Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 28 July 2020
Sec. Gender, Sex and Sexualities

The Effect of Passively Viewing a Consent Campaign Video on Attitudes Toward Rape

  • Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University, Talbot Campus, Poole, United Kingdom

Around 90% of rape victims know their perpetrator, making acquaintance rape the most common form of rape, contradicting societal beliefs. There is ambiguity about the meaning and use of consent in sexual scenarios (Beres, 2007). This study used a mixed methods approach to test the effectiveness of a campaign video aimed at increasing understanding of consent. We assessed whether the video affected rape judgments in vignettes depicting consensual or non-consensual sexual scenarios. We also manipulated whether making consent the primary or secondary question influenced attitudes. Text responses were also obtained to gain an insight into participant reasoning. The campaign showed no increase in rape judgments. Making consent primary in question order did lead to greater accuracy in rape judgment. A content analysis of the free-text responses indicated that the presence of the campaign actually reduced people’s use of consent in explaining why a scenario may represent rape: Instead they focused on the attractiveness of the attacker. These results are discussed in relation to the effectiveness of passively viewing campaign material.

Introduction

Rape is typically defined as intentional penetration of the vagina, anus, or mouth of another person without their consent and when the alleged perpetrator does not reasonably believe that the person consented (Home Office, 2003). Most rape is acquaintance rape: a situation in which the rape victim knows the perpetrator (Hester and Lilley, 2016). Core to the definition of rape is consent: A person consents if the person agrees by choice and has the freedom and capacity to make that choice in United Kingdom law (Ministry of Justice Home Office Office for National Statistics, 2013), however, there are some debates in the psychological literature as to what defines consent (Beres, 2007; Muehlenhard et al., 2016). Perceptions of what consent is varies from believing that consent is any agreement to participate to others who suggest it should be freely given, but it is suggested that there is a general consensus among the population that consent is an agreement to take part in sexual activity (Beres, 2007). There are many campaigns that aim to increase public knowledge of sexual consent: This is especially important because the public comprises those who could be victims, perpetrators, bystanders, or jurors in rape trials. The effectiveness of such campaigns is limited (Brecklin and Forde, 2001). Despite this, governments, universities, and charities invest significant sums of money into developing new campaigns. It is important to assess the efficacy of such campaigns. The present study, therefore, investigates the effectiveness of a video campaign aimed at changing attitudes toward consent. It may also be important to target jurors directly because research suggests changing public attitudes with media campaigns faces many difficulties (Ogunfowokan and Fajemilehin, 2012); therefore, this study also investigates methods to increase jurors accurate judgments of whether a scenario is rape or not by establishing the importance of priming consent or rape when interpreting a sexual scenario. This determines whether an education campaign or highlighting consent is effective in increasing accurate identification of rape in sexual scenarios. Both public attitudes and the criminal justice system (CJS) are discussed in terms of their importance regarding rape occurrence and conviction.

Reporting (Temkin and Krahé, 2008) and conviction rates (Weller, 1992) for rape are drastically low. There are many potential reasons for the low reporting rate (including stigma and humiliation; Moszynski, 2004) and acknowledgment (Peterson and Muehlenhard, 2007) of rape. Many theorists indicate societal acceptance of rape myths is a strong deterrent for reporting rape and the low conviction rate (Weidner and Griffitt, 1983; George and Martínez, 2002; Sable et al., 2006). Public knowledge of rape and consent is of extreme importance due to the effects that rape can have on an individual. The effects can include but are not limited to posttraumatic stress disorder and self-blame (Peter-Hagene and Ullman, 2018), fear, anxiety, depression, and effects on self-esteem as well as sexual dysfunction (Resick, 1993). We suggest that a societal attitudinal change is necessary for a change to occur in both reporting and conviction. Reducing public acceptance of rape myths may increase reporting and conviction rates because the public is represented by jurors in the United Kingdom (Legislation gov uk, 2018) and many other democracies.

The importance of campaigns is revealed through several lines of evidence indicating that there is significant misunderstanding in what rape is by the general public, who could potentially be jurors, victims, or unintentional perpetrators of rape. Researchers have found that many aspects of a scenario can determine whether or not or how much a person perceives a scenario to be rape (Pollard, 1992; McHugh, 1996; Hills et al., 2020) such as wantedness, pleasure (Hills et al., 2020) gender, or traditionality of those who are placing the judgment (Pollard, 1992). Furthermore, respectability and attractiveness of the defendant can also influence judgment (Luginbuhl and Mullin, 1981; Tieger, 1981; Richardson and Campbell, 1982). Beres (2007) argues that society’s understanding of consent is underdeveloped and relies on assumed definitions of consent that could be challenged with appropriate campaigns. There is much ambiguity around the idea of consent, often because of the different ways in which it can be given (Hall, 1998; Humphreys, 2004, 2007) such as no response at all, verbal or non-verbal methods, and directly or indirectly (Waldby et al., 1990; Hickman and Muehlenhard, 1999; Beres et al., 2004; Humphreys, 2004). People see consent through many different behaviors and indicators in sexual scenarios that are related to wanting and pleasure, whereas others see it only as the clear verbal agreement to each sexual act (Hills et al., 2020). Others use consent as a boundary line for good and bad sex (Wertheimer, 2003). However, some suggest that the concept of consent is well understood by sexually active young adults and that people are skilled at sexual communication (Beres et al., 2004; Beres, 2007).

Many media campaigns have been developed to alter the attitudes toward rape and consent in specific groups of people. In recent years, there has been a movement from a “no means no” attitude to a “yes means yes” attitude toward consent (Beres, 2014) which is believed to be due to the increase in educational programs, activism, and media campaigns that aim to improve understanding of consent and rape (Beres, 2014). Campaigns generally aim to increase the understanding of sexual situations and, thus, consent and rape (Donat and White, 2000). Media campaigns have been effective in promoting sexual responsibility and information about consensual sex. For example Keller and Brown (2002) found that safe sex media campaigns were associated with increased teen condom use with casual partners, and other researchers have found that media campaigns have no effect on public attitude and that attitudinal changes can be hard to obtain (Brecklin and Forde, 2001; DeGue et al., 2014). Potter et al. (2009) have highlighted that poster campaigns are significantly more effective for those that have previously engaged in campaign behavior compared to those that have not. Even when campaigns have a wide reach in terms of the number of people viewing them, there is little evidence they actually change attitudes. Hills et al. (2020) have shown that passive viewing of a television campaign aimed to raise awareness of relationship abuse (“This is Abuse,” Home Office, 2003) does not increase accuracy of rating sexual scenarios as rape or not. Currently, little is known about particularly effective strategies for media campaigns (Lonsway, 1996).

If people do not fully understand consent, this may raise issues within the criminal justice system and court cases because the general public make up jurors in many Western democracies. It is important to not only try to change the opinions of the public in general who will be jurors, but also to look at ways to improve juror decision making through other means such as questioning. This is because they have the potential to contribute to the conviction rates. It is suggested that decisions made by jurors in legal cases are susceptible to social preexisting attitudes, myths, and biases toward rape and consent (McEwan, 2005). It is, therefore, important to target jurors directly to determine techniques that can influence the way jurors think before making a judgment on rape trials. Because the misunderstanding of rape is said to be surrounding consent (Beres, 2007) highlighting this element before juror decision making might have an impact on the judgment that a possible juror may make.

The present study, therefore, sees if highlighting the element of consent before a judgment is made is able to increase the accurate identification of rape in sexual scenarios. In the present study, we aim to test whether a campaign video that is widely used to highlight consent to the public (“Tea and Consent,” Blue Seat Studios, 2015), which depicts common rape myths but applied to drinking tea as an analogy to sexual intercourse, can increase accuracy in identification of rape in sexual scenarios. This video aims to highlight the importance of sexual consent in all scenarios and aims to tackle rape myths, such as assuming consent because it has been obtained previously (Monson et al., 2000) or that a lack of physical or vocal rejection is quite common in rape and sexual assault (Jozkowski, 2015). We use the scenarios devised by Hills et al. (2020) as these describe sexual scenarios that occur between two acquaintances and in which the sex can be consensual or not. In Hills et al.’s work, 65% of non-consensual scenarios were judged as representing rape, and this figure did not depend on viewing a campaign video. The current study also attempts to gain an understanding into participants’ verbal reasons for making such decisions by taking short free-text responses (a few sentences long) in addition to numerical data. The purpose of this is to provide deeper elaboration of the reasons why participants believe sexual scenarios represent rape, allowing us to identify particular themes that influence people’s judgments. We hypothesize that highlighting consent before asking participants to make a rape judgment will lead to significantly more (accurate) identification than when consent is highlighted after making a rape judgment. Further, if “Tea and Consent” (Blue Seat Studios, 2015) is successful in highlighting consent as it is intended, then participants who view the campaign video before making judgments will make significantly more (accurate) identification of rape than those who do not view the campaign video.

Materials and Methods

Participants

An opportunity sample of 173 participants was used, of which 90 were in the campaign-present condition and 83 were in the campaign-absent condition. Sample size was determined based on the effect size reported in Hills et al. (2020). Participants were recruited by responding to an online ad that asked potential participants if they would like to contribute to research on attitudes toward sexual violence that was distributed on survey exchange platforms. It was also advertised to students at Bournemouth University on their online research participation platform. Those from Bournemouth University were participating in return for course credits. No participant personal or demographic data was collected to ensure anonymity and to encourage participants to be completely honest in their responses. This anonymity was deliberate to protect the participants and ensure the study remained fully ethical. Participants were informed not to take part if they had any personal experience of sexual violence.

Design

A 2 × 2 mixed subjects design was employed with two independent variables: campaign (present or absent; manipulated between subjects) and question order (consent question first or rape question first; manipulated within subjects). Question order aims to determine whether or not highlighting the concept of consent (in the form of a question asking participants to identify where it lies) before asking participants to make a judgment increases the likelihood of a rape judgment being made. The dependent variable was rape judgment of a scenario in which a response of “yes” or “no” was provided. Qualitative responses were also collected to gather an understanding into participant reasoning behind their responses (Braun and Clarke, 2013) in which participants were simply asked why they gave the answer they gave in terms of verbal reasoning. The free-text responses were approximately one to two sentences long, and an expandable answer box of approximately four lines was provided. The presentation order of the vignettes was randomized across participants such that there was a roughly equal number of consensual scenarios presented with the consent question first and the rape question first.

Materials

Twenty-three short vignettes were used in this study developed by Hills et al. (2020). These vignettes were two to three sentences long and described hypothetical scenarios that were either consensual (11) or non-consensual (12) between acquaintances developed from the Reasons for Wanting Sex and Reasons for Not Wanting Sex questionnaire (Peterson and Muehlenhard, 2007). They were written in the second person and were gender-neutral. Hills et al. (2020) report that they had good face validity.

The campaign video used was “Tea and Consent” (Blue Seat Studios, 2015)1 which aimed to increase awareness of consent. This is a short video (2 min, 50 s) that demonstrates the concept of consent through the use of stick people: One of the stick people tries to force the other to drink a cup of tea in many of the scenarios that are rape myths (e.g., if they consented to drinking tea in the past, it doesn’t mean they want to now). Tea represents sexual intercourse in the video. The video was used as part of Thames Valley Police’s “Consent is everything” campaign (Thames Valley Police., 2015) which aims to raise awareness and understanding of sexual consent and suggests that, if someone is struggling to understand the idea of consent, they should imagine you’re making them a cup of tea and whether or not you’d make them drink it based on similar rape myth–type scenarios.

Procedure

This study was granted full ethical approval from the Research Ethics Panel at Bournemouth University. The study was administered online (using Qualtrics) to encourage participants to give open, honest, anonymous responses. Participants first provided informed consent; they knew the study would involve judging sexual scenarios that may or may not be consensual. Participants in the campaign-present condition were shown the short campaign video. The survey could not move onto the next question until the video was played fully. Participants were instructed to watch the video with the sound on. This mimics passive viewing of campaigns (such as on TV) and better reflects how campaigns are viewed. Participants who were in the campaign-absent condition did not experience this.

All participants were then presented with the scenarios, one at a time in a random order. They were asked to read the scenario and to imagine themselves in the described scenario. Following each scenario, participants answered three questions concerning whether they thought the scenario displayed consent and why and whether the scenario showed rape and why. The third question was contingent on the previous responses: Participants were either asked when consent was given (if they had said the scenario was consensual) or rape occurred (if they said the scenario depicted rape). The order of the questions was manipulated between subjects with the consent question asked before the others for one group of participants. Participants answered each question on a separate page and were not allowed to access previous responses to ensure answers could not be altered. Once participants had answered the 23 vignettes, they were given debrief information, including signposting to organizations offering support.

Data Analysis

The free-text data was analyzed using content analysis. Content analysis is a flexible method for analysis of text data (Cavanagh, 1997). The first author became familiar with the data to make sense of the data collected. Analysis was then conducted using inductive content analysis. Inductive content analysis was used because the research is not based on an earlier model (Bengtsson, 2016).

The data was first open coded, and categories were freely generated from the data. Following this, the codes were grouped into higher order codes. The aim of this was to reduce the number of similar descriptive categories in higher order categories. This allows for data to “belong” to a group that allows for comparison between groups. The process of abstraction then involved naming each group using data-congruent words that allowed for description and understanding. Once the data was in discrete categories, frequencies were collected (Bengtsson, 2016). This allowed a quantitative summary of qualitative data in addition to the depth of analysis associated with interpretation of the written text.

Triangulation was completed by the second author independently coding the data and comparing the codes. The two sets of coding were largely consistent (inter-rater reliability r = 0.92).

Results and Discussion

Here, we present the quantitative results, followed by the qualitative results to use these to explain the quantitative results. This form of mixed methods approach allows for a more in-depth understanding of the reasons for participant responses. Quantitative results were analyzed using a 2 × 2 mixed factorial ANOVA with the between-subjects factor of campaign and within-subjects factor of question order. The rape judgments are summarized in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Proportion of rape judgments.

There was a significant effect of question order on rape identification, F(1, 90) = 32.81, MSE = 0.02, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.27, in which the consent-first question order led to significantly more rape judgments (M = 0.47, SE = 0.02) than the rape-first question order (M = 0.36, SE = 0.02). There was no significant effect of the campaign video on rape judgment F(1, 90) = 0.12, MSE = 0.04, p = 0.735, ηp2 ≤ 0.01, BF10 < 0.01, in which the campaign-present condition had a similar number of rape judgments (M = 0.42, SE = 0.03) as the campaign-absent condition (M = 0.41, SE = 0.02). We used Bayes factor (with the prior estimated based on the significant effect of question order) to show that these results show we have strong confidence in the null hypothesis for the effect of the campaign. The interaction between question order and campaign viewing on rape judgment was not significant, F(1, 90) = 0.43, MSE = 0.02, p = 0.515, ηp2 < 0.01).

To explain these somewhat unexpected findings, we analyzed the free-text responses. We analyzed each question separately because they focused on separate issues, starting with the question about whether the participants thought the scenario was consensual or not. For the frequencies, we were able to analyze whether there was an association between the campaign presence and whether the scenario depicted consent or not. This particular analytical approach was chosen as it directly assesses the effectiveness of the campaign video. The four questions were analyzed separately as they addressed different issues. Several themes emerged from most of the questions (coercion, attraction, wanting, pleasure, and communication). Although these were subtly different across the questions, it highlights how these are core in determining whether the scenarios we presented to participants depicted consensual sex or not. Figure 2 presents an overall picture of the resulting themes for each of the questions, highlighting some significant overlap in what determines rape and consent but also some key differences. These are expanded upon here.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Overall picture of the resulting themes for each of the questions, highlighting some significant overlap in what determines rape and consent.

Was the Scenario Rape or Not?

The category of consent was used more than any other category for both consensual scenarios and non-consensual scenarios (see Table 1) to describe why a non-consensual scenario may be seen as consensual. There were significantly fewer mentions of consent in the campaign-present condition for the non-consensual scenarios than in the campaign-absent condition (Table 1). This is the opposite of expected results because the video highlights the idea of consent and may suggest the video has produced a rebound effect. The rebound effect is the result of suppressing thoughts that are then likely to subsequently reappear with greater insistence than if they had never been suppressed (Macrae et al., 1994). Corrigan and Penn (1999) suggest that attempts to suppress stereotypes can lead to a rebound effect, which may explain such frequencies. Wanting was also a category that arose from the data. Participants often used consent and wanting synonymously when it came to explaining their judgments. The results from the content analysis also suggest relevance to the criminal justice system because McHugh (1996) has found that that jurors tend to ignore the absence of consent if the situation is wanted as reflected in current findings. It may be possible that an increase in understanding would help to increase conviction rates, which is important because increasing conviction may encourage higher reporting rates because many rapes go unreported due to fear of the criminal justice process and not being believed (Ryan, 2011; Wolitzky-Taylor et al., 2011).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary content analysis (full analysis presented in Supplementary Table S1) for the question “Please explain why you think this scenario is rape or not rape?”

There is also some debate as to whether consent is a psychological or a physical act, in which psychological is based on one’s inner thoughts and physical is based on behavior that represents agreement (Muehlenhard et al., 2016). This debate may be reflected in the current findings because some participants indicated that consent was psychological, for example, not wanting, and others suggested it was a physical act with one participant saying, “I would have said that this is a rape scenario if the “victim” showed indication of wanting to stop.”

In consensual scenarios, the fact that they “both agreed” and “both consented” were most common in suggesting reasons as to why consent was given, and the scenario was therefore consensual. However, many participants still suggested that consent was given in a non-consensual scenario, suggesting the idea that any form of consent is enough (see Supplementary Table S1). Many participants said that “giving in” and “being persuaded” were forms of consent, which can reflect the category of coercion in which participants described coercive consent to be both a reason as to why a scenario was consensual as well as why consent was not given.

“Even though there was persistence on one person’s part, the other still gave in, which means that both parties consented before actually having sex.”

Participants often suggested that a non-consensual scenario was consensual because it was wanted and, similarly, suggested that a consensual scenario could have been non-consensual because it was not wanted. Responses may reflect previous research findings that suggest wantedness and consent can be rated as synonymous and also contrasting concepts (Artime and Peterson, 2015) which may suggest a lack of understanding in public perceptions of consent. Participant responses seem to represent wanting from the victim rather than the perpetrator in which, if the victim wanted to have sex, then the person has consented to it.

Some participants suggested that pleasure influenced their judgment in whether or not a scenario was consensual. Some referred to arousal, pleasure, or “because they enjoyed it” as justification for why a scenario was consensual. Some non-consensual pleasurable scenarios were judged to be consensual with participants suggesting that “bad sex does not mean rape” possibly suggesting that participants believed the common rape myth that women have bad experiences or regret and then “cry rape” (Stewart et al., 1996). Some participants were clear that elements, such as enjoyment, do not represent consent with one participant stating that “pleasure was irrelevant if they didn’t consent.”

“Because even though you didn’t give consent, you enjoyed it.”

It was also found that some non-consensual scenarios were perceived as consensual because the absence of consent was not a “clear message,” which led to the development of the category communication. Indeed, there was a significant association between campaign presence and scenario type, in which communication was not mentioned as frequently for non-consensual scenarios for those who had seen the campaign than those who had not. It was also indicated by participants that there needed to be communication of the lack of pleasure or wanting, and participants suggested that the partner cannot “read my mind” but that if intercourse were to continue after it was communicated, it would then depict non-consensual sex. This may also suggest that victim blaming is present in the current findings because participants are suggesting that the victim is at fault for not communicating clearly enough that the person did not consent to sex.

Participants also suggested that being in a relationship with a person was often a reason as to why they believed a scenario to be consensual. One participant indicated that “it is just what happens in relationships” and that, therefore, it was not as serious or as likely to be non-consensual compared to if they were not in a relationship. Research has suggested that this rape myth, that consent can be assumed in relationships, is also reflected in barrister opinions in court. A qualitative study found that barristers thought it was not a “terrible offense” if the victim of rape has previously had sex with the perpetrator and suggested that jurors would agree with them (Estrich, 1987; Temkin, 2000). This may also reflect or contribute to the low conviction rate for acquaintance rapes as previously mentioned (Temkin and Krahé, 2008).

Last, although not mentioned as often, attractiveness still arose as a category that participants used to suggest why they thought a non-consensual scenario was consensual. Most participants that referred to this in their responses suggested that being attracted to someone or finding their behavior attractive meant that consent could be assumed.

In non-consensual scenarios, the perceived lack of consent present in the scenarios was the most common reason as to why participants thought the scenario was non-consensual. This was often depicted with the phrases “they didn’t agree,” “they refused,” and “they didn’t consent.” In consensual scenarios, consent that was given after coercion was used to describe why they perceived the scenario to be non-consensual, especially following the campaign (as evidenced by the significant association between campaign and consent). This suggests the campaign was making participants see coercion in consensual scenarios where they should not have seen it. Reasons such as “scared to say no” and “forced to say yes” were used to represent coercive consent, which some suggested was not consent that was “freely given” and, therefore, did not represent “proper” or “full” consent. However, some participants said that “giving in” or “changing their mind” after coercion was a form of consent and that being uncomfortable due to coercion “does not make it rape.” Coercion, however, was more commonly a reason as to why a situation was non-consensual than why a scenario was consensual (Supplementary Table S1). Some participants showed some confusion and suggested that insistence was not coercive and that the scenario is “not rape unless insistence was coercive,” and others thought the level of coercion was important: “This is difficult without knowing the level of insistence.”

“No true consent can exist under pressure/coercion; the end result makes no difference.”

An interesting finding that arose from the data was the use of second and third person and gender labeling. Despite being asked to imagine themselves in the scenario, second and third person was said more often in non-consensual scenarios than consensual scenarios (see Table 1). This may suggest a form of psychological distancing from the scenarios by participants and may suggest that participants may not have wanted to be responsible for a non-consensual scenario and may have wanted to distance themselves and put more responsibility onto the person said to be in the scenario. This may also represent victim blaming because participants may not have wanted to be responsible for the result of the scenario. In addition to this, it was also interesting to see the use of gender labeling that appeared in the responses. Both the video and scenarios were gender neutral, but many participants still referred to people in the scenarios as “he” or “she.” It is important to note that the use of “she” was often used to describe the person in the scenario, and “he” was often used to refer to the partner. This could reflect sex-role stereotyping in rape scenarios and demonstrate participants answering with a general sex script in mind that is that females tend to accept or reject a man’s actions and not the reverse (Proite et al., 1993).

When Was Consent Given?

Table 2 shows the categories for the question regarding when consent was given. Some participants simply stated that consent was not given in a scenario, but for most participants, the point at which consent was given was when the scenario stated either “you agree” or “both people agree.” This supports Beres (2007) findings that the concept of consent is quite well understood as an agreement to have sex. There was a difference in the number of mentions between campaign conditions in the agreement category in which there were more mentions of agreement from participants that had not watched the campaign, again possibly suggesting the campaign has produced a rebound effect (Corrigan and Penn, 1999).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary content analysis (full analysis presented in Supplementary Table S2) for the question “Please explain at which point in the scenario you think consent was given?”

Some participants said that an agreement was given in a non-consensual scenario. This was often when coerced consent was given, which links this category to coercion because many participants suggested that coercive agreement was still an agreement and that consent was given then.

“Consent was given after insistence from the partner.”

The initiation of sex or “when sex began” was commonly used to suggest when consent was given. Some participants assumed that because the two individuals in the scenario had sex, then, at some point, consent must have been given but did not specify at what point this may have been. Some participants still said this even when the scenario made the lack of consent quite clear. After seeing the campaign video, participants were more likely to see consent given at the initiation point for consensual scenarios and less likely to see it for non-consensual scenarios, indicating some evidence of the campaign producing insight into when to look for consent.

“Although it says ‘you do not consent’ the person still had sex with them.”

Some participants thought that engaging in sex was consenting (see Supplementary Table S2), which could be a concerning belief to have because the initiation of sex without consent is actually believed by many to be the point at which a scenario becomes non-consensual (Supplementary Table S3) and is also close to the definition of rape (Ministry of Justice Home Office Office for National Statistics, 2013).

Similar to the reasoning as to why a scenario is consensual or non-consensual, categories of enjoyment and attraction arose from the data. Participants suggested that the point in time at which the victim began to enjoy themselves or began to find the partner or their behavior attractive was the point in time when consent was given. Participants suggested that consent “must have been given” if the person found the experience pleasurable or the behavior of the partner attractive. This highlights the conflation of enjoyment and consent with one participant suggesting that they thought this would also be reflected in the criminal justice system:

“I think a barrister would argue if it was enjoyable it must have been consensual.”

This may suggest that participants think that this opinion is a widely held belief and that others, including those within the legal system who are responsible for defending both victims and perpetrators of rape, also hold the same opinion.

Some participants suggested that “agreeing after persistence” was not “freely” agreeing to take part, and this was not classed as agreeing to take part, and therefore, an agreement was not made, and consent was not given at any point. This reflects the debate present in literature in which some researchers think “any yes” is acceptable as consent (Dripps, 1992) whereas others believe that only “yes means yes” (Beres, 2014). Such a research debate reflects societal opinions. Similar to the first question, coercion was falsely made more apparent in consensual scenarios by those who had seen the campaign than those who had not as indicated by the significant chi-square value.

The category protest arose from the data demonstrating that consent was given in a scenario at the point when there was a lack of disapproval, for example, “you did not stop it” or “you didn’t say a clear no,” suggesting that, unless one fights against the initiation of sex, you, therefore, agree. This may represent victim blaming by suggesting that it is the victim’s fault for not fighting or not making their rejection clear enough rather than the perpetrators fault for not obtaining consent. Given that the campaign video was designed to combat these kinds of rape myths, it is somewhat surprising to see that it had no significant effect in changing attitudes here.

When Did the Scenario Become Rape?

Table 3 describes the categories following when the scenario becomes rape. The content analysis indicated that the use of force was a point in time when participants suggested a scenario became rape, which reflects previous findings that suggest a forceful scenario is more likely to be perceived as rape (Bachman, 1993; Krahé et al., 2008; Mayers, 2013). The forceful reasoning by participants does not refer to physical force or violence and tends to reflect a verbal force. Force is somewhat described as a more serious case of coercion, which was also one of the most prevalent points in time when a scenario became non-consensual (see Supplementary Table S3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary content analysis (full analysis presented in Supplementary Table S3) for the question “At which point in the scenario do you think It became rape?”

“When the partner forces sex without consent.”

In terms of coercion, participants varied by the point in time at which coercion became non-consensual with some participants suggesting that it became non-consensual “when pressure is put on a person,” whereas others suggested that the scenario became non-consensual when coercive consent was given or when sex was initiated after coercion had occurred: “When sex occurred due to pressure.” “Pressure,” “persistence,” and “persuasion” were often words used to describe coercion that made a scenario non-consensual. This suggests that these participants have different opinions from other participants that suggested that consent was given when coercion occurred, demonstrating the variability in public perceptions (see Table 3).

Initiation was also a point in time defined by participants that suggested a scenario became non-consensual as soon as “sex began” or sex was “initiated.” This could reflect knowledge of the meaning of consent because a scenario would be defined as non-consensual only once penetration of the mouth, anus, or vagina had occurred and not before, possibly suggesting that these participants may have knowledge of the definition of rape and, thus, when a scenario may become non-consensual. Those who had not seen the video were significantly more likely to indicate rape occurring at the initiation of consensual scenarios than those who had not seen the video. Similar to coercion, participants suggested that the given scenario became rape “during” refusal. Although some referred to the refusal itself, for example, “when you refused to engage in sexual activity,” others suggested that it was when sexual advances continued after refusal and some participants said that it became rape when sex occurred after refusal was given.

“When the partner had intercourse even after the individual refused.”

In line with some participant’s reasons for why they thought the scenario was consensual or not, participants also mentioned wantedness and suggested that the scenario became non-consensual when the person did not want to have sex or when intercourse occurred without one or both of the participants wanting to have sex. Wantedness, as previously mentioned, was being conflated with consent and, therefore, highlights the misunderstanding of consent and that there may be little differentiation between the two in public perceptions. This also suggests that participants may have a lack of understanding that someone can consent to an unwanted scenario. Research has suggested there are many reasons for such to occur, such as intimacy, to satisfy partners needs, and to reduce tension (O’Sullivan and Allgeier, 1998).

When Was Consent Not Given?

Finally, to assess when participants thought consent was not given in scenarios, we ran another content analysis (Table 4). Agreement (or lack of) was one of the main reasons as to why consent may have not been given in a scenario and suggested that, when one or both participants do not agree, then consent is not given. Participants who had seen the campaign mentioned agreement significantly less frequently for consensual scenarios than those who had not seen the video. Some participants mentioned that an agreement was not made between the two individuals, and others mentioned that the individual displayed a lack of agreement, such as “refusal” or “rejection.” It could be suggested that these are positive findings because they suggest that people do understand that an agreement is necessary in sexual encounters and that, if such agreement is not achieved, that consent is, therefore, not given, and a scenario represents rape. However, as previously mentioned, consent is not the only factor participants used to determine whether or not consent is given in a scenario (Supplementary Table S1). This category supports Beres (2007) findings that there is a general consensus among the population that consent is an agreement to take part in sexual activity and does suggest that the concept of consent could be quite well understood (Beres et al., 2004).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Summary content analysis (full analysis presented in Supplementary Table S4) for the question “Why do you think consent is not given in this scenario?”

“Both you and your partner agree to do so.”

“I don’t think consent is given freely; therefore, it is not properly given.”

A lack of freedom to give consent was mentioned by participants and suggested that, although consent was given in a scenario, the person who gave the consent did not give consent with the freedom to choose meaning that participants believed it to be non-consensual, which suggests participants may be aware that, by definition, consent needs to be given with free choice (Home Office, 2003). Participants also said that many scenarios were non-consensual because they were unwanted. This, again, highlights the possible confusion between wanting and consent.

“It wasn’t what you wanted to happen. You didn’t want sex.”

Coercive methods were also referred to, suggesting that the use of coercive strategies, such as pressure and persuasion could suggest why consent was not given, however, it should be noted that coercive methods have previously been mentioned as a reason as to why a scenario was consensual (Table 3), which suggests a difference in perceptions across participants. Participants who used coercion to suggest why consent was not given in a scenario suggested that “true” consent cannot be given under pressure, which is in line with some researchers’ definitions of consent (Humphreys, 2004).

“Consent is given when the person agrees to have sex, but it’s not free consent in the same way someone who initially wanted to have sex gives consent.”

Some suggested that consent was not given in a scenario because of the presence of fear in the individual in the scenario; this was often related to the consequences of the sexual act and being “scared of the repercussions of saying no.” “How the partner may react” was also referred to by participants. Participants mentioned that simply being fearful of the partner themselves may suggest a reason as to why consent was not given. Additionally, fear was only mentioned in coercive consensual scenarios, suggesting that the consent given in the scenarios was not “proper” consent. This may be because there were more “clear cut” reasons as to why consent was not given in scenarios that were not coerced or non-consensual, such as lack of agreement.

“Although they agreed, they agreed because they were scared and were trying to protect themselves from danger.”

Conclusion

The quantitative findings have shown that the campaign video was not effective given that participants who had seen the campaign gave similar responses to those who had not. This supports previous findings that media campaigns may not be effective in changing public attitudes (Ogunfowokan and Fajemilehin, 2012; DeGue et al., 2014; Hills et al., 2020) and that attitudinal changes in society can be difficult to achieve (Brecklin and Forde, 2001). The qualitative analysis seemed to offer a potential explanation for this. Consent was actually mentioned more often by those who had not seen the campaign, suggesting some sort of rebound effect. It might reflect that participants acted contrary to the instructions in the video. Of course, we were only considering participant identification of rape rather than actual behaviors. Further, the viewing of the video was immediately before reading the sexual scenarios. Nevertheless, we would have expected more convincing findings regarding the effectiveness of the campaign if it was processed deeply by our participants.

A possible reason as to why the campaign was not effective may be due to the lack of realistic elements in the video. The campaign showed stick men to represent humans as well as tea to represent consent. Because both of these elements are very different from how real-life rapes occur, it is possible that it could have been hard for participants to relate the video to the scenarios. This, however, could have increased the external validity of the video campaign because it is unlikely that media campaigns are able to demonstrate exactly what will happen in a real-life rape, which means, for such campaigns to be effective, the resulting attitudinal change needs to be applicable and generalizable to other non-consensual examples (Camilleri et al., 2015). A second reason is that there is no reason for our participants to engage with the video during its presentation (Fabiano et al., 2003; Binet and Field, 2009). They were simply instructed to watch it (similar to if it were on TV or given during a university induction event). If the participants were asked to discuss the video with others, potentially it would have led to more deep coding and analysis and more attitudinal change. Further, the scenarios depicted, although representative of the most common type of sexual assault, are not consistent with stereotypical rape (Stirling et al., 2020). When the message is further from one’s internal script, it is harder to process the message (Neuschatz et al., 2002) meaning that, without elaboration, the “Tea and Consent” video may not be remembered. However, because little is known about what makes a campaign effective in changing attitudes and behaviors long term (Lonsway, 1996) we can only speculate that these elements may have contributed to the lack of effectiveness of this media campaign.

Potentially a more disturbing effect of the video was that it appeared to cause participants to discuss coercion in consensual sexual scenarios. This indicates that, although the video did not encourage participants to use consent in their understanding, it made them see coercion where it was not present. This may be a demand characteristic (participants felt they should be looking for more coercive behaviors in the scenarios following seeing the video), but if so, one would expect to see coercion discussed more in the non-consensual scenarios as well. This was not the case. Furthermore, seeing the video made people less likely to suggest the initiation of sex in non-consensual scenarios was when rape occurred than not seeing the video. This evaluation point highlights a clear need to properly evaluate how people interpret and use campaign videos.

More broadly, the content analysis of the free-text responses gave an insight into public perceptions around rape and consent. Although there is some coherent understanding in some areas, such as the need for agreement, there is still some confusion in many aspects, particularly the conflation of wanting and consent as well as high variability in opinions regarding coercive consent and the concept of “real” consent. Many participants referred to the scenarios as highlighting not “proper,” “full,” or “real” consent. This may suggest that participants think that consent is not a simple dichotomy as implied by the law. Participants see consent along a spectrum depending on various factors in the scenario, such as fear, coercion, wanting, or choice.

The conflation of consent and wantedness has arisen throughout the content analysis and may suggest a possible avenue to develop training for jurors in rape cases to help them to better discriminate between wanting and consenting in sexual scenarios to increase understanding and, thus, conviction. This will help to ensure that all sexual assaults that occur without consent are treated as such in court. Future research may wish to investigate effective methods to inform participants and, therefore, potential jurors about the differences between wanting and consenting.

Future research may wish to look into alternative methods to change public attitudes and should focus on a more rounded approach to target more than just rape awareness and to target victim empathy, rape myths, and demonstrate the consequences of rape (Schewe, 2002). Schewe (2002) has suggested these elements are necessary for effective rape prevention programs. This may lead to more empathetic perceptions toward victims of rape as well as being better informed of the consequences and the prevalence of rape. This may lead to a change in both public attitudes toward rape and the occurrence of rape.

Throughout our analysis, we have ignored several individual difference factors that may influence understanding of consent and rape. This was deliberate because we did not collect demographic data. Although this was intended to increase the anonymity and honesty for participants, it means we are unable to assess the representativeness of our sample. Further, we were unable to assess gender differences in response to the campaign and possible gender differences in perceptions of the scenarios. Gender differences are generally present in the perceptions of sexual scenarios and sexual abuse (Schutte and Hosch, 1997; Hockett et al., 2016; James, 2018), however, in the scenarios we used, gender differences have typically not been found (Hills et al., 2020). Nevertheless, this limitation should be acknowledged.

Question order was found to be effective in changing the judgments of rape, and the results show that highlighting consent before asking participants to make a judgment on rape significantly increased the number of rape judgments. The implications of this result are twofold. In the first, it highlights that participants do not fully realize that rape is defined as sex without consent. Second, when participants are explicitly made aware of the lack of consent, they understand that a scenario depicts rape, indicating unconscious knowledge of the definition of rape. The results demonstrate that the problem may not lie within the knowledge of consent because results show that, once consent has been highlighted, participants were able to better identify it. Although the qualitative content analysis does suggest some misunderstanding, it may be that the findings have provided implications for jurors in the decision-making process in court. This could be difficult to implement due to the strict and structured judicial system and jurors’ decision making process (Pennington and Hastie, 1986), however, it highlights the need to inform jurors that consent is all that matters in the judgment of rape. The primacy of this knowledge can impact on the accuracy of rape judgments. This could, in turn, encourage reporting of rapes because many victims of rape do not report due to the fear of not being believed (Sable et al., 2006); therefore, if victims feel like they are more likely to believed and convict their perpetrator, it may lead to progress in increasing reporting and conviction.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the article/Supplementary Material.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Bournemouth University Research Ethics Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

ER: design and implementation of the study, data analysis, and drafting of the work. PH: supervision, guidance on study design, corrections on drafts, and some data analysis. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01741/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.youtube.com/watch?v~=~oQbei5JGiT8

References

Artime, T. M., and Peterson, Z. D. (2015). Feelings of wantedness and consent during nonconsensual sex: implications for posttraumatic cognitions. Psychol. Trauma 7:570. doi: 10.1037/tra0000047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bachman, R. (1993). Predicting the reporting of rape victimizations: have rape reforms made a difference? Crim. Justice Behav. 20, 254–270. doi: 10.1177/0093854893020003003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bengtsson, M. (2016). How to plan and perform a qualitative study using content analysis. NursingPlus Open 2, 8–14. doi: 10.1016/j.npls.2016.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beres, M. (2014). Rethinking the concept of consent for anti-sexual violence activism and education. Feminism Psychol. 24, 373–389. doi: 10.1177/0959353514539652

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beres, M., Herold, E., and Maitland, S. (2004). Sexual consent behaviors in same-sex relationships. Arch. Sex. Behav. 33, 475–486. doi: 10.1023/b:aseb.0000037428.41757.10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beres, M. A. (2007). “Spontaneous” sexual consent: an analysis of sexual consent literature. Feminism Psychol. 17, 93–108. doi: 10.1177/0959353507072914

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binet, L., and Field, P. (2009). Empirical generalizations about advertising campaign success. J. Advert. Res. 49:130. doi: 10.2501/s0021849909090163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blue Seat Studios (2015). Tea Consent [Video file]. Available online at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=oQbei5JGiT8 (accessed September 13, 2017).

Google Scholar

Braun, V., and Clarke, V. (2013). Successful Qualitative Research: A Practical Guide for Beginners. London: Sage, 1–18.

Google Scholar

Brecklin, L. R., and Forde, D. R. (2001). A meta-analysis of rape education programs. Violence Victims 16:303. doi: 10.1891/0886-6708.16.3.303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camilleri, M., Kohut, T., and Fisher, W. (2015). Condom use behavioural skills mediate the relationship between condom use motivation and condom use behaviour among young adult heterosexual males: an information-motivation-behavioural skills analysis. Can. J. Hum. Sex. 24, 131–140. doi: 10.3138/cjhs.242-a1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, S. (1997). Content analysis: concepts, methods and applications. Nurse Res. 4, 5–16. doi: 10.7748/nr.4.3.5.s2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corrigan, P. W., and Penn, D. L. (1999). Lessons from social psychology on discrediting psychiatric stigma. Am. Psychol. 54, 765–776. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.54.9.765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeGue, S., Valle, L. A., Holt, M. K., Massetti, G. M., Matjasko, J. L., and Tharp, A. T. (2014). A systematic review of primary prevention strategies for sexual violence perpetration. Aggress. Violent Behav. 19, 346–362. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2014.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donat, P. L., and White, J. W. (2000). “Re-examining the issue of nonconsent in acquaintance rape,” in Psychology of women; 4. Sexuality, Society, and Feminism, eds C. B. Travis and J. W. White (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association).

Google Scholar

Dripps, D. A. (1992). Beyond rape: an essay on the difference between the presence of force and the absence of consent. Colum. L. Rev. 92:1780. doi: 10.2307/1123045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Estrich, S. (1987). Real Rape. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Fabiano, P. M., Perkins, H. W., Berkowitz, A., Linkenbach, J., and Stark, C. (2003). Engaging men as social justice allies in ending violence against women: evidence for a social norms approach. J. Am. Coll. Health 52, 105–112. doi: 10.1080/07448480309595732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

George, W. H., and Martínez, L. J. (2002). Victim blaming in rape: effects of victim and perpetrator race, type of rape, and participant racism. Psychol. Women Q. 26, 110–119. doi: 10.1111/1471-6402.00049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, D. S. (1998). Consent for sexual behavior in a college student population. Electron. J. Hum. Sex. 1, 1–16.

Google Scholar

Hester, M., and Lilley, S. (2016). Rape investigation and attrition in acquaintance, domestic violence and historical rape cases. J. Investig. Psychol. Offender Prof. 14, 175–188. doi: 10.1002/jip.1469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickman, S., and Muehlenhard, C. (1999). “By the semi-mystical appearance of a condom”: how young women and men communicate sexual consent in heterosexual situations. J. Sex Res. 36, 258–272. doi: 10.1080/00224499909551996

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hills, P. J., Seib, E., Pleva, M., Smythe, J., Gosling, M., and Cole, T. (2020). Consent, wantedness and pleasure: three dimensions affecting the perceived stress of and judgements of rape in sexual encounters. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 26, 171–197. doi: 10.1037/xap0000221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hockett, J. M., Smith, S. J., Klausing, C. D., and Saucier, D. A. (2016). Rape myth consistency and gender differences in perceiving rape victims: a meta-analysis. Violence Against Women 22, 139–167. doi: 10.1177/1077801215607359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Home Office (2003). Sexual Offences Act. London: Crown Copyright.

Google Scholar

Humphreys, T. (2007). Perceptions of sexual consent: the impact of relationship history and gender. J. Sex Res. 44, 307–315. doi: 10.1080/00224490701586706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphreys, T. P. (2004). “Understanding sexual consent: an empirical investigation of the normative script for young heterosexual adults,” in Making Sense of Sexual Consent, eds M. Cowling, and P. Reynolds (Abingdon: Routledge), 209–225.

Google Scholar

James, N. (2018). Gender differences in attitudes towards sexual assault. J. Appl. Psychol. Soc. Sci. 4, 83–100.

Google Scholar

Jozkowski, K. N. (2015). “Yes means yes”? Sexual consent policy and college students. Change 47, 16–23. doi: 10.1080/00091383.2015.1004990

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keller, S. N., and Brown, J. D. (2002). Media interventions to promote responsible sexual behavior. J. Sex Res. 39, 67–72. doi: 10.1080/00224490209552123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krahé, B., Temkin, J., Bieneck, S., and Berger, A. (2008). Prospective lawyers’ rape stereotypes and schematic decision making about rape cases. Psychol. Crime Law 14, 461–479. doi: 10.1080/10683160801932380

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Legislation gov uk (2018). Juries Act 1974. Available online at: https://www.legislation.gov.uk/ukpga/1974/23 (accessed April 4, 2018).

Google Scholar

Lonsway, K. A. (1996). Preventing acquaintance rape through education what do we know. Psychol. Women Q. 20, 229–265. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1996.tb00469.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luginbuhl, J., and Mullin, C. (1981). Rape and responsibility: how and how much is the victim blamed? Sex Roles 7, 547–559. doi: 10.1007/bf00288631

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., and Jetten, J. (1994). Out of mind but back in sight: stereotypes on the rebound. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 808–817. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.5.808

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayers, A. (2013). Introduction to Statistics and SPSS in Psychology. London: Pearson Higher Ed.

Google Scholar

McEwan, J. (2005). Proving consent in sexual cases: legislative change and cultural evolution. Int. J. Evid. Proof 9, 1–28. doi: 10.1350/ijep.9.1.1.64789

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McHugh, J. T. (1996). Interpreting the sexual contract in pennsylvania: the motivations and legacy of commonwealth of Pennsylvania v. Robert Berkowitz. Alb. L. Rev. 60:1677.

Google Scholar

Ministry of Justice Home Office Office for National Statistics (2013). An Overview of Sexual Offending in England and Wales. Ministry of Justice, Home Office & the Office for National Statistics. Available online at: http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20160105160709/https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/214970/sexual-offending-overview-jan-2013.pdf (accessed April 3, 2018).

Google Scholar

Monson, C. M., Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J., and Binderup, T. (2000). Does “no” really mean “no” after you say “yes”? Attributions about date and marital rape. J. Interpers. Violence 15, 1156–1174. doi: 10.1177/088626000015011003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moszynski, P. (2004). Rape victims in Sudan face life of stigma, says report. BMJ Br. Med. J. 329:251.

Google Scholar

Muehlenhard, C. L., Humphreys, T. P., Jozkowski, K. N., and Peterson, Z. D. (2016). The complexities of sexual consent among college students: a conceptual and empirical review. J. Sex Res. 53, 457–487. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1146651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuschatz, J. S., Lampinen, J. M., Preston, E. L., Hawkins, E. R., and Toglia, M. P. (2002). The effect of memory schemata on memory and the phenomenological experience of naturalistic situations. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 16, 687–708. doi: 10.1002/acp.824

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogunfowokan, A., and Fajemilehin, R. (2012). Impact of a school-based sexual abuse prevention education program on the knowledge and attitude of high school girls. J. Sch. Nurs. 28, 459–468. doi: 10.1177/1059840512446949

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Sullivan, L. F., and Allgeier, E. R. (1998). Feigning sexual desire: consenting to unwanted sexual activity in heterosexual dating relationships. J. Sex Res. 35, 234–243. doi: 10.1080/00224499809551938

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennington, N., and Hastie, R. (1986). Evidence evaluation in complex decision making. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 242–258.

Google Scholar

Peter-Hagene, L. C., and Ullman, S. E. (2018). Longitudinal effects of sexual assault victims’ drinking and self-blame on posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Interpers. Violence 33, 83–93. doi: 10.1177/0886260516636394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, Z. D., and Muehlenhard, C. L. (2007). Conceptualizing the “wantedness” of women’s consensual and nonconsensual sexual experiences: implications for how women label their experiences with rape. J. Sex Res. 44, 72–88. doi: 10.1080/00224490709336794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollard, P. (1992). Judgements about victims and attackers in depicted rapes: a review. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 31, 307–326. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1992.tb00975.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potter, S. J., Moynihan, M. M., Stapleton, J. G., and Banyard, V. L. (2009). Empowering bystanders to prevent campus violence against women: a preliminary evaluation of a poster campaign. Violence Against Women 15, 106–121. doi: 10.1177/1077801208327482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proite, R., Dannells, M., and Benton, S. L. (1993). Gender, sex-role stereotypes, and the attribution of responsibility for date and acquaintance rape. J. College Stud. Dev. 34, 411–417.

Google Scholar

Resick, P. A. (1993). The psychological impact of rape. J. Interpers. Violence 8, 223–255. doi: 10.1177/088626093008002005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, D., and Campbell, J. L. (1982). Alcohol and rape: the effect of alcohol on attributions of blame for rape. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 8, 468–476. doi: 10.1177/0146167282083013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, K. M. (2011). The relationship between rape myths and sexual scripts: the social construction of rape. Sex Roles 65, 774–782. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-0033-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sable, M. R., Danis, F., Mauzy, D. L., and Gallagher, S. K. (2006). Barriers to reporting sexual assault for women and men: perspectives of college students. J. Am. Coll. Health 55, 157–162. doi: 10.3200/jach.55.3.157-162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schewe, P. A. (2002). “Guidelines for developing rape prevention and risk reduction interventions,” in Preventing Violence in Relationships: Interventions Across the Life Span, ed. P. A. Schewe (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 107–136. doi: 10.1037/10455-005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutte, J. W., and Hosch, H. M. (1997). Gender differences in sexual assault verdicts: a meta-analysis. J. Soc. Behav. Pers. 12, 761–772.

Google Scholar

Stewart, M., Dobbin, S., and Gatowski, S. (1996). “Real rapes” and “real victims”: the shared reliance on common cultural definitions of rape. Feminist Legal Stud. 4, 159–177. doi: 10.1007/bf02167608

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stirling, J. L., Hills, P. J., and Wignall, L. (2020). Narrative approach to understand people’s comprehension of acquaintance rape: the role of sex role stereotyping. Psychol. Sex. 1–18.

Google Scholar

Temkin, J. (2000). Prosecuting and defending rape: perspectives from the bar. J. Law Soc. 27, 219–248. doi: 10.1111/1467-6478.0015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Temkin, J., and Krahé, B. (2008). Sexual Assault and The Justice Gap: A Question of Attitude. London: Bloomsbury Publishing.

Google Scholar

Thames Valley Police. (2015). Consent Is Everything. Available online at: https://www.thamesvalley.police.uk/police-forces/thames-valley-police/areas/c/2017/consent-is-everything/ (accessed May 24, 2020).

Google Scholar

Tieger, T. (1981). Self-rated likelihood of raping and the social perception of rape. J. Res. Pers. 15, 147–158. doi: 10.1016/0092-6566(81)90014-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waldby, C., Kippax, S., and Crawford, J. (1990). Theory in the bedroom: a report from the Macquarie University AIDS and heterosexuality project. Austral. J. Soc. Issues 25, 177–185. doi: 10.1002/j.1839-4655.1990.tb00882.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weidner, G., and Griffitt, W. (1983). Rape: a sexual stigma? J. Pers. 51, 151–166. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1983.tb00859.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weller, S. (1992). Why is date rape so hard to prove? Health 6, 62–64.

Google Scholar

Wertheimer, A. (2003). Consent to Sexual Relations. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B., Resnick, H. S., McCauley, J. L., Amstadter, A. B., Kilpatrick, D. G., and Ruggiero, K. J. (2011). Is reporting of rape on the rise? A comparison of women with reported versus unreported rape experiences in the National Women’s Study-Replication. J. Interpersonal Violence 26, 807–832. doi: 10.1177/0886260510365869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: rape, perceptions, campaigns, attitudes, justice

Citation: Rowe EM and Hills PJ (2020) The Effect of Passively Viewing a Consent Campaign Video on Attitudes Toward Rape. Front. Psychol. 11:1741. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01741

Received: 28 February 2020; Accepted: 23 June 2020;
Published: 28 July 2020.

Edited by:

Katherine Louise Appleford, Kingston University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Emma Sleath, Coventry University, United Kingdom
Irina Anderson, University of East London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2020 Rowe and Hills. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Peter J. Hills, phills@bournemouth.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.