Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 16 November 2020
Sec. Eating Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic Mind the Sustainable Food: New Insights in Food Psychology View all 16 articles

How to Effectively Encourage Sustainable Food Choices: A Mini-Review of Available Evidence

  • School of Geography, Environment and Earth Sciences, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand

Food choices are difficult to change. People’s individual motivations (such as taste, cost, and food preferences) can be at odds with the negative environmental outcomes of their food choices (such as deforestation, water pollution, and climate change). How then can people be encouraged to adopt more sustainable food choices? This rapid review uses a dual-processing framework of decision-making to structure an investigation of the effectiveness of interventions to encourage sustainable food choices (e.g., local and organic food consumption, reducing meat and dairy intake, reducing food waste) via voluntary behavior change. The review includes interventions that rely on fast, automatic decision-making processes (e.g., nudging) and interventions that rely on more deliberate decision-making (e.g., information provision). These interventions have varying degrees of success in terms of encouraging sustainable food choices. This mini-review outlines some of the ways in which our understanding of sustainable food choices could be enhanced. This includes a call for the inclusion of possible moderators and mediators (past behavior, attitudes, beliefs, values) as part of effect measurements, because these elucidate the mechanisms by which behavior change occurs. In light of the climate change challenge, studies that include long-term effect measurements are essential as these can provide insight on how to foster sustained and durable changes.

Introduction

Encouraging people to adopt environmentally sustainable diets is an important step toward lowering greenhouse gas emissions. Several studies indicate that individual food-related behaviors–adopting plant-based diets, buying foods with a low carbon footprint, recycling of edible food waste–have significant impacts on overall emission reductions (Steinfeld et al., 2006; Parfitt et al., 2010; Berners-Lee, 2011; Garnett, 2013, 2016; Gerber et al., 2013; Hoolohan et al., 2013; Godfray et al., 2018). This suggests that encouraging the uptake of environmentally sustainable food behaviors can have a substantive impact on limiting climate change.

Encouraging people to alter their food choices is notoriously difficult (Nestle et al., 1998). Large-scale initiatives, such as the “5-a-day” campaign promoting fruit and vegetable intake, are well-known among the general public, but have not necessarily resulted in substantive changes in behavior (Wood, 2019). In the field of environmentally sustainable food choices, a growing body of intervention studies can help shed light on the efficacy of behavior change interventions.

This integrative mini-review (note that this is not a systematic literature review) summarizes what is known about the effectiveness of interventions to encourage environmentally sustainable food choices. It draws on a range of peer-reviewed studies, from randomized control trials to pre-test/post-test design, conducted in a variety of settings, from university cafeterias to convenience stores, and a variety of food-related behaviors. The methods that were used to select the studies, including keyword searches, inclusion and exclusion criteria, and the time period covered, can be found in the Supplementary Materials. The two overarching questions of this review are: how effective are behavior change interventions to encourage environmentally sustainable food choices and what psychological mechanisms can account for the effects?

Theoretical Framework

In psychology, human behavior is often explained via dual-process theories of behavior (Evans, 2008). Dual-process theories of behavior posit that there are two distinct processes that govern decision-making. One is automatic, quick and unconscious, the other is deliberative, slow, and conscious (Kahneman, 2011). Some decisions are informed by the central route (requiring cognitive effort) and other decisions are guided by the peripheral route of information processing (based on cues and heuristics) (Petty and Cacioppo, 1986).

Several scholars argue that food choices are habitual: they are automatic responses to cues in the environment (Wood and Neal, 2009; Lally et al., 2010; Neal et al., 2011). Neal et al. (2011) found that when people in a cinema were given a box of 7 days old stale popcorn, those with strong popcorn eating habits ate 70% of the stale popcorn. What is more, nobody liked the stale popcorn. The cue (watching a movie in a cinema) made the response (eating popcorn) more or less automatic, regardless of people’s motivations (disliking the popcorn). Other researchers propose that food choices are (also) driven by a deliberate decision-making process. For example, a recent meta-analysis found that people’s organic food choices are strongly associated with attitudes toward organic foods as well as past behavior (a proxy for habits) (Nardi et al., 2019).

The distinction between fast and slow decision-making processes could help explain the (in)effectiveness of behavior change interventions. For example, one of the reasons why the “five-a-day” campaign may not have resulted in increased fruit and vegetable consumption is that this type of information provision relies on the slow mode of processing. If fruit and vegetable consumption is indeed habitual behavior, information provision will not change people’s behavior. Rather, cues in the environment could be altered (placing fruits by the check-out counter; see Kroese et al., 2016) to facilitate the desired behavior.

This integrative mini-review (please see Supplementary Materials for methods detailing study selection) uses a dual-processing framework to structure an investigation of the effectiveness of interventions to encourage environmentally sustainable food choices. The review includes nudging, food labels, visual prompts, information provision, and the use of social norms (for detailed information about each study, see Table 1). This review has two main aims: (i) examine the effectiveness of behavior change interventions and (ii) explore underlying psychological mechanisms that can help explain why an intervention is (in)effective. In doing so, this review summarizes recent advances and the current state of our understanding in the field.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary table of key characteristics of the intervention studies included in this mini-review.

Overview of Behavior Change Interventions

Nudging

Nudges involve a (simple) change to the context in which people make decisions (Thaler and Sunstein, 2009). Nudges do not change economic incentives or ban certain products. Rather, nudges steer people toward the desired behavior by changing the choice architecture. Different types of nudges have been implemented in food research, including changes to the default (e.g., labeling a vegetarian option the “dish of the day”) and changes to the food environment (e.g., placing healthy foods by the check-out counter instead of unhealthy foods; increasing the availability of vegetarian options on a menu).

The assumptions that underlie nudging are grounded in behavioral economics. Behavioral economics identifies common patterns of thinking that deviate from the assumption that people are rational decision makers (Sunstein, 2014). Nudging interventions alter the choice architecture (e.g., the food environment) so that people’s automatic, quick mode of decision making is activated. This suggests that nudging might be particularly effective in changing behaviors that rely on automatic processes, such as food choices (van Kleef and van Trijp, 2018; Vecchio and Cavallo, 2019).

Changes to the Default

One version of nudging consists of labeling a specific menu item as “dish of the day,” or “Chef’s recommendation.” While scenario studies (involving hypothetical meal choices) have shown promising effects of this type of nudging on vegetarian meal choices (e.g., Campbell-Arvai et al., 2014; Bacon and Krpan, 2018), experimental field studies do not seem to observe significant effects. In a randomized controlled field experiment conducted in four European countries, Zhou et al. (2019) found that labeling plant-based options as “dish of the day” did not influence people’s meal choices in a restaurant setting. Study findings by dos Santos et al. (2018) also indicate that a “dish of the day” nudge in cafeterias did not increase the uptake of plant-based meals.

Changes to the Food Environment

Other applications of nudging involve changing something in the food environment to encourage sustainable food choices. Kurz (2018), for example, found that when the vegetarian option on a menu was made more visible (putting it on the counter where customers placed their order) sales of vegetarian dishes showed a small but significant increase relative to baseline. Altering the availability or portion size is another form of changing the food environment. Garnett et al. (2019) varied the vegetarian offerings in three University of Cambridge college cafeterias and collected individual-level meal selection data. A doubling of the availability of vegetarian offerings (from 25 to 50%) was associated with an 8% increase in sales, compared with a control group. Similarly, Vandenbroele et al. (2018) found that adding smaller portion sizes to a retailer’s assortment reduced the total volume of meat sold, relative to a control retailer.

Carbon and Environmental Labels

Studies show that people are generally unaware of the extent to which their food choices impact the environment (e.g., de Boer et al., 2016). Carbon labels can provide insight into the climate impact of a particular food. Environmental (or eco) labels provide a holistic overview of impacts, such as land use changes, deforestation, water use, pesticide use and greenhouse gas emissions. These environmental impacts are often estimated via Life Cycle Analysis (LCA), whereby impacts associated with all phases of a product’s life cycle (production, distribution, consumption, and disposal) are added up (see for example Berners-Lee, 2011; Berners-Lee et al., 2012; Hallström et al., 2015). Food labels are a type of information provision that guide food choices in the food environment, when people make decisions about which product to buy.

Researchers have proposed that food labels affect people’s food choices by virtue of being an environmental label (e.g., Vlaeminck et al., 2014). Such labels might “prime” people to choose an environmentally friendly food product via a quick, unconscious decision-making process. Guéguen et al. (2012), for example, found that when menus contained watermark visual cues related to the sea, diners were more likely to choose fish dishes. Other scholars, in contrast, would suggest that a more conscious and deliberate process is involved: carbon labels activate people’s environmental values and beliefs, which in turn influence food choices. Empirical studies indicate that the effect of carbon labels on food choices depends on people’s levels of environmental concern (e.g., Thøgersen, 2000; Grunert et al., 2014; Shewmake et al., 2015; Thøgersen and Nielsen, 2016).

Food labels seem to have a positive, but modest effect on people’s food choices (see Table 1). In a randomized field experiment in Swedish retail stores, Elofsson et al. (2016) found that when milk carried a “climate certified” label, daily sales increased by approximately 6% relative to a standard milk label. A study in an Australian convenience store by Vanclay et al. (2011) observed an increase in sales of food products that carried a “green” low carbon label, relative to products with a higher carbon impact. However, this study also found that carbon labels did not necessarily discourage consumers from buying products with a high climate impact, such as milk (see also Vlaeminck et al., 2014 for a similar finding).

Carbon labels have also been used alongside other interventions (Spaargaren et al., 2013; Visschers and Siegrist, 2015; Brunner et al., 2018). For example, Brunner et al. (2018) developed carbon labels (using a green/yellow/red traffic light system to indicate climate impact) for dishes in a university student cafeteria in Gothenburg, Sweden. In addition, information about links between food and climate change was provided via a website and posters in the cafeteria. While there was a significant 11.5% increase in sales of climate-friendly green dishes during the label phase (compared with baseline), there were no changes in sales of yellow-or red-labeled meat dishes (i.e., dishes with a higher climate impact). Because a combination of information provision and food labels was used in these studies, it is difficult to attribute any effect of the use of food labels alone.

The Provision of Information

In contrast to food labels, which guide food choices “in the moment” (i.e., when people are in a supermarket), information provision generally occurs outside the food environment. This can be, for example, mass media information campaigns, or guidelines from the government (e.g., the ever-changing “food pyramid”; see Nestle, 2013). The provision of information or education is based on a “knowledge-deficit” approach and assumes that when people have more information and “better” knowledge, that behavior change will follow. As such, information provision generally assumes a deliberate, conscious decision-making process.

For example, Monroe et al. (2015) developed an interactive web-based intervention to encourage the uptake of environmentally friendly eating behaviors among university students. The intervention consisted of modules on local food, food waste, and environmentally friendly protein and was displayed as text, pictures, video clips and interactive quizzes. A significant increase in self-reported green eating behaviors was observed, relative to a control group. Carfora et al. (2019) found that text messages about health or environmental benefits (combined with a reminder to reduce meat consumption) were associated with a reduction in self-reported red meat consumption immediately following the intervention and a follow-up 1 month later.

Bernstad et al. (2013) found that while written information was not effective in encouraging food waste recycling, when the same information was delivered in a face-to-face format, it did change behavior. In a separate study, Bernstad (2014) found that written information did not result in behavior change, but the subsequent installation of waste sorting equipment was associated with a significant 49% increase in the amount of recycled food waste. Linder et al. (2018) developed information that specifically addressed key barriers to recycling food waste (based on interviews with residents) and found that the provision of targeted information was associated with a significant 26% increase in food waste recycling (relative to baseline).

Visual Prompts

Visual prompts are a brief form of information provision that act as a reminder to engage in a certain behavior (e.g., stickers, posters, signs, flyers). Prompts appear to be most effective when the behavior is easy to do (Abrahamse and Matthies, 2018) and when people are already motivated to perform the behavior (Schultz, 2014). Prompts can act as cues and promote behaviors via a quick decision-making process. Indeed, some researchers refer to prompts as “nudges” (e.g., Shearer et al., 2017).

In a randomized control trial, Shearer et al. (2017) found that placing a sticker on general waste bins reminding people to recycle their food waste (“No food waste please. Remember to use your food recycling caddy”) increased the amount of recycled food waste by 20%, relative to a control group (no sticker on bin). Whitehair et al. (2013) examined the effectiveness of a visual prompt to reduce edible food waste in a university dining facility. When a visual prompt was introduced (reminding students to not waste food), the amount of edible food waste was reduced by 15%. When information was then provided on how much food waste was generated in the cafeteria, this did not have an additional effect. This may be because telling students that a lot of food is wasted may have (inadvertently) made a social norm salient (cf. Cialdini, 2003) that everybody wastes food. Sussman et al. (2013) observed that a visual prompt was associated with a significant increase in composting behavior in a repeated measures study. In a between-subjects study, however, they found that a prompt did not influence composting behavior (Sussman and Gifford, 2013).

Social Norms

Social norms refer to the notion that behavior is influenced by what other people do (descriptive social norms) and what people think is expected of them (injunctive norms) (Cialdini, 2003). Social norms influence behavioral choices when they are made salient. There is some evidence to suggest that people differ in the degree to which they are susceptible to social norms (e.g., Stöckli and Hofer, 2020). This implies that people may not necessarily follow social norms because these norms are “cued,” but because they are important to people in their deliberate decision making.

Social norms are used as part of information provision or feedback provision and sometimes as part of short prompts. Sparkman and Walton (2017) used social norms as part of information provision to encourage a reduction in meat consumption in a campus cafeteria. The authors examined the effect of descriptive social norms (the % of other people who do a behavior) with so-called “dynamic” social norms (norms about the changes in behavior other people engage in). Patrons who were given information about dynamic norms (“30% of Americans have started to make an effort to limit their meat consumption”) were significantly more likely to choose a meatless lunch, compared with a descriptive social norm message and control. Stöckli et al. (2018) found that a standard prompt (encouraging people to ask for a takeaway box for any leftovers) and a prompt with a descriptive norm message (i.e., “many guests ask us to wrap their pizza leftovers”) were associated with an increase in patrons asking for takeaway boxes. The normative prompt was no more effective than the standard prompt.

Nomura et al. (2011) conducted a randomized control trial to examine the effect of social norm feedback on participation in a food waste reduction scheme. They found that households in the social norm group significantly increased participation rates, relative to a control group. Households who had received feedback accompanied by a smiley face (the street performed better than average) and those who had received a sad face (the street performed worse than average) had higher participation rates relative to control streets.

Psychological Mechanisms: Exploring Mediators and Moderators

It is important to consider the psychological mechanisms through which interventions result in behavior change. Relatively few studies in this review included potential moderators or mediators and what follows illustrates what some of these mechanisms might be.

Past Behavior

Past behavior refers to the extent to which people engage in the target behavior prior to the intervention. In some cases, frequency of past behavior is used as an indicator of habit. Garnett et al. (2019) found that past behavior was a significant moderator of the effect of nudging on food choices. The effect of the nudge (increased availability of vegetarian meal choices) was stronger for those who would not normally eat vegetarian options. Scenario studies point to a similar effect. For example, Bacon and Krpan (2018) found that labeling a vegetarian option as “Chef’s Recommendation” (nudge) did not affect vegetarian meal choices, but the effect of nudging was moderated by past behavior. Infrequent vegetarians were more likely to choose the vegetarian option when this was presented as the recommended option, compared with frequent vegetarians.

Universalism Values

Studies have found that human values (i.e., guiding principles in people’s lives; Schwartz, 1994) are associated with sustainable food choices. Universalism values, for example, are (positively) associated with organic food choices (Vermeir and Verbeke, 2008) and vegetarianism (Hayley et al., 2015; Graham and Abrahamse, 2017). Universalism values are part of the self-transcendence dimension and reflect the value people place on care for nature.

Some studies have found that universalism values are predictive of food choices independently of the effect of an intervention (e.g., Campbell-Arvai et al., 2014; Zhou et al., 2019) Zhou et al. (2019) found that participants with stronger universalism values were more likely to choose plant-based options, irrespective of a nudge intervention. This suggests, perhaps, that for people with strong universalism values, the choice of plant-based meals is the result of deliberate decision-making and not easily changed by an intervention that relies more on automatic decision making.

It might be that values moderate the effect of an intervention on sustainable food choices, but the evidence for this is limited. Interventions to encourage sustainable food choices may well be more effective when people have stronger universalism values. For example, Graham and Abrahamse (2017) found that an informational message about the climate impacts of meat consumption was associated with stronger intentions to reduce meat consumption, particularly for people with strong self-transcendence values. However, none of the intervention studies included in this review reported possible moderating effects of universalism (or other) values.

Attitudes and Beliefs

The literature points to a close connection between people’s attitudes and beliefs and their food choices. Carfora et al. (2019) found that attitudes toward red meat mediated the effect of their text message intervention on red meat consumption. These text messages were associated with a more positive attitude toward reducing the consumption of red meat. This strengthened attitude, in turn, was associated with a reduction in red meat consumption. Lab studies have also found evidence for a mediating role of attitudes and beliefs. For example, Vainio et al. (2018) found that people’s prior beliefs about meat influenced the effectiveness of an informational message. Reading an informational message was only associated with stronger behavioral intentions among those who already held strong negative beliefs about meat (“meat-skeptics”) and not among so-called “meat believers.”

Discussion

The findings of this review indicate that interventions can be used effectively to encourage environmentally sustainable food choices. The review draws on a substantive body of research on this topic. This ranges from carefully crafted interventions that focus on people’s motivations and deliberate decision-making processes to interventions that involve simple changes in the choice architecture that facilitate certain behaviors in more “cued” and unconscious ways.

Nudging interventions have some potential to encourage sustainable food choices. Increasing the availability of vegetarian dishes was shown to be effective (Garnett et al., 2019) and so was a reduction in portion sizes of meat (Vandenbroele et al., 2018). However, a “dish of the day” approach seemed ineffective (Zhou et al., 2019). Overall, food labels are effective in encouraging sustainable food choices on their own (e.g., Vanclay et al., 2011) and as part of wider information campaigns about links between food and climate change (e.g., Spaargaren et al., 2013; Visschers and Siegrist, 2015). But it would appear that carbon labels do not necessarily discourage the uptake of products with a high climate impact, such as milk or meat (e.g., Vanclay et al., 2011; Vlaeminck et al., 2014; Brunner et al., 2018).

The provision of information alone is generally not considered to be an effective strategy for behavior change more generally (Abrahamse et al., 2005; Schultz, 2014). The findings in the area of sustainable food corroborate this (e.g., Bernstad, 2014). However, when information is crafted to address specific behavioral barriers (Linder et al., 2018), when it is combined with a motivational goal (Monroe et al., 2015; Carfora et al., 2019) or when it emphasizes social norms (Sparkman and Walton, 2017), information provision can have a positive impact.

The evidence-base for the effectiveness of interventions to encourage environmentally sustainable food choices is growing. However, more research is needed on possible mediators and moderators that can explain why a behavior change intervention was successful (or not). Including moderators or mediators, such as past behavior, cultural values, and prior beliefs and attitudes can provide valuable insights into the mechanisms by which interventions change behavior.

Only one intervention study (Zhou et al., 2019) examined cross-country differences in the effectiveness of a behavior change intervention (a “dish-of-the-day” nudge). While the nudge intervention was not effective in any of the four countries, the authors did observe that participants from the United Kingdom more often tried plant-based dishes compared with French participants. This could for example be due to a higher prevalence of vegetarianism in the United Kingdom relative to France. More research is needed to explore the role of social and cultural processes and how they are linked to food choices (for a review on this topic, see Carrus et al., 2018). More comparative research would also be useful to better understand the effectiveness of different interventions in different food environments (e.g., at home vs. a restaurant vs. a supermarket).

Lastly, relatively little is known about the long-term effects of interventions, as a majority of studies measured immediate, short term effects only. More research could be conducted on the durability of behavior change. Increasing the availability of vegetarian options may be effective in the short term (e.g., at the point of sale), but it is not clear whether this “nudge” will have the potential to affect behavior in other settings, or to instill durable changes (see also Ewert, 2020). This is an important area for future research, because moving toward the adoption of lower carbon diets will require sustained changes in behavior.

Author Contributions

WA conducted the literature search, conducted the literature review, and wrote the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.589674/full#supplementary-material

References

Abrahamse, W., and Matthies, E. (2018). “Informational strategies to promote pro-environmental behaviour: changing knowledge, awareness and attitudes,” in Environmental Psychology: An Introduction, eds L. Steg, and J. I. M. de Groot (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons), 263–272.

Google Scholar

Abrahamse, W., Steg, L., Vlek, C., and Rothengatter, T. (2005). A review of intervention studies aimed at household energy conservation. J. Environ. Psychol. 25, 273–291. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bacon, L., and Krpan, D. (2018). (Not) Eating for the environment: the impact of restaurant menu design on vegetarian food choice. Appetite 125, 190–200. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berners-Lee, M. (2011). How Bad Are Bananas? The Carbon Footprint of Everything. Vancouver: Greystone Books.

Google Scholar

Berners-Lee, M., Hoolohan, C., Cammack, H., and Hewitt, C. N. (2012). The relative greenhouse gas impacts of realistic dietary choices. Energy Policy 43, 184–190. doi: 10.1016/j.enpol.2011.12.054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstad, A. (2014). Household food waste separation behavior and the importance of convenience. Waste Manag. 34, 1317–1323. doi: 10.1016/j.wasman.2014.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstad, A., la Cour Jansen, J., and Aspegren, A. (2013). Door-stepping as a strategy for improved food waste recycling behaviour–Evaluation of a full-scale experiment. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 73, 94–103. doi: 10.1016/j.resconrec.2012.12.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunner, F., Kurz, V., Bryngelsson, D., and Hedenus, F. (2018). Carbon label at a university restaurant–label implementation and evaluation. Ecol. Econ. 146, 658–667. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2017.12.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bucher, T., Collins, C., Rollo, M. E., McCaffrey, T. A., De Vlieger, N., Van der Bend, D., et al. (2016). Nudging consumers towards healthier choices: a systematic review of positional influences on food choice. Br. J. Nutr. 115, 2252–2263. doi: 10.1017/s0007114516001653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell-Arvai, V., Arvai, J., and Kalof, L. (2014). Motivating sustainable food choices: the role of nudges, value orientation, and information provision. Environ. Behav. 46, 453–475. doi: 10.1177/0013916512469099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carfora, V., Catellani, P., Caso, D., and Conner, M. (2019). How to reduce red and processed meat consumption by daily text messages targeting environment or health benefits. J. Environ. Psychol. 65:101319. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrus, G., Pirchio, S., and Mastandrea, S. (2018). Social-cultural processes and urban affordances for healthy and sustainable food consumption. Front. Psychol. 9:2407. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cialdini, R. B. (2003). Crafting normative messages to protect the environment. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 12, 105–109. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01242

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Boer, J., de Witt, A., and Aiking, H. (2016). Help the climate, change your diet: a cross-sectional study on how to involve consumers in a transition to a low-carbon society. Appetite 98, 19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

dos Santos, Q., Nogueira, B. M., Rodrigues, V. M., Hartwell, H., Giboreau, A., Monteleone, E., et al. (2018). Nudging using the ‘dish of the day’ strategy does not work for plant-based meals in a Danish sample of adolescent and older people. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 42, 327–334. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12421

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elofsson, K., Bengtsson, N., Matsdotter, E., and Arntyr, J. (2016). The impact of climate information on milk demand: evidence from a field experiment. Food Policy 58, 14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.foodpol.2015.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, J. S. B. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 255–278. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ewert, B. (2020). Moving beyond the obsession with nudging individual behaviour: towards a broader understanding of behavioural public policy. Public Policy Adm. 35, 337–360. doi: 10.1177/0952076719889090

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garnett, E. E., Balmford, A., Sandbrook, C., Pilling, M. A., and Marteau, T. M. (2019). Impact of increasing vegetarian availability on meal selection and sales in cafeterias. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 20923–20929. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1907207116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garnett, T. (2013). Food sustainability: problems, perspectives and solutions. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 72, 29–39. doi: 10.1017/s0029665112002947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garnett, T. (2016). Plating up solutions. Science 353, 1202–1204. doi: 10.1126/science.aah4765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerber, P. J., Steinfeld, H., Henderson, B., Mottet, A., Opio, C., Dijkman, J., et al. (2013). Tackling Climate Change Through Livestock: A Global Assessment of Emissions and Mitigation Opportunities. Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations (FAO.

Google Scholar

Godfray, H. C. J., Aveyard, P., Garnett, T., Hall, J. W., Key, T. J., Lorimer, J., et al. (2018). Meat consumption, health, and the environment. Science 361:eaam5324.

Google Scholar

Graham, T., and Abrahamse, W. (2017). Communicating the climate impacts of meat consumption: The effect of values and message framing. Global Environ. Change 44, 98–108. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2017.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grunert, K. G., Hieke, S., and Wills, J. (2014). Sustainability labels on food products: consumer motivation, understanding and use. Food Policy 44, 177–189. doi: 10.1016/j.foodpol.2013.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guéguen, N., Jacob, C., and Ardiccioni, R. (2012). Effect of watermarks as visual cues for guiding consumer choice: an experiment with restaurant menus. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 31, 617–619. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2011.04.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallström, E., Carlsson-Kanyama, A., and Börjesson, P. (2015). Environmental impact of dietary change: a systematic review. J. Cleaner Prod. 91, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2014.12.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayley, A., Zinkiewicz, L., and Hardiman, K. (2015). Values, attitudes, and frequency of meat consumption. Pred. Meat Reduced Diet Aust. Appet. 84, 98–106. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoolohan, C., Berners-Lee, M., McKinstry-West, J., and Hewitt, C. N. (2013). Mitigating the greenhouse gas emissions embodied in food through realistic consumer choices. Energy Policy 63, 1065–1074. doi: 10.1016/j.enpol.2013.09.046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York, NY: Macmillan.

Google Scholar

Kroese, F. M., Marchiori, D. R., and de Ridder, D. T. (2016). Nudging healthy food choices: a field experiment at the train station. J. Public Health 38, e133–e137.

Google Scholar

Kurz, V. (2018). Nudging to reduce meat consumption: immediate and persistent effects of an intervention at a university restaurant. J. Environ. Econ. Manag. 90, 317–341. doi: 10.1016/j.jeem.2018.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lally, P., Van Jaarsveld, C. H., Potts, H. W., and Wardle, J. (2010). How are habits formed: modelling habit formation in the real world. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 998–1009. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.674

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linder, N., Lindahl, T., and Borgström, S. (2018). Using behavioural insights to promote food waste recycling in urban households—Evidence from a longitudinal field experiment. Front. Psychol. 9:352. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monroe, J. T., Lofgren, I. E., Sartini, B. L., and Greene, G. W. (2015). The Green Eating Project: web-based intervention to promote environmentally conscious eating behaviours in US university students. Public Health Nutr. 18, 2368–2378. doi: 10.1017/s1368980015002396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nardi, V. A. M., Jardim, W. C., Ladeira, W., and Santini, F. (2019). Predicting food choice: a meta-analysis based on the theory of planned behavior. Br. Food J. 121, 2250–2264. doi: 10.1108/bfj-08-2018-0504

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neal, D. T., Wood, W., Wu, M., and Kurlander, D. (2011). The pull of the past: when do habits persist despite conflict with motives? Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 1428–1437. doi: 10.1177/0146167211419863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nestle, M. (2013). Food Politics: How the Food Industry Influences Nutrition and Health, Vol. 3. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Google Scholar

Nestle, M., Wing, R., Birch, L., DiSogra, L., Drewnowski, A., Middleton, S., et al. (1998). Behavioral and social influences on food choice. Nutr. Rev. 56(5 Pt 2), 50–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.1998.tb01732.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nomura, H., John, P. C., and Cotterill, S. (2011). The use of feedback to enhance environmental outcomes: a randomised controlled trial of a food waste scheme. Local Environ. 16, 637–653. doi: 10.1080/13549839.2011.586026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parfitt, J., Barthel, M., and Macnaughton, S. (2010). Food waste within food supply chains: quantification and potential for change to 2050. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 365, 3065–3081. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petty, R. E., and Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). “The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion,” In Communication and Persuasion. Springer Series in Social Psychology. New York, NY: Springer, 1–24. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4612-4964-1_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, P. (2014). Strategies for promoting proenvironmental behavior: lots of tools but few instructions. Eur. Psychol. 19:107. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of human values? J. Soc. Issues 50, 19–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1994.tb01196.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shearer, L., Gatersleben, B., Morse, S., Smyth, M., and Hunt, S. (2017). A problem unstuck? Evaluating the effectiveness of sticker prompts for encouraging household food waste recycling behaviour. Waste Manag. 60, 164–172. doi: 10.1016/j.wasman.2016.09.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shewmake, S., Okrent, A., Thabrew, L., and Vandenbergh, M. (2015). Predicting consumer demand responses to carbon labels. Ecol. Econ. 119, 168–180. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2015.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spaargaren, G., Van Koppen, C. S. A., Janssen, A. M., Hendriksen, A., and Kolfschoten, C. J. (2013). Consumer responses to the carbon labelling of food: a real life experiment in a canteen practice. Sociol. Ruralis 53, 432–453.

Google Scholar

Sparkman, G., and Walton, G. M. (2017). Dynamic norms promote sustainable behaviour, even if it is counternormative. Psychol. Sci. 28, 1663–1674. doi: 10.1177/0956797617719950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinfeld, H., Gerber, P., Wassenaar, T. D., Castel, V., Rosales, M., Rosales, M., et al. (2006). Livestock’s Long Shadow: Environmental Issues and Options. Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations (FAO).

Google Scholar

Stöckli, S., Dorn, M., and Liechti, S. (2018). Normative prompts reduce consumer food waste in restaurants. Waste Manag. 77, 532–536. doi: 10.1016/j.wasman.2018.04.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stöckli, S., and Hofer, D. (2020). Susceptibility to social influence predicts behavior on Facebook. PLoS One 15:e0229337. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0229337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sunstein, C. R. (2014). Nudging: a very short guide. J. Consum. Policy 37, 583–588. doi: 10.1007/s10603-014-9273-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sussman, R., and Gifford, R. (2013). Be the change you want to see: modeling food composting in public places. Environ. Behav. 45, 323–343. doi: 10.1177/0013916511431274

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sussman, R., Greeno, M., Gifford, R., and Scannell, L. (2013). The effectiveness of models and prompts on waste diversion: a field experiment on composting by cafeteria patrons. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 43, 24–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00978.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taufik, D., Verain, M. C., Bouwman, E. P., and Reinders, M. J. (2019). Determinants of real-life behavioural interventions to stimulate more plant-based and less animal-based diets: a systematic review. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 93, 281–303. doi: 10.1016/j.tifs.2019.09.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaler, R. H., and Sunstein, C. R. (2009). Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness. London: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Thøgersen, J. (2000). Psychological determinants of paying attention to eco-labels in purchase decisions: model development and multinational validation. J. Consum. Policy 23, 285–313. doi: 10.1023/a:1007122319675

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thøgersen, J., and Nielsen, K. S. (2016). A better carbon footprint label. J. Cleaner Prod. 125, 86–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2016.03.098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vainio, A., Irz, X., and Hartikainen, H. (2018). How effective are messages and their characteristics in changing behavioural intentions to substitute plant-based foods for red meat? The mediating role of prior beliefs. Appetite 125, 217–224. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Kleef, E., and van Trijp, H. C. (2018). “Methodological challenges of research in nudging,” in Methods in Consumer Research, Vol. 1, eds G. Ares, and P. Varela (Cambridge: Woodhead Publishing), 329–349. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-08-102089-0.00013-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanclay, J. K., Shortiss, J., Aulsebrook, S., Gillespie, A. M., Howell, B. C., Johanni, R., et al. (2011). Customer response to carbon labelling of groceries. J. Consum. Policy 34, 153–160. doi: 10.1007/s10603-010-9140-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandenbroele, J., Slabbinck, H., Van Kerckhove, A., and Vermeir, I. (2018). Curbing portion size effects by adding smaller portions at the point of purchase. Food Qual. Prefer. 64, 82–87. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.10.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vecchio, R., and Cavallo, C. (2019). Increasing healthy food choices through nudges: a systematic review. Food Q. Prefe. 78:103714. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.05.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermeir, I., and Verbeke, W. (2008). Sustainable food consumption among young adults in Belgium: theory of planned behaviour and the role of confidence and values. Ecol. Econ. 64, 542–553. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2007.03.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visschers, V. H., and Siegrist, M. (2015). Does better for the environment mean less tasty? Offering more climate-friendly meals is good for the environment and customer satisfaction. Appetite 95, 475–483. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vlaeminck, P., Jiang, T., and Vranken, L. (2014). Food labeling and eco-friendly consumption: experimental evidence from a Belgian supermarket. Ecol. Econ. 108, 180–190. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2014.10.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitehair, K. J., Shanklin, C. W., and Brannon, L. A. (2013). Written messages improve edible food waste behaviors in a university dining facility. J. Acad. Nutr. Dietet. 113, 63–69. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2012.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, W. (2019). In: Creatures of Habit: How habits shape who we are – and who we become. Vedantam, S. (Host). (2015-present). Hidden brain [Audio podcast]. NPR. https://www.npr.org/transcripts/787160734?t=1588769290406

Google Scholar

Wood, W., and Neal, D. T. (2009). The habitual consumer. J. Consum. Psychol. 19, 579–592. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2009.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, X., Perez-Cueto, F. J., Dos Santos, Q., Bredie, W. L., Molla-Bauza, M. B., Rodrigues, V. M., et al. (2019). Promotion of novel plant-based dishes among older consumers using the ‘dish of the day’ as a nudging strategy in 4 EU countries. Food Q. Prefer. 75, 260–272. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sustainable food choices, interventions, nudges, prompts, information provision, social norms

Citation: Abrahamse W (2020) How to Effectively Encourage Sustainable Food Choices: A Mini-Review of Available Evidence. Front. Psychol. 11:589674. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.589674

Received: 31 July 2020; Accepted: 21 October 2020;
Published: 16 November 2020.

Edited by:

Valentina Carfora, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy

Reviewed by:

Therese Lindahl, Beijer Institute of Ecological Economics, Sweden
Miranda Mirosa, University of Otago, New Zealand

Copyright © 2020 Abrahamse. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wokje Abrahamse, wokje.abrahamse@vuw.ac.nz

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.