories (V = 211.5, p < 0.001 and V = 194, p < 0.001, respectively). Once again, difference between NDE and autobiographical memories did not reach significance (V = 73.5, p = 0.793).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Score differences between NDE (n = 25), flashbulb (n = 25), and autobiographical (n = 25) memories on the sMCQ.

Centrality of Event Scale

There was a significant effect of the memory type on the reported centrality [F(2,48) = 48.256, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.668]. Post hoc comparisons indicated that the mean score for the NDE memory (M = 81.4, SD = 15.767) was significantly higher than for the autobiographical memory (M = 62, SD = 23.429) [F(1,24) = 12.767, p = 0.002] as well as for the flashbulb memory (M = 35.36, SD = 12.783) [F(1,24) = 115.525, p < 0.001]. Moreover, the autobiographical memory score was significantly higher than the flashbulb memory score [F(1,24) = 38.149, p < 0.001]. Additionally, a significant positive correlation was found between CES scores and the number of reported internal details (rs = 0.512, p < 0.001), but not with the number of external details (rs = 0.208, p = 0.073).

Finally, we found a significant correlation between CES scores and the feeling of reexperiencing the event, the feeling of reexperiencing the emotions felt at the time of the event, the importance of the event and its reactivation frequency (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Association between CES total scores and sMCQ items.

Discussion

Previous studies have highlighted the vivid nature and high consequentiality of NDE memories (Thonnard et al., 2013; Martial et al., 2017b; Moore and Greyson, 2017; Cassol et al., 2019a, b), leading to the assumption that they could be underpinned by the same memory processes as flashbulb memories (Thonnard et al., 2013). Therefore, the aim of this study was to adopt a more fine-grained approach to highlight and compare the episodic and non-episodic contributions to the recall of NDE, flashbulb and control autobiographical events that occurred around the same time. To do so, we used the AI which provides reliable and valid indices of episodic and semantic contributions to personal remote memories.

First, analysis of verbal recollections highlighted that NDE memories include a higher overall amount of details and a higher amount of internal/episodic details than flashbulb and autobiographical memories. This is consistent with previous studies suggesting a particularly high amount of qualitative characteristics associated with NDE memories in comparison with other types of memories (e.g., Thonnard et al., 2013; Moore and Greyson, 2017). Moreover, the difference between the number of internal and external details for NDE memories is significantly larger than the one observed for flashbulb and autobiographical memories. The richness and high amount of overall details (and particularly internal/episodic information) delivered for NDE memories, compared to other more or less notable memories dating from the same period, could be explained by various factors. First, a NDE itself is characterized by an exceptional and unique association between unusual perceptions (such as the impression of leaving the physical body, entering a different dimension or being in an unknown spatio-temporal dimension) and a clear sensorium and intense sensation of “reality” (i.e., comparable to the sense of certainty that accompanies everyday perception; Schwaninger et al., 2002; Dell’Olio, 2010), conditions that are likely to lead to vivid memories. Second, previous studies have highlighted a tendency toward a decline in memory reports (normal forgetting process) in flashbulb memories in comparison with other emotionally arousing events (e.g., Christianson, 1989; Christianson and Engelberg, 1999). Consistency and amplification of memories over time would be conditioned by one’s degree of involvement and the severity of the emotionally arousing event (Neisser et al., 1996; van Giezen et al., 2005). In line with this view, studies including participants who were directly exposed to or personally involved in traumatic events identified different memory patterns than studies addressing flashbulb memories, in which subjects were not personally involved in the emotional event. In fact, some studies have even shown that unlike the emotionally charged traumatic events whose narratives become richer over time, flashbulb memories rather tend to decline (Krinsley et al., 2003; van Giezen et al., 2005). When looking closer at the subtypes of details that have been recalled, we identified more event and perceptual internal details within the NDE memory, as compared to flashbulb and autobiographical memories. Specifically, event details describe the unfolding of the story, and perceptual details comprise auditory, olfactory, tactile, taste, visual and spatial-temporal (allocentric-egocentric space, body position and duration) information. Some authors suggested that the overall quantity of details comprised in NDE memories could be due to the fact that self-referential information can improve recalling performances (Conway and Dewhurst, 1995). Specifically, self-referential information would enhance the encoding process, the organization in memory, as well as the enrichment of the event by extended knowledge (Conway and Dewhurst, 1995). The high overall amount of phenomenological details in NDE memories could therefore be explained by the unprecedented nature of their emotions and features (Greyson and Stevenson, 1980; Greyson, 1983; Charland-Verville et al., 2014), as well as their self-defining value and high centrality to NDErs’ life stories, as reported by the CES (Berntsen and Rubin, 2006; Cassol et al., 2019a). From a broader cognitive perspective, the high amount of details observed in verbal recollections of NDE memories is interesting as it suggests that people might be able to recall memories of a moment characterized by an altered state of consciousness where the brain and its associated processes are thought to be working with altered capacities. Nevertheless, it is still unclear when exactly these events are experienced as well as when their memory encoding precisely occurs.

Second, we compared the phenomenological characteristics (i.e., sMCQ scores) as well as the consequentiality and centrality (i.e., CES scores) of NDE, flashbulb and autobiographical memories. The analyses revealed that sMCQ scores related to NDEs and autobiographical memories were higher than those of flashbulb memories regarding the sensation of feeling the emotions felt during the event while remembering, their importance and their reactivation frequency. Moreover, the feeling of mentally reliving the event was higher for the autobiographical memory compared to the flashbulb memory. The finding that NDEs are emotionally highly charged and constitute an important part of NDErs’ life story is in line previous reports (e.g., Greyson, 1997; Bianco et al., 2017; Cassol et al., 2019a). On the contrary, the high scores of autobiographical memories might seem unexpected but could find explanation in the fact that some of them were somewhat connected to the NDE. Indeed, NDErs have lived their NDE several decades ago in average and reported difficulties when having to recall another event from the same period. From then on, recalled autobiographical memories were events closely linked to their NDE/coma (i.e., return to their daily lives after a long hospitalization, recovery after their coma) or significant/life-changing events such as a child birth or their encounter with their spouse (i.e., categories of memories that have typically been reported as self-defining in previous studies; e.g., Cassol et al., 2019a). Furthermore, regarding reactivation frequency, it has been shown that shocking memories of personal events are more thought and talked about than news reception memories such as flashbulb events (Pillemer, 2009). Finally, we also noted significant differences regarding the perspective taken during recollection. Autobiographical memories can either be retrieved from the first person perspective, in which the experience is visualized through one’s own eyes, or from the third person perspective, in which the experience is seen through an observer’s eyes (Nigro and Neisser, 1983). While participants tended to see the scene from their own perspective (field perspective) when recalling their NDE and their autobiographical memory, it was less the case for flashbulb memories for which they were more likely to see themselves from an observer’s perspective. Adopted visual perspectives are influenced by different variables and appear to be the consequence of contextual as well as dispositional factors, and/or their interaction (Nigro and Neisser, 1983; Sutin and Robins, 2008). These findings are consistent with previous research (Robinson and Swanson, 1993; Sutin and Robins, 2008) highlighting that first-person memories are generally rated higher on the phenomenological dimensions related to the reliving of a memory. Moreover, it has been shown that the adoption of a third-person perspective could be an avoidance strategy set up to distance the individual from a memory and reduce its emotional intensity (Kenny and Bryant, 2007; Sutin and Robins, 2008). Therefore, the difference in the adopted visual perspective could be due to the emotions felt at the time of the event (i.e., valence), that are overall very positive in NDE as well as in autobiographical memories and more negative in case of flashbulb events. Regarding centrality, answers to the CES revealed that NDE memories had the higher scores, followed by autobiographical memories and then by flashbulb memories. These findings corroborate previous findings and confirm the particular status of NDEs with regard to NDErs’ identities and life stories (Cassol et al., 2019a). The lower scores associated with flashbulb memories highlight the distinction between the consequences that one predicts when hearing about a surprising event and its actual impact on one’s life. These results are consistent with previous research on hearing shocking public events that have shown lower life impact, consequentiality, and personal importance (Pillemer, 2009). In addition, we found that the amount of episodic details provided upon recall, the feeling of reexperiencing the event while remembering, the feeling of reexperiencing the emotions felt at the time of the event and the reactivation frequency of the event in memory were all associated to the reported centrality of the event, regardless of the memory type. It is therefore reasonable to hypothesize that central/landmark events could benefit from a preferential encoding in autobiographical memory and lead to a richer phenomenology.

Finally, we would like to mention limitations to this study. NDErs voluntarily contacted us after calls in the lay media and our sample consequently may suffer from a self-selection bias. Moreover, given the relative scarcity of NDEs and the fact that we had to meet participants personally in accordance to the AI protocol, we were limited in the recruitment of our participants and our sample is relatively small. Therefore, results should be confirmed by larger studies. Besides, human memory is by definition a reconstructive process (Schacter et al., 1998) sensitive to numerous factors such as aging (e.g., Devitt et al., 2017); therefore, the generalizability of our results could also be impacted by the fact that all memories are drawn from a homogenous pool of NDErs. To address this issue, it would be interesting to assess autobiographical and flashbulb memories in healthy controls and to further explore the cognitive processes specific to NDErs, especially since previous studies suggest a specific cognitive profile, including a propensity for dissociation (Greyson, 2000a) and illusory recollections (Martial et al., 2017c). In the present study, we did not control for the production of false memories and the subjective feeling of remembering that may accompany false remembering (i.e., illusory recollection). In this context, it is difficult to control for the veracity of the personal memories recalled by NDErs. Nonetheless, we believe that the production of false memories and illusory recollections would have affected the three subtypes of memories equally. Indeed, some factors such as age and affect were shown to impact the emergence of false memories. Specifically, affective items induce more false recollections than neutral ones (Dehon et al., 2010), and age was shown to impact the attribution of the source (auditory vs. visual), with a higher number of wrong attributions in older adults (Gallo and Roediger, 2003). Given the design of our study (i.e., within subject measures) and the temporal proximity between events, the three types of memories should have been impacted similarly by the age of participants. Regarding affect, all memories were highly emotional, albeit autobiographical and NDE memories were associated with positive emotions and flashbulb memories with negative ones. Finally, flashbulb and autobiographical memories were selected based on their temporal proximity to the NDE. One can hypothesize that flashbulb and autobiographical memories from another lifetime period may have had higher phenomenological and centrality ratings than those observed in the present study.

Conclusion

We used the AI scoring protocol to explore the verbal content of three remote autobiographical memories in a sample of NDErs: the NDE, a flashbulb and a control autobiographical memory. Overall, NDE verbal recollections include a higher overall amount of details and a higher amount of internal/episodic details than flashbulb and autobiographical memories. More precisely, they comprise more event and perceptual internal details. Moreover, we found that flashbulb memories are associated to a lower intensity of feelings while remembering and a lower personal importance, and are less reactivated and less susceptible to be remembered from a first person perspective. Finally, NDE memories are more central than autobiographical memories, which in turn are more central than flashbulb memories. Besides, more central events contain more episodic verbal details. Overall, our results fall in line with existing literature that highlights the uniqueness of NDE memories.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Liège.

Author Contributions

HC: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, investigation, data curation, writing ߝ original draft, writing ߝ review and editing, and visualization. EB: formal analysis, investigation, and writing ߝ review and editing. CB: conceptualization, methodology, and supervision writing ߝ review and editing. NP: formal analysis and writing ߝ review and editing. VC-V: conceptualization and writing ߝ review and editing. SL: supervision, writing ߝ review and editing, and funding acquisition. CM: conceptualization, methodology, writing ߝ review and editing, supervision, and funding acquisition.

Funding

This study was supported by the University and University Hospital of Liege, the Belgian National Fund for Scientific Research (FRS-FNRS), the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Framework Programme for Research and Innovation under the Specific Grant Agreement No. 785907 (Human Brain Project SGA2), the Luminous project (EU-H2020-fetopenga686764), the European Space Agency (ESA) and the Belgian Federal Science Policy Office (BELSPO) in the framework of the PRODEX Programme, the Center-TBI project (FP7-HEALTH-602150), the Public Utility Foundation “Université Européenne du Travail,” “Fondazione Europea di Ricerca Biomedica,” the Bial Foundation, the Mind Science Foundation and the European Commission, the fund Generet, the King Baudouin Foundation, DOCMA project (EU-H2020-MSCA-RISE-778234).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the International Association for Near-Death Studies (IANDS France).

References

Addis, D. R., Musicaro, R., Pan, L., and Schacter, D. L. (2010). Episodic simulation of past and future events in older adults: evidence from an experimental recombination task. Psychol. Aging 25, 369–376. doi: 10.1037/a0017280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychol. Sci. 19, 33–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02043.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastin, C., Feyers, D., Jedidi, H., Bahri, M. A., Degueldre, C., Lemaire, C., et al. (2013). Episodic autobiographical memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: what are the neural correlates? Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1811–1825. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berntsen, D., and Rubin, D. C. (2006). The centrality of event scale: a measure of integrating a trauma into one’s identity and its relation to post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 219–231. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.01.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berntsen, D., and Thomsen, D. K. (2005). Personal memories for remote historical events: accuracy and clarity of flashbulb memories related to World War II. J. Exp. Psychol. 134, 242–257. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianco, S., Sambin, M., and Palmieri, A. (2017). Meaning making after a near-death experience: the relevance of intrapsychic and interpersonal dynamics. Death Stud. 41, 562–573. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2017.1310768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R., and Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition 5, 73–99. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(77)90018-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassol, H., D’Argembeau, A., Charland-Verville, V., Laureys, S., and Martial, C. (2019a). Memories of near-death experiences: are they self-defining? Neurosci. Conscious. 2019:niz002. doi: 10.1093/nc/niz002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassol, H., Martial, C., Annen, J., Martens, G., Charland-Verville, V., Majerus, S., et al. (2019b). A systematic analysis of distressing near-death experience accounts. Memory 2019:1626438. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2019.1626438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassol, H., Pétré, B., Degrange, S., Martial, C., Charland-Verville, V., Lallier, F., et al. (2018). Qualitative thematic analysis of the phenomenology of near-death experiences. PLoS One 13:e0193001. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0193001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charland-Verville, V., Jourdan, J.-P., Thonnard, M., Ledoux, D., Donneau, A.-F., Quertemont, E., et al. (2014). Near-death experiences in non-life-threatening events and coma of different etiologies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:203. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christianson, S.-A. (1989). Flashbulb memories: special, but not so special. Mem. Cogn. 17, 435–443. doi: 10.3758/BF03202615

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christianson, S.-A., and Engelberg, E. (1999). Memory and emotional consistency: the MS estonia ferry disaster. Memory 7, 471–482. doi: 10.1080/741944919

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, M. A., and Dewhurst, S. A. (1995). Remembering, familiarity, and source monitoring. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 48, 125–140.

Google Scholar

Curci, A., Luminet, O., Finkenauer, C., Gisle, L., Botermans, N., Chambres, P., et al. (2001). Flashbulb memories in social groups: a comparative test±retest study of the memory of French President Mitterrand’s death in a French and a Belgian group Requests for reprints should be sent to. Memory 9, 81–101. doi: 10.1080/09658210042000120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Argembeau, A., and Van der Linden, M. (2008). Remembering pride and shame: self-enhancement and the phenomenology of autobiographical memory. Memory 16, 538–547. doi: 10.1080/09658210802010463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehon, H., Laroi, F., and Van der Linden, M. (2010). Affective valence influences participant’s susceptibility to false memories and illusory recollection. Emotion 10, 627–639. doi: 10.1037/a0019595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dell’Olio, A. J. (2010). Do near-death experiences provide a rational basis for belief in life after death? Sophia 49, 113–128. doi: 10.1007/s11841-009-0154-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demiray, B., and Freund, A. M. (2015). Michael Jackson, Bin Laden and I: functions of positive and negative, public and private flashbulb memories. Memory 23, 487–506. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2014.907428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devitt, A. L., Addis, D. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2017). Episodic and semantic content of memory and imagination: a multilevel analysis. Mem. Cogn. 45, 1078–1094. doi: 10.3758/s13421-017-0716-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finkenauer, C., Luminet, O., Gisle, L., El-Ahmadi, A., and van Der, M. (1998). Flashbulb memories and the underlying mechanisms of their formation: toward an emotional-integrative model. Mem. Cogn. 26, 516–531. doi: 10.3758/bf03201160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallo, D. A., and Roediger, H. L. (2003). The effects of associations and aging on illusory recollection. Mem. Cogn. 31, 1036–1044. doi: 10.3758/BF03196124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greyson, B. (1983). The near-death experience scale. Construction, reliability, and validity. J. Nervous Mental Dis. 171, 369–375. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198306000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greyson, B. (1997). The near-death experience as a focus of clinical attention. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 185, 327–334. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199705000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greyson, B. (2000a). Dissociation in people who have near-death experiences: out of their bodies or out of their minds? Lancet 355, 460–463. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)82013-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greyson, B. (2000b). “Near-death experiences,” in Varieties of Anomalous Experiences: Examining the Scientific Evidence, eds E. Cardena, S. Lynn, and S. Krippner (Washigton, DC: American Psychological Association), 315–352.

Google Scholar

Greyson, B. (2003). Incidence and correlates of near-death experiences in a cardiac care unit. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 25, 269–276. doi: 10.1016/S0163-8343(03)00042-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greyson, B., and Stevenson, I. (1980). The phenomenology of near-death experiences. Am. J. Psychiatry 137, 1193–1196.

Google Scholar

Groth-Marnat, G., and Summers, R. (1998). Altered beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors following near-death experiences. J. Hum. Psychol. 38, 110–125. doi: 10.1177/00221678980383005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. K., Foley, M. A., Suengas, A. G., and Raye, C. L. (1988). Phenomenal characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical events. J. Exp. Psychol. 117, 371–376.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kenny, L. M., and Bryant, R. A. (2007). Keeping memories at an arm’s length: Vantage point of trauma memories. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 1915–1920. doi: 10.1016/J.BRAT.2006.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knoblauch, H., Schmied, I., and Schnettler, B. (2001). Different kinds of near-death experience: A report on a survey of near-death experiences in Germany. J. Near Death Stud. 20, 15–29.

Google Scholar

Kraha, A., and Boals, A. (2014). Why so negative? Positive flashbulb memories for a personal event. Memory 22, 442–449. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2013.798121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krinsley, K. E., Gallagher, J. G., Weathers, F. W., Kutter, C. J., and Kaloupek, D. G. (2003). Consistency of retrospective reporting about exposure to traumatic events. J. Trauma Stress 16, 399–409. doi: 10.1023/A:1024474204233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanciano, T., Curci, A., Matera, G., and Sartori, G. (2018). Measuring the flashbulb-like nature of memories for private events: the flashbulb memory checklist. Memory 26, 1053–1064. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2018.1428348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange, R., Greyson, B., and Houran, J. (2004). A Rasch scaling validation of a “core” near-death experience. Br. J. Psychol. 95, 161–177. doi: 10.1348/000712604773952403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., and Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychol. Aging 17, 677–689. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luminet, O., and Curci, A. (2009). The 9/11 attacks inside and outside the US: testing four models of flashbulb memory formation across groups and the specific effects of social identity. Memory 17, 742–759. doi: 10.1080/09658210903081827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martial, C., Cassol, H., Antonopoulos, G., Charlier, T., Heros, J., Donneau, A.-F., et al. (2017a). Temporality of features in near-death experience narratives. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:311. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martial, C., Charland-Verville, V., Cassol, H., Didone, V., Van Der Linden, M., and Laureys, S. (2017b). Intensity and memory characteristics of near-death experiences. Conscious Cogn. 56, 120–127. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.06.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martial, C., Charland-Verville, V., Dehon, H., and Laureys, S. (2017c). False memory susceptibility in coma survivors with and without a near-death experience. Psychol. Res. 82, 806–818. doi: 10.1007/s00426-017-0855-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martial, C., Cassol, H., Charland-Verville, V., Merckelbach, H., and Laureys, S. (2018). Fantasy proneness correlates with the intensity of near-death experience. Front. Psychiatry 9:190. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martial, C., Cassol, H., Charland-Verville, V., Pallavicini, C., Sanz, C., Zamberlan, F., et al. (2019a). Neurochemical models of near-death experiences: a large-scale study based on the semantic similarity of written reports. Conscious Cogn. 69, 52–69. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martial, C., Mensen, A., Charland-Verville, V., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Rentmeister, D., Bahri, M. A., et al. (2019b). Neurophenomenology of near-death experience memory in hypnotic recall: a within-subject EEG study. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-50601-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, L. E., and Greyson, B. (2017). Characteristics of memories for near-death experiences. Conscious Cogn. 51, 116–124. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moscovitch, D. A., Vidovic, V., Lenton-Brym, A. P., Dupasquier, J. R., Barber, K. C., Hudd, T., et al. (2018). Autobiographical memory retrieval and appraisal in social anxiety disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 107, 106–116. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2018.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moscovitch, M., Rosenbaum, R. S., Gilboa, A., Addis, D. R., Westmacott, R., Grady, C., et al. (2005). Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: a unified account based on multiple trace theory. J. Anat. 207, 35–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00421.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment. MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neisser, U., Winograd, E., Bergman, E. T., Schreiber, C. A., Palmer, S. E., and Weldon, M. S. (1996). Remembering the Earthquake: direct Experience vs. Hearing the News. Memory 4, 337–358. doi: 10.1080/096582196388898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigro, G., and Neisser, U. (1983). Point of view in personal memories. Cogn. Psychol. 15, 467–482. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90016-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noyes, R. J. (1980). Attitude change following near-death experiences. Psychiatry 43, 234–242. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1980.11024070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmieri, A., Calvo, V., Kleinbub, J., Meconi, F., Marangoni, M., Barilaro, P., et al. (2014). “Reality” of near-death-experience memories: evidence from a psychodynamic and electrophysiological integrated study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:429. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pillemer, D. B. (2009). ““Hearing the news” versus “being there”: comparing flashbulb memories and recall of first-hand experiences,” in Flashbulb memories: New issues and new perspectives (pp. 125–140). New York, NY, US, eds O. Luminet and A. Curci (Amsterdem: Psychology Press).

Google Scholar

Robinson, J. A., and Swanson, K. L. (1993). Field and observer modes of remembering. Memory 1, 169–184. doi: 10.1080/09658219308258230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schacter, D. L., Norman, K. A., and Koutstaal, W. (1998). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 49, 289–318. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaefer, A., and Philippot, P. (2005). Selective effects of emotion on the phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories. Memory 13, 148–160. doi: 10.1080/09658210344000648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwaninger, J., Eisenberg, P. R., Kenneth, B., and Weiss, A. N. (2002). A prospective analysis of near-death experiences in cardiac arrest patients. J. Near Death Stud. 20, 215–232. doi: 10.1023/a:1015258818660

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinvorth, S., Levine, B., and Corkin, S. (2005). Medial temporal lobe structures are needed to re-experience remote autobiographical memories: evidence from H.M. and W.R. Neuropsychologia 43, 479–496. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutin, A. R., and Robins, R. W. (2008). When the “I” looks at the “Me”: autobiographical memory, visual perspective, and the self. Conscious Cogn. 17, 1386–1397. doi: 10.1016/J.CONCOG.2008.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talarico, J. M., and Rubin, D. C. (2003). Confidence, not consistency, characterizes flashbulb memories. Psychol. Sci. 14, 455–461. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.02453

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thonnard, M., Charland-Verville, V., Brédart, S., Dehon, H., Ledoux, D., Laureys, S., et al. (2013). Characteristics of near-death experiences memories as compared to real and imagined events memories. PLoS One 8:e0057620. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057620

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tinti, C., Schmidt, S., Sotgiu, I., Testa, S., and Curci, A. (2009). The role of importance/consequentiality appraisal in flashbulb memory formation: the case of the death of Pope John Paul II. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 23, 236–253. doi: 10.1002/acp.1452

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Giezen, A. E., Arensman, E., Spinhoven, P., and Wolters, G. (2005). Consistency of memory for emotionally arousing events: a review of prospective and experimental studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 25, 935–953. doi: 10.1016/J.CPR.2005.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Lommel, P., Van Wees, R., Meyers, V., and Elfferich, I. (2001). Near-death experience in survivors of cardiac arrest: a prospective study in the Netherlands. Lancet 358, 2039–2045. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(01)07100-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, H. L., Conway, M. A., and Cohen, G. (2008). “Autobiographical memory,” in Memory in the Real World, eds G. Cohen and M. A. Conway (Hove: Psychology Press), 21–90.

Google Scholar

Keywords: autobiographical memory, near-death experience, flashbulb memory, autobiographical interview, memory ߝ consolidation

Citation: Cassol H, Bonin EAC, Bastin C, Puttaert N, Charland-Verville V, Laureys S and Martial C (2020) Near-Death Experience Memories Include More Episodic Components Than Flashbulb Memories. Front. Psychol. 11:888. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00888

Received: 03 December 2019; Accepted: 09 April 2020;
Published: 13 May 2020.

Edited by:

Gezinus Wolters, Leiden University, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Maddalena Boccia, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Steve M. J. Janssen, University of Nottingham Malaysia Campus, Malaysia

Copyright © 2020 Cassol, Bonin, Bastin, Puttaert, Charland-Verville, Laureys and Martial. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Helena Cassol, hcassol@uliege.be

ORCID: Helena Cassol, orcid.org/0000-0001-6616-7688

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.