Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 12 January 2021
Sec. Organizational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Age-Based Stereotype Threat Effects on Performance Outcomes View all 9 articles

The Moderating Role of Education on the Relationship Between Perceived Stereotype Threat and False Memory in Aging

  • 1Laboratoire de Psychologie des Pays de la Loire (LPPL – EA 4638), Université de Nantes, Nantes, France
  • 2Laboratoire de Mathématiques Jean Leray (LMJL-UMR 6629), Université de Nantes, Nantes, France

Studies regularly show that an age-based stereotype threat impairs older adults’ performance on memory tasks. Results regarding stereotype threat effects on false memories are less clear. Some studies suggest that education may moderate the relationship between an age-related stereotype threat and episodic memory performance in older adults. The present study aimed at examining the moderating role of education on the relationship between perceived stereotype threat (PST) and false memories in older adults. With this aim, 82 adults between 60 and 70 years of age performed a Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) task followed by a free recall test and completed questionnaires assessing both their perception of an age-based stereotype threat and their education level. Regression analyses showed no effect of PST on the production of critical lures. However, as was expected, our results showed that in higher educated older adults, as the perception of stereotype increases, the production of critical lures increases. These results confirm the moderating role of education and highlight its key role in the relationship between the age-based stereotype threat and older adults’ susceptibility to false memories.

Introduction

Age-related stereotypes are very present in our environment and our daily lives. Research shows that both young and older adults share many positive and negative perceptions and stereotypes about aging (e.g., Hummert et al., 1994; Grühn et al., 2011). However, people’s representations of older adults are more imbued with negative perceptions than those of younger adults (e.g., Kite et al., 2005; Hummert, 2011). The effects of negative age-related stereotypes on older adults’ episodic memory performance have been regularly highlighted in the literature. Several studies have shown that older participants previously subliminally exposed to negative age-related stereotypes performed worse in memory tasks than older adults exposed to positive age-related stereotypes or non-exposed to age-related stereotypes (e.g., Hess et al., 2004; Levy, 2009). In other studies, researchers observed that memory performance of participants aged 60 and over was worse (and the age-related difference increased) when the memory component of the task was emphasized than when it was not (e.g., Rahhal et al., 2001; Chasteen et al., 2005; Desrichard and Köpetz, 2005). In such laboratory conditions, older adults may perceive and experience what Steele and Aronson (1995, p. 797) called the stereotype threat, i.e., “being at risk of confirming, as self-characteristic, a negative stereotype about one’s group.”

A growing body of research has shown that an induced age-based stereotype threat impairs older adults’ performance in a large variety of cognitive tasks (e.g., Barber and Mather, 2014; Lamont et al., 2015; Barber, 2017) such as episodic memory (see Armstrong et al., 2017 for a recent meta-analysis on this topic). Several hypotheses have been proposed to explain these effects relying either on cognitive or on motivational mechanisms. According to the executive control interference hypothesis (Schmader and Johns, 2003), the stereotype threat takes up cognitive resources, thereby increasing the cognitive load placed upon the cognitive system (particularly upon working memory), which induces a performance decrease. Recent results showing a deleterious effect of an age-based stereotype threat on older adults’ associative memory (Brubaker and Naveh-Benjamin, 2018) or the use of controlled processes while strengthening the use of automatic processes (Mazerolle et al., 2012) offer support to this cognitive hypothesis. The motivational explanation is based on the regulatory focus theory (Higgins, 1997). Research suggests that when participants perform a memory task under a stereotype threat, they adopt a prevention focus, become more vigilant to errors and try to avoid committing errors as much as possible (see Barber, 2017 for an extensive presentation; Seibt and Förster, 2004; as opposed to a promotion focus leading participants to remember as many words as possible whatever the number of errors they would make). This motivational hypothesis finds support in studies showing a decrease in correct recalls and/or a decrease in memory errors (e.g., Hess et al., 2003, 2009; Barber and Mather, 2013a,b; Popham and Hess, 2015; Barber, 2017).

One type of memory errors that is likely to be affected by an age-based stereotype threat is false memory. False memories are usually defined as distorted memories of events that occurred or memories of events that did not happen (Roediger and McDermott, 1995) and are associated with high levels of certitude. One of the most widely used paradigm to study individuals’ susceptibility to false memories is the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM; Deese, 1959; Roediger and McDermott, 1995) paradigm, in which participants study lists of semantically related words (e.g., bed, awake, tired, dream, etc.) strongly associated with a critical lure that is never presented (e.g., sleep), before being asked to recognize and/or recall as many words as possible without guessing. Participants often wrongly recall or recognize critical lures, thus making false memories. A large amount of studies has focused on factors influencing the occurrence of false memories. Research has shown that factors, such as age (e.g., Tun et al., 1998; Dehon and Brédart, 2004), positive and high arousing moods (e.g., Storbeck and Clore, 2005; Corson and Verrier, 2007; Storbeck and Clore, 2011), cognitive style (e.g., Corson et al., 2009), stress (Payne et al., 2002), the use of long rather than short DRM lists (e.g., Robinson and Roediger, 1997), or a relational rather than an item-specific processing during encoding (e.g., Hunt and Einstein, 1981), increase the occurrence of false memories.

To our knowledge, only three studies investigated the effects of an induced age-based related stereotype threat on older adults’ propensity to false memories (Thomas and Dubois, 2011; Wong and Gallo, 2016; Smith et al., 2017), but these studies did not yield any consensual results. In these studies, participants were first presented with DRM lists, then some of them were exposed to a blatant age-based stereotype threat induction (by reading short paragraphs on age-related cognitive decline), and finally performed a recognition task. Thomas and Dubois (2011) observed that adults aged between 60 and 74 years were more inclined to falsely recognize critical lures in the age-based stereotype condition than in the control condition (reading a short text on language processing after encoding DRM lists). No significant effects of the age-based stereotype threat were found on hits. On the contrary, Wong and Gallo (2016) found that older adults (65–87 years) exposed to an age-based stereotype threat induction made fewer false alarms on the critical lures than their counterparts in the control condition who read an age-neutral text on language research. However, it is worth noting that Wong and Gallo (2016) had included a DRM warning before the participants performed the recognition task. Participants were informed that some of the words in the recognition task were strongly associated with words they had studied but that these words were not included in the initial list. Participants were then told to avoid confusion between those words and the words they had studied. As this warning may partly explain the contradictory results, and in an attempt to explain the discrepancies between Thomas and Dubois (2011) and Wong and Gallo (2016) studies, Smith et al. (2017) chose to include four experimental conditions combining the presence or the absence of an age-based stereotype threat and the presence or the absence of a DRM warning. This warning was presented before the recognition task and consisted of a short text explaining the principles of the DRM paradigm, i.e., that studied words were all related to another word that is often falsely remembered by participants. Participants aged 56–90 years thus performed a DRM associated with a recognition test in one of the four experimental conditions. Consistent with the results of Thomas and Dubois (2011), they showed that older adults exposed to negative age-related stereotypes made more false recognition of critical lures than older adults non-exposed to negative age-related stereotypes and did not replicate the results obtained in Wong and Gallo (2016). Interestingly, Smith et al. (2017) also highlighted the moderating role of several factors such as years of education and retirement status. They showed that older adults’ susceptibility to false recognitions (false memories) increases under the stereotype threat even more in retired or highly educated participants.

Armstrong et al. (2017) also investigated the potential moderator effects of education, age, and type of memory tests. In their meta-analysis comprising studies using blatant or subtle (e.g., non-direct communication of memory decline) stereotype threat manipulation, they concluded that neither the years of education nor the age moderated the association between the age-based stereotype threat and the episodic memory performance in older adults. They also showed that the age-based stereotype threat effect was significant in free recall tasks but not in cued-recall or recognition tasks. This is consistent with the idea that, in addition to aging, a stereotype threat reduces the ability to use strategic processes leading to larger memory impairments in older adults (e.g., Craik and McDowd, 1987; Mazerolle et al., 2012).

The objective of the present study was to investigate the moderator role of years of education on the relationship between the perceived age-based stereotype threat and the production of false memories in older adults. Unlike previous studies on false memories, we chose not to induce a stereotype threat but to assess instead the older adults’ perceived stereotype threat (PST) when performing such a DRM task. This allowed us to be closer to natural situations of memory examination that older adults encounter (e.g., Rahhal et al., 2001; Brubaker and Naveh-Benjamin, 2018). Besides, since the age-based stereotype threat is known to be higher under difficult tasks (e.g., Steele and Aronson, 1995), we decided to present adults aged 60–70 years with a DRM task associated with a free recall task. First, we expected older adults to correctly recall fewer words as the perception of stereotype threat increases. We also expected the relationship between participants’ perception of stereotype threat and correct recalls to increase with the participants’ level of education. Second, regarding false memories, we expected older adults to produce more critical lures as their perception of a stereotype threat increases. In line with Smith et al. (2017), we expected this relationship to be moderated by education. The relationship between participants’ perception of a stereotype threat and the production of critical lures should be stronger as education increases.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Eighty-two older adults (60–70 years, M = 64.65 years, SD = 3.15, 54% female) were recruited in senior centers and community dwelling homes in the area of Nantes. Participants were all native French speakers; they received no compensation for their participation. All participants gave their informed consent before their inclusion in the study. Global cognitive efficiency was assessed using the French version of the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE; Derouesné et al., 1999). All older adults scored equal or higher than 26 on the MMSE; results indicated a preserved global cognitive functioning (M = 28.74, SD = 1.18) in our participants. The French version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger et al., 1993) revealed no signs of trait-anxiety in our sample (MDState = 29.84, SDState = 8.62; MTrait = 37.09, SDTrait = 8.56). Participants also completed a demographic questionnaire comprising questions regarding the highest diploma they got, the age until which they have been to school, and their current or former occupation. These items were used to compute the Education variable, which is defined as the number of years of instruction since the age of 6 (as instruction was mandatory from the age of 6 in France at the time). Overall, our participants reported a mean of 12.72 years of education (SD = 3.12). Participant characteristics are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics: means, SD, and Pearson correlations with confidence intervals.

An ethics approval was not required for this study as per institutional and national guidelines and regulations. However, the study was carried out in accordance with University of Nantes ethics guidelines and the French law no. 2004-801 of August 6, 2004 relating to the protection of the natural persons with regard to the processing of personal data and amending law no. 78-17 of January 6, 1978 relating to data, files, and freedoms. Ethics was checked at the laboratory level. Participation in the research was voluntary, and the data were collected anonymously in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013). Participants were informed that they could receive a written document explaining the main results of the study upon request.

Materials

Perception of Stereotype Threat

Participants’ stereotype threat perception was assessed with two items drawn from Gaillard et al. (2011). Participants rated their level of agreement with each of the two statements (“I worry that my ability to perform well on this test is affected by my age” and “I worry that if I perform poorly on this test, the experimenter will attribute my poor performance to my age”) using a 7-point scale ranging from “1-strongly disagree” to “7-strongly agree.” As responses to these two statements were highly correlated (r = 0.66, p < 0.001), we computed one averaged score of PST.

DRM Task

The material used in the present study consisted of six DRM lists, each comprising 15 words, drawn from Corson and Verrier (2007). We created two orders of presentation of the six DRM lists and counterbalanced them across participants.

Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1.

Procedure

Participants were tested individually in a single session that lasted approximately 40 min. After giving their informed consent, participants completed the MMSE, the demographic questionnaire, the state-anxiety questionnaire, and then the DRM task. The DRM lists were presented orally by the experimenter, one at a time, at a rate of one word every 1.5 s. Immediately after each list presentation, participants were given 90 s maximum to perform an oral free recall task. Participants were told that the experimenter was more interested in the type of words (quality of the recall) than in the number of words they recall (quantity of the recall). Immediately after the DRM task, participants assessed their perception of stereotype threat and then their level of trait-anxiety. At the end of the session, participants were fully debriefed and thanked for their participation.

Results

The results are presented in the next three sections. We first examined the correlations between all the variables taken into account in our study. Second, we examined the moderator role of education on the relationship between PST and the correct recalls. Finally, we investigated the moderating role of education on the relationship between PST and false memories production (production of critical lures). Statistical analyses were performed using R software (Version 3.6.0; R Core Team, 2019) and JASP (Version 0.13.1; JASP Team, 2020).

Correlation Analyses

The analysis revealed that Education was negatively and significantly correlated with Age but positively and significantly correlated with global cognitive efficiency assessed with the MMSE. Higher levels of education were associated with higher global cognitive efficiency and younger age. As expected, the two measures of state‐ and trait-anxiety were positively and significantly correlated. In addition, proportions of correct recalls in the DRM task were significantly and positively correlated with Education and global cognitive efficiency but negatively with Age. No other significant correlation was found (see Table 1).

Correct Recalls

We conducted a regression analysis to investigate both the role of PST as a predictor of the proportion of correct recalls and the moderating role of Education on the proportions of correct recalls in the DRM task (see Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Hierarchical regression analyses for variables predicting the proportion of correct recalls.

First, PST and Education were introduced as predictors of the proportion of correct recalls (Model 1). The analysis yielded a significant main effect of Education (b = 0.01, p = 0.001, sr2 = 0.00). When the interaction between Education and PST was added to the model, the results indicated that 19% of the variance of the correct recalls is related to the model (p < 0.001). More precisely, Education was the only significant predictor of the proportion of correct recalls (b = 0.01, p = 0.002, sr2 = 0.11). Neither the PST (b = −0.01, p = 0.235) nor the Education × PST interaction (b = 0.01, p = 0.068) predicted the proportion of correct recalls (Model 2).

Finally, we added Age, MMSE, State-, and Trait-Anxiety to the previous model as controlled variables (Model 3). The overall fit increases significatively (R2 = 0.401, p < 0.001; ΔR2 = 0.209, p < 0.001) and MMSE became the only significant predictor (b = 0.04, p < 0.001, sr2 = 0.15) offsetting the effect of Education (b = 0.01, p = 0.176, sr2 = 0.02).

False Memories

We conducted a regression analysis to investigate both the role of PST as a predictor of the proportion of critical lures produced and the moderating role of Education on the proportions of critical lures produced in the DRM task (see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Hierarchical regression analyses for variables predicting the proportion of critical lures.

First, PST and Education were introduced as predictors of the proportion of critical lures. The analysis yielded no significant main effect (Model 1). When the interaction between Education and PST was introduced, the results indicated that the model predicted 8% of the total variance (p = 0.078). More precisely, neither the PST (b = 0.01, p = 0.455, sr2 = 0.01) nor the Education (b = −0.01, p = 0.364, sr2 = 0.01) predicted the proportion of critical lures (Model 2). However, results indicated a moderating role of Education with 8% of the total variance associated with the PST × Education interaction (b = 0.01, p = 0.013, sr2 = 0.08); the relation between PST and the production of critical lures depends on an individual’s education level.

When Age, MMSE, State-, and Trait-Anxiety were entered in the previous model as controlled variables, the overall fit of the model increases nonsignificantly (R2 = 0.11, p = 0.238; ΔR2 = 0.031, p = 0.638), but PST × Education interaction still was the only significant predictor (b = 0.01, p = 0.020, sr2 = 0.07) confirming the moderating role of Education on the relationship between PST and the production of critical lures (Model 3).

One way to visualize and summarize simply the moderated effect of Education is to follow the Johnson-Neyman technique (Johnson and Fay, 1950). From Model 2 in Table 3, we used this technique to determine the education level intervals in which the relationship between PST and the proportion of critical lures is significant or not significant (see Figure 1). When the level of education is between 6.9 and 15.8 years, the relationship between PST and the proportion of critical lures is not significant; when the level is above 15.8 years, an increase in PST is significantly related to an increase in the proportion of critical lures, and when the level is below 6.9 years, an increase in PST is significantly related to a decrease in the proportion of critical lures. For instance, for the higher educated group (>16 years), the slope of PST on the proportion of critical lures is greater than b = 0.06 (and significant). One should note, however, that lower levels of Education such as 6.9 years and less are scarce in adults now aged from 60 and over.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Representation of the slope of perceived stereotype threat on the production of critical lures according to the level of education. The range of observed values of Education is [8.00, 20.00].

Discussion

The present study was aimed at identifying the moderating role of education on the relationship between the perceived age-based stereotype threat and false memory. As expected, our results showed that the production of critical lures was best predicted by the PST × Education interaction. To our knowledge, this is the first study that shows an increase of false memories’ production in highly educated older adults as their perception of an age-based stereotype threat increases, without relying on any experimental stereotype threat manipulation. Our results are consistent with previous research that has shown that highly educated older adults are more susceptible to false memories (Smith et al., 2017) under the induced stereotype threat. These results support the idea that highly educated individuals may place a high value on their memory and feel especially threatened by age-based stereotypes in such a false memory task. Consistent with the cognitive hypothesis (Schmader and Johns, 2003), they may engage most of their cognitive resources in order to distance themselves from the stereotype, thereby leaving only few resources to correctly perform the task. The increase of false memories in these highly educated participants along with the increase of their perception of an age-based stereotype threat is also consistent with the increase of false alarms observed by Brubaker and Naveh-Benjamin (2018) in adults aged 65 and over under the stereotype threat and the increase of false memories showed by Thomas and Dubois (2011) and Smith et al. (2017). This is particularly interesting since, in contrast to most previous studies, we did not use experimental stereotype threat induction. This suggests that naturally occurring stereotype threat in testing situations (e.g., Brubaker and Naveh-Benjamin, 2018) may have as deleterious effects on cognitive performance as an induced stereotype threat.

The literature shows that the occurrence of false memories is usually the result of global, heuristic, relational, familiarity-based, or gist-based processing rather than in distinctive, item-specific, or verbatim processing (e.g., Smith and Hunt, 1998; Brainerd and Reyna, 2002; McCabe et al., 2004). Our results indicate that participants with a strong PST may have engaged in a familiarity-based or gist-based strategy rather than in a recollection or verbatim processing during the free recall task. This higher reliance on familiarity leads participants to falsely recall critical lures. This is consistent with the idea that under threat, participants may rely more on global, heuristic, or automatic processes than on analytic and controlled processes (Mazerolle et al., 2012). This strategy is quite maladaptive in the false memory context but is particularly effective when trying to recall studied words from DRM lists. Indeed, with regards to correct recalls, our results showed that education is the only predictor of participants’ performance; the more educated they are, the more they recall previously studied words. This positive effect of education is consistent with previous research showing that education, as a major component of cognitive reserve, is a protective factor against the decline of cognitive abilities in aging (e.g., Nyberg and Pudas, 2019; Clouston et al., 2020). However, when MMSE total score was controlled for, it became the only significant predictor of correct recalls, offsetting the effect of education. In addition, we showed a strong positive correlation between MMSE total score and the proportion of correct recalls. These results are consistent with previous studies that have highlighted the positive relationship between MMSE total score and episodic memory performance (e.g., Taylor et al., 1992; Aartsen et al., 2002). Our results also pointed to a positive correlation between MMSE total score and education. This is consistent with previous studies showing that higher levels of education are associated with better cognitive functioning (e.g., Crum, 1993; Albert et al., 1995; but see Bleecker et al., 1988) or that education is a good predictor of the MMSE total score (e.g., Bertolucci et al., 1994). Finally, we highlighted a significant negative correlation between age and education level which is consistent with previous studies showing that earlier born cohorts of adults received less education than those born later (e.g., Matthews et al., 2012).

Contrary to our expectations and to previous research, our results did not show any effect of a perceived of stereotype threat either on correct recalls (e.g., Hess et al., 2009; Barber and Mather, 2013b; Armstrong et al., 2017) or on false memories (Thomas and Dubois, 2011; Smith et al., 2017). One explanation might be that, in contrast with previous studies, we did not use a blatant stereotype threat induction using short texts describing cognitive and memory declines in aging. To be closer to natural situations of memory assessments, we chose to assess participants’ PST naturally occurring in a testing situation. Our participants reported scores of PST as high as those reported by the participants in Gaillard et al. (2011) who underwent an experimental stereotype threat induction. As participants were informed both in the consent form and in the instructions that the experiment was about memory, one cannot rule out the possibility that this constitutes a subtle induction of stereotype threat (Nguyen and Ryan, 2008; Marquet et al., 2016). Another explanation may lie in the fact that we used a DRM associated with a free recall task that, despite our specific instructions regarding the recall task, may have elicited a promotion focus in our participants. Since it has been argued that the stereotype threat induces a prevention focus (see Barber, 2017), the lack of regulatory fit may have led to these results. However, our study did not directly address the regulatory focus theory since we did not manipulate nor assess promotion or prevention foci in our participants. Future research using subtle induction of stereotype threat would benefit from investigating this issue further.

In conclusion, this study showed that an age-based stereotype threat may naturally occur under testing situations in adults aged between 60 and 70 and affect their susceptibility to false memories. The present study also highlighted the central role of education in the evaluation of memory in older adults. While it predicted correct recalls, it also moderated the effects of PST on false memories. These results encourage more consideration of the education level as a study variable in research on aging, and not just as a simple controlled variable.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

A-LG, CE, and FC contributed to conception and design of the study. J-MG conducted the statistical analyses. A-LG wrote the first draft of the manuscript. CE, FC, and J-MG provided comments on the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision and read and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Manon Bonneau, Soukaina Hasnaoui, Tiphaine Housty, Marine Morillon, and Michèle Taridec for their assistance in recruitment and data collection. We also thank Johana Desbordes for early discussions on this topic. These data were presented at the 59th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society.

References

Aartsen, M. J., Smits, C. H. M., van Tilburg, T., Knipscheer, K. C. P. M., and Deeg, D. J. H. (2002). Activity in older adults: cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 57, 153–162. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.2.p153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albert, M. S., Jones, K., Savage, C. R., Berkman, L., Seeman, T., Blazer, D., et al. (1995). Predictors of cognitive change in older persons: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Psychol. Aging 10, 578–589. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.10.4.578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armstrong, B., Gallant, S. N., Li, L., Patel, K., and Wong, B. I. (2017). Stereotype threat effects on older adults’ episodic and working memory: a meta-analysis. Gerontologist 57(Suppl. 2), S193–S205. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnx056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, S. J. (2017). An examination of age-based stereotype threat about cognitive decline: implications for stereotype-threat research and theory development. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 62–90. doi: 10.1177/1745691616656345

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, S. J., and Mather, M. (2013a). Stereotype threat can both enhance and impair older adults’ memory. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2522–2529. doi: 10.1177/0956797613497023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, S. J., and Mather, M. (2013b). Stereotype threat can reduce older adults’ memory errors. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 66, 1888–1895. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.840656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, S. J., and Mather, M. (2014). “Stereotype threat in older adults: when and why does it occur and who is most affected?” in The Oxford handbook of emotion, social cognition, and problem solving in adulthood. eds. P. Verhaeghen and C. Hertzog (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 302–319.

Google Scholar

Bertolucci, P. H., Brucki, S. M., Campacci, S. R., and Juliano, Y. (1994). The Mini-Mental State Examination in a general population: impact of educational status. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 52, 1–7.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bleecker, M. L., Bolla-Wilson, K., Kawas, C., and Agnew, J. (1988). Age-specific norms for the Mini-Mental State Exam. Neurology 38, 1565–1568. doi: 10.1212/wnl.38.10.1565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brainerd, C. J., and Reyna, V. F. (2002). Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 11, 164–169. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brubaker, M. S., and Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2018). The effects of stereotype threat on the associative memory deficit of older adults. Psychol. Aging 33, 17–29. doi: 10.1037/pag0000194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chasteen, A. L., Bhattacharyya, S., Horhota, M., Tam, R., and Hasher, L. (2005). How feelings of stereotype threat influence older adults’ memory performance. Exp. Aging Res. 31, 235–260. doi: 10.1080/03610730590948177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clouston, S. A., Smith, D. M., Mukherjee, S., Zhang, Y., Hou, W., Link, B. G., et al. (2020). Education and cognitive decline: an integrative analysis of global longitudinal studies of cognitive aging. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 75, e151–e160. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbz053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corson, Y., and Verrier, N. (2007). Emotions and false memories: valence or arousal? Psychol. Sci. 18, 208–211. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01874.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corson, Y., Verrier, N., and Bucic, A. (2009). False memories and individual variations: the role of field dependence-independence. Pers. Individ. Differ. 47, 8–11. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craik, F. I., and McDowd, J. M. (1987). Age differences in recall and recognition. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 13, 474–479. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.13.3.474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crum, R. M. (1993). Population-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational level. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 269, 2386–2391. doi: 10.1001/jama.1993.03500180078038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. J. Exp. Psychol. 58, 17–22. doi: 10.1037/h0046671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehon, H., and Brédart, S. (2004). False memories: young and older adults think of semantic associates at the same rate, but young adults are more successful at source monitoring. Psychol. Aging 19, 191–197. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derouesné, C., Poitreneau, J., Hugonot, L., Kalafat, M., Dubois, B., and Laurent, B. (1999). Le Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE): Un outil pratique pour l’évaluation de l’état cognitif des patients par le clinicien version française consensuelle. Presse Med. 28, 1141–1148.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Desrichard, O., and Köpetz, C. (2005). A threat in the elder: the impact of task-instructions, self-efficacy and performance expectations on memory performance in the elderly. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 35, 537–552. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaillard, M., Desmette, D., and Keller, J. (2011). Regulatory focus moderates the influence of age-related stereotypic expectancies on older adults’ test performance and threat-based concerns. Eur. Rev. Appl. Psychol. 61, 23–29. doi: 10.1016/j.erap.2010.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grühn, D., Gilet, A. -L., Studer, J., and Labouvie-Vief, G. (2011). Age-relevance of person characteristics: Persons’ beliefs about developmental change across the lifespan. Dev. Psychol. 47, 376–387. doi: 10.1037/a0021315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M., Auman, C., Colcombe, S. J., and Rahhal, T. A. (2003). The impact of stereotype threat on age differences in memory performance. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 58, P3–P11. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.1.P3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M., Hinson, J. T., and Hodges, E. A. (2009). Moderators of and mechanisms underlying stereotype threat effects on older adults’ memory performance. Exp. Aging Res. 35, 153–177. doi: 10.1080/03610730802716413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M., Hinson, J. T., and Statham, J. A. (2004). Explicit and implicit stereotype activation effects on memory: do age and awareness moderate the impact of priming? Psychol. Aging 19, 495–505. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.3.495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. Am. Psychol. 52, 1280–1300. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.52.12.1280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hummert, M. L. (2011). “Age stereotypes and aging” in Handbook of the psychology of aging. 7th Edn. eds. K. W. Schaie and S. L. Willis (Elsevier Academic Press), 249–262.

Google Scholar

Hummert, M. L., Garstka, T. A., Shaner, J. L., and Strahm, S. (1994). Stereotypes of the elderly held by young, middle-aged, and elderly adults. J. Gerontol. 49, P240–P249. doi: 10.1093/geronj/49.5.p240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunt, R. R., and Einstein, G. (1981). Relational and item-specific information in memory. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 20, 497–514. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90138-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

JASP Team (2020). JASP (Version 0.13.1) [Computer software].

Google Scholar

Johnson, P. O., and Fay, L. C. (1950). The Johnson–Neyman technique, its theory and application. Psychometrika 15, 349–367. doi: 10.1007/BF02288864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kite, M. E., Stockdale, G. D., Whitley, B. E. Jr., and Johnson, B. T. (2005). Attitudes toward younger and older adults: an updated meta-analytic review. J. Soc. Issues 61, 241–266. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00404.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamont, R. A., Swift, H. J., and Abrams, D. (2015). A review and meta-analysis of age-based stereotype threat: negative stereotypes, not facts, do the damage. Psychol. Aging 30, 180–193. doi: 10.1037/a0038586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, B. (2009). Stereotype embodiment: a psychosocial approach to aging. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 18, 332–336. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01662.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marquet, M., Missotten, P., and Adam, S. (2016). Âgisme et surestimation des difficultés cognitives des personnes âgées: une revue de la question [Ageism and overestimation of cognitive difficulties in older people: a review]. Geriatr. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 14, 177–186. doi: 10.1684/pnv.2016.0609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, F., Marioni, R., and Brayne, C. Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study (2012). Examining the influence of gender, education, social class and birth cohort on MMSE tracking over time: a population-based prospective cohort study. BMC Geriatr. 12:45. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-12-45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazerolle, M., Régner, I., Morisset, P., Rigalleau, F., and Huguet, P. (2012). Stereotype threat strengthens automatic recall and undermines controlled processes in older adults. Psychol. Sci. 23, 723–727. doi: 10.1177/0956797612437607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCabe, D. P., Presmanes, A. G., Robertson, C. L., and Smith, A. D. (2004). Item-specific processing reduces false memories. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 11, 1074–1079. doi: 10.3758/BF03196739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, H. -H. D., and Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. J. Appl. Psychol. 93, 1314–1334. doi: 10.1037/a0012702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyberg, L., and Pudas, S. (2019). Successful memory aging. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 70, 219–243. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, J. D., Nadel, L., Allen, J. J., Thomas, K. G., and Jacobs, W. J. (2002). The effects of experimentally induced stress on false recognition. Memory 10, 1–6. doi: 10.1080/09658210143000119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popham, L. E., and Hess, T. M. (2015). Age differences in the underlying mechanisms of stereotype threat effects. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 70, 223–234. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2019). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Available at: https://www.R-project.org/ (Accessed December 04, 2020).

Google Scholar

Rahhal, T. A., Hasher, L., and Colcombe, S. J. (2001). Instructional manipulations and age differences in memory: now you see them, now you don’t. Psychol. Aging 16, 697–706. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.4.697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, K. J., and Roediger, H. L. (1997). Associative processes in false recall and false recognition. Psychol. Sci. 8, 231–237. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00417.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roediger, H. L., and McDermott, K. (1995). Creating false memories: remembering words not presented in lists. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 21, 803–814. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmader, T., and Johns, M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 440–452. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seibt, B., and Förster, J. (2004). Stereotype threat and performance: how self-stereotypes influence processing by inducing regulatory foci. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 38–56. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.38

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, A. M., Gallo, D. A., Barber, S. J., Maddox, K. B., and Thomas, A. K. (2017). Stereotypes, warnings, and identity-related variables influence older adults’ susceptibility to associative false memory errors. Gerontologist 57(Suppl. 2), S206–S215. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnx057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R. E., and Hunt, R. R. (1998). Presentation modality affects false memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 5, 710–715. doi: 10.3758/BF03208850

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Bruchon-Schweitzer, M., and Paulhan, I. (1993). Inventaire d’Anxiété Etat-Trait Forme Y (STAI-Y). Paris: Les Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.

Google Scholar

Steele, C. M., and Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 797–811. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storbeck, J., and Clore, G. L. (2005). With sadness comes accuracy; with happiness, false memory. Mood and the false memory effect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 785–791. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01615.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storbeck, J., and Clore, G. L. (2011). Affect influences false memories at encoding: evidence from recognition data. Emotion 11, 981–989. doi: 10.1037/a0022754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. L., Miller, T. P., and Tinklenberg, J. R. (1992). Correlates of memory decline: a 4-year longitudinal study of older adults with memory complaints. Psychol. Aging 7, 185–193. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.7.2.185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, A. K., and Dubois, S. J. (2011). Reducing the burden of stereotype threat eliminates age differences in memory distortion. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1515–1517. doi: 10.1177/0956797611425932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tun, P. A., Wingfield, A., Rosen, M. J., and Blanchard, L. (1998). Response latencies for false memories: gist-based processing in normal aging. Psychol. Aging 13, 230–241. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.13.2.230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, J. T., and Gallo, D. A. (2016). Stereotype threat reduces false recognition when older adults are forewarned. Memory 24, 650–658. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2015.1036885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Medical Association (2013). World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 310, 2191–2194. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: memory, aging, DRM, false memories, stereotype threat

Citation: Gilet A-L, Evrard C, Galharret J-M and Colombel F (2021) The Moderating Role of Education on the Relationship Between Perceived Stereotype Threat and False Memory in Aging. Front. Psychol. 11:606249. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.606249

Received: 14 September 2020; Accepted: 11 December 2020;
Published: 12 January 2021.

Edited by:

Hannah J. Swift, University of Kent, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Sara N. Gallant, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, United States
Tara Patterson, University of California Los Angeles Life Sciences, United States

Copyright © 2021 Gilet, Evrard, Galharret and Colombel. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anne-Laure Gilet, anne-laure.gilet@univ-nantes.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.