Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 08 April 2021
Sec. Psychopathology

Functional Polymorphisms in Oxytocin and Dopamine Pathway Genes and the Development of Dispositional Compassion Over Time: The Young Finns Study

\nHenrik Dobewall,,
Henrik Dobewall1,2,3*Aino Saarinen,Aino Saarinen1,2Leo-Pekka LyytikinenLeo-Pekka Lyytikäinen3Liisa Keltikangas-JrvinenLiisa Keltikangas-Järvinen2Terho LehtimkiTerho Lehtimäki3Mirka HintsanenMirka Hintsanen1
  • 1Research Unit of Psychology, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland
  • 2Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
  • 3Fimlab Laboratories, and Finnish Cardiovascular Research Center - Tampere, Department of Clinical Chemistry, Faculty of Medicine and Health Technology, Tampere University, Tampere, Finland

Background: We define compassion as an enduring disposition that centers upon empathetic concern for another person's suffering and the motivation to act to alleviate it. The contribution of specific candidate genes to the development of dispositional compassion for others is currently unknown. We examine candidate genes in the oxytocin and dopamine signaling pathways.

Methods: In a 32-year follow-up of the Young Finns Study (N = 2,130, 44.0% men), we examined with multiple indicators latent growth curve modeling the molecular genetic underpinnings of dispositional compassion for others across the life span. We selected five single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) whose functions are known in humans: rs2268498 (OXTR), rs3796863 (CD38) (related to lower oxytocin levels), rs1800497 (ANKK1/DRD2), rs4680 (COMT), and rs1611115 (DBH) (related to higher dopamine levels). Compassion was measured with Cloninger's Temperament and Character Inventory on three repeated observations spanning 15 years (1997–2012). Differences between gender were tested.

Results: We did not find an effect of the five SNPs in oxytocin and dopamine pathway genes on the initial levels of dispositional compassion for others. Individuals who carry one or two copies of the T-allele of DBH rs1611115, however, tend to increase faster in compassion over time than those homozygotes for the C-allele, b = 0.063 (SE = 0.027; p = 0.018). This effect was largely driven by male participants, 0.206 (SE = 0.046; p < 0.001), and was not significant in female participants when analyzed separately.

Conclusions: Men who are known to have, on average, lower compassion than women seem to reduce this difference over time if they carry the T-allele of DBH rs1611115. The direction of the association indicates that dopamine signaling activity rather than overall dopamine levels might drive the development of compassion.

Introduction

Compassion can be defined as an enduring disposition that centers upon empathetic concern for another person's suffering and the motivation to act to alleviate it (Lazarus, 1991; Goetz et al., 2010). Experiencing compassion leads to more prosocial behavior, reductions in aggression, and improved intergroup relationships (Eisenberg et al., 2010). Compassion for others as a dispositional personality trait can be understood as the general reactivity of an individual to socio-emotional stimuli (Larsen and Ketelaar, 1991). Garcia et al. (2017) describe compassionate individuals as forgiving, charitable, and benevolent. Individuals characterized as high in compassion usually do not seek revenge and instead try to be constructive in a relationship (Garcia et al., 2017). In interpersonal conflict, compassionate individuals were found to reply with more compromising, integrating, and obliging behavior (Zhang et al., 2014).

From an evolutionary perspective, compassion has emerged from a caring motivation toward the offspring, the kin, and other in-group members, which has an advantage for survival, reproductive fitness, and health (Goetz et al., 2010; Gilbert, 2015, 2020). Compassion for others is consequently essential for forming and maintaining social relationships and building more harmonic societies. What distinguishes compassion from closely related phenotypes, such as empathy and especially empathic concern, is that it is based on the personal desire to help and not the sharing of others' emotions (Lazarus, 1991; Garcia et al., 2017). It is elicited by witnessing undeserved misery of others (Rudolph et al., 2004). Compassion aims at alleviating human suffering and the causes of this suffering, while the adjacent concept of kindness is linked to experiencing joyful emotions (Gilbert et al., 2019). Compassion is, thus, a motivational trait that precedents prosocial behavior (Steffen and Masters, 2005). An emotionally sensitive individual who notices suffering, however, is not inevitably motivated to take action (Poulin, 2017). Compassionate motives require certain competencies to be enacted (Gilbert, 2015). Individuals who understand, for instance, that they share common humanity and hardship with others, and those who are better at tolerating the distress experienced when responding to another person's suffering may also behave more compassionately (Oveis et al., 2010; Klimecki et al., 2014; Lebowitz and Dovidio, 2015; Strauss et al., 2016; Gilbert, 2020). Apart from trait-like compassion that is relatively stable across contexts and time compassion may also refer to a state-like episode or emotion that can change from situation to situation (Goetz et al., 2010). The different aspects of being compassionate are thus context dependent so that an individual may be good at some of them but not necessarily others (Gilbert, 2020). In addition, there is not one way to assess compassion that covers all aspects of it. The current study focuses on dispositional compassion.

The Genetic Base of Compassion and Closely Related Phenotypes

There is a broad consensus that compassion and compassion-related phenotypes have a significant genetic component (Gillespie et al., 2003; Ando et al., 2004; Knafo and Israel, 2010). A large twin study from Japan, for instance, found that dispositional compassion for others was 34% heritable (Ando et al., 2004). Despite the moderate heritability of dispositional compassion, the contribution of specific candidate genes in its development remains understudied. Only a single small-sample study related candidate genes to dispositional compassion for others (Pełka-Wysiecka et al., 2012), while we can draw on relevant knowledge from studies on empathy and related phenotypes (Gong et al., 2017). Moreover, trait-like compassion might have a different genetic background from compassionate states or the emotion compassion. Focusing on known functional polymorphisms in oxytocin and dopamine signaling pathway genes could offer a potential starting point to shed light on the molecular genetic architecture of dispositional compassion. A polymorphism is referred to as functional if it has been demonstrated that it alters the function of a gene or set of genes (Albert, 2011). The physiological infrastructure that supported the evolution of compassion is not well-understood in humans (Gilbert, 2020); however, it has been suggested that individual differences in compassion are neurologically related to processes of the oxytocinergic and vasopressinergic systems (Carter et al., 2017; Ebert et al., 2018) and dopaminergic reward mechanisms (Klimecki et al., 2013). It is also well-known that the neuropeptide oxytocin has played a vital role in the evolution of compassion's underlying caring motivation (Gilbert, 2020).

The neuropeptides oxytocin and dopamine are known to regulate socio-emotional behavior in general (Ebstein et al., 2012) and compassionate states in particular (Keum and Shin, 2019), and play a putative role in modulating social interaction (Ben-Israel et al., 2015; Pearce et al., 2017). Neuroimaging studies have further found that compassion for other's pain and suffering are related to more activity in oxytocinergic and dopaminergic brain regions (Immordino-Yang et al., 2009; Kim et al., 2009). A recent genome-wide study suggests that allelic variation in the oxytocin and dopamine signaling pathway might indeed be associated with distinct personality profiles that include low vs. high compassion (Zwir et al., 2018).

Functional Polymorphisms in Oxytocin Pathway Genes

The neurochemical processes in which oxytocin is involved are modulated by a single type of receptor. There are two oxytocin polymorphisms with known functionality. Allelic variation in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) variant rs2268498 was found to have relevance for presumed differences in endogenous oxytocin activity. C-allele carriers showed a 2-fold higher messenger RNA expression rate when compared with individuals with TT genotype (Reuter et al., 2017). Furthermore, the single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) OXTR rs35062132 was found in functional analyses to cause changes in cellular responses (Ma et al., 2013). Allelic variation in rs2268498 was additionally found to modulate the connectivity of oxytocinergic brain regions (Zimmermann et al., 2018). Melchers et al. (2017) suggested that T-allele carriers, who had better social information processing and memory capacity than individuals with CC genotype, might more easily think and act empathically, and indeed, carrying the TT genotype was associated with decreased threat avoidance (O'Connell et al., 2012) and higher empathic concern, which in turn predicted more prosocial behavior (Christ et al., 2016).

From animal models, it has been known for some time that CD38 (nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide+-glycohydrolase gene) plays an important role in the oxytocin signaling pathway by regulating the secretion of the neuropeptide, which directly influences the plasma oxytocin level (Jin et al., 2007). Feldman et al. (2012) were able to show that individuals with the CC genotype of CD38 rs379863 have lower plasma oxytocin levels compared with those carrying one or two copies of the A-allele. A-allele carriers, compared with homozygotes of the C-allele, also reported stronger empathic responses leading to more prosocial behavior (Liu et al., 2017).

The soothing and calming qualities of compassion (Pace et al., 2009; Gilbert, 2020), such as not experiencing distress when helping a suffering person, were suggested to be oxytocin mediated (Carter et al., 2017). Oxytocin might thus also be involved in experiencing compassion because of its known role in regulating physiological stress responses (Eisenberg, 2000; Lebowitz and Dovidio, 2015). Of interest here are the parasympathetic autonomic nervous system and its role in conserving energy and calming the body (e.g., decreasing heart rate) and mind. Heightened activation of the vagus nerve has been found to positively correlate with trait-like compassion (Eisenberg et al., 1996) and compassionate states (Stellar et al., 2015). It is therefore not surprising that practicing compassion has been found to decrease an individual's negative affect when dealing with another person's distress (Klimecki et al., 2014) and might improve coping with a stressful situation (Pace et al., 2009; Abelson et al., 2014).

Functional Polymorphisms in Dopamine Pathway Genes

In the dopamine pathway, three candidate genes with known functional variants have been suggested for compassion-related phenotypes.

In humans, there are five dopamine receptors. The ANKK1 (ankyrin repeat and kinase domain containing 1) polymorphism rs1800497 is located downstream of the dopamine D2 receptor gene (DRD2). Carrying T-allele of this SNP, when compared with homozygotes for the C-allele, results in reduced dopamine receptor-binding density and availability (Pohjalainen et al., 1998) and a reduced relative glucose metabolic rate in dopaminergic brain regions (Noble et al., 1997). The T-allele was found to be associated with an individual's greater need for intimacy and emotional closeness (Gillath et al., 2008), to make the brain's reward centers less reactive (Stice et al., 2008), as well as to modify the effects of stressful life events (Elovainio et al., 2007). ANKK1/DRD2 rs1800497 appears to be related to self-reported empathy (Pearce et al., 2017), yet, the strength of the association is dependent on the sample studied (Jern et al., 2017; Pearce et al., 2018).

Second, allelic variation in the COMT (catechol-O-methyltransferase) gene might play a role in dopamine signaling because it codes a key enzyme for regulating cortical dopamine release (Tunbridge et al., 2004). The substitution polymorphism of COMT rs4680 has received a lot of attention as the A-allele codes for a methionine instead of a valine amino acid coded for by the G-allele. The A-allele (Met) carriers were shown to have lower COMT enzymatic activity, resulting in higher dopamine levels than individuals with one or two copies of the G-allele (Val) (Stein et al., 2006). Individuals with GG genotype also showed higher empathic concern for an unfortunate other (Ru et al., 2017), higher prosocial behavior (Reuter et al., 2011), and higher scores on a personality dimension, which includes the compassion subscale used in the current study (Baeken et al., 2014). Val carrier “warriors” were further better in the processing of unpleasant stimuli but worse in memory and attention tasks than Met carriers “worriers” (Stein et al., 2006), suggesting that the A-allele might be associated with higher compassion. Two later studies (Calati et al., 2011; Pełka-Wysiecka et al., 2012), however, could not replicate the association of COMT rs4680 with cooperativeness.

A third candidate with known functionality is the DBH (dopamine beta-hydroxylase) gene, which codes the enzyme that converts dopamine to norepinephrine. The T-allele of the DBH gene polymorphism rs1611115 is associated with lower plasma DBH activity (Zabetian et al., 2001; Mustapic et al., 2014). Low HBH activity results in lower norepinephrine availability negatively related to several aspects of social behavior, which in turn associates with higher dopamine levels (Marino et al., 2005). The CC genotype of rs1611115 has been related to greater empathic ability (Gong et al., 2014), while the TT genotype is associated with personality traits related to impulsiveness and aggression (Hess et al., 2009).

A functional variable number tandem repeats (VNTR) in the dopamine receptor D4 gene was found to be associated with social cognition (Leerkes et al., 2017) and diverse compassion-related phenotypes (Bachner-Melman et al., 2005; Gong et al., 2014; Uzefovsky et al., 2014). Finally, another VNTR in the dopamine transporter (SLC6A3) gene has been further found to be associated with dispositional compassion for others and cooperativeness (Pełka-Wysiecka et al., 2012). Yet, we concentrated the current study on SNPs and excluded other forms of genetic variation.

The Current Study

The current study examines whether SNPs of known functionality in OXTR, CD38, ANKK1/DRD2, COMT, and DBH are associated with the initial levels of dispositional compassion for others. For the reason that the heritability of closely related phenotypes tends to increase with age (Knafo and Plomin, 2006), comprehensive studies of genetic effects on compassion need to be conducted with longitudinal research designs. Therefore, we also examine the associations of the SNPs with the change in compassion over time. We expect that polymorphisms previously linked to lower oxytocin and higher dopamine levels are associated with lower initial levels of dispositional compassion and a slower increase in compassion over time. These neuropeptides, yet, might work slightly differently in males and females (Benenson, 2014; Carter et al., 2017). Previous studies have found gender-sensitive associations for genetic variation in the oxytocin (Christ et al., 2016; Shang et al., 2017) and dopamine (Pełka-Wysiecka et al., 2012; Uzefovsky et al., 2014) signaling pathways. This suggests that potential differences between male and female participants should be considered when studying molecular genetic associations of compassion. The current study will, therefore, contribute to the ongoing discussion on the etiology and consequences of gender differences in personality traits (Costa et al., 2001; Miettunen et al., 2007; Schmitt et al., 2008) by investigating the underlying neurochemical processes. A well-powered prospective study design is used based on three repeated observations, measured 15 years apart in a representative sample, and assessing compassion with a reliable and well-known inventory.

Methods

Procedure and Participants

Participants were drawn from the ongoing Young Finns Study (YFS; Raitakari et al., 2008). The YFS is a population-based, prospective study that, since 1980, follows individuals from six different birth cohorts (3, 6, 9, 12, 15, and 18 years old at the baseline). We used data of the 1980 (T0), 1997 (T1), 2001 (T2), and 2012 (T3) waves. Dispositional compassion for others was measured for the first time when the youngest participants were 20 years old (T1), for a second time 4 years later (T2), and for a third time after another 11 years when the oldest participants were 50 years old (T3).

The original sample consisted of 3,596 individuals, and 2,443 participants were successfully genotyped. In the current study, we excluded those who did not respond to the dependent variable at least once (n = 313). The inclusion criteria were met by 2,130 participants (59.2% of the original sample).

The YFS was approved by all participating universities' ethics committees at the beginning of the study in 1980, and the follow-ups were approved by the ethics committee of the University of Turku [vernacular institution name: Varsinais-Suomen sairaanhoitopiirin kuntayhtymä, Eettinen toimikunta, Meeting Number 9/2010; study name, “Lasten sepelvaltimotaudin riskitekijät projekti (Laseri) 30-vuotis seurantatutkimus, 25.8.2010”]. The YFS was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki declaration. Written informed consent was obtained from the participants or their parents if the participant was underage.

Measures

Dispositional Compassion

Dispositional compassion for others was measured with Cloninger's Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) (Cloninger et al., 1993). Compassion (vs. revengefulness) is a sub-scale of the character trait cooperativeness (Garcia et al., 2017). The scale consists of 10 items [e.g., “It gives me pleasure to see my enemies suffer” (reverse scored); “I hate to see anyone suffer” (positively scored); “It gives me pleasure to help others, even if they have treated me badly” (positively scored); and “I like to imagine my enemies suffering” (reverse scored)]. The questions were answered on a five-point Likert scale. Higher compassion, as measured with the TCI, has been found to be associated with higher social warmth, sociability, and positive emotions (García et al., 2012), better well-being and mental health (Saarinen et al., 2019a,b), and regular healthy behaviors (Gluschkoff et al., 2019). Lower compassion, on the contrary, has been found to be correlated with anger, narcissism, hostility, verbal and physical aggression, while the scales has also discriminant validity (De Fruyt et al., 2006; García et al., 2012). The TCI has demonstrated a stable factorial structure and high internal consistency in the general population as well as in clinical samples (Goncalves and Cloninger, 2010; Vitoratou et al., 2015). The compassion scale has high internal consistency (Cronbach's αT1−T3 ≥ 0.86) and a high test–retest reliability (rT1−>T2 = 0.60, rT1−>T3 = 0.69, both p < 0.001). Confirmatory factor analyses confirmed across measurement occasions a good fit of the data to a model that accounts for the correlated error structure between reversely scored items (CFI values ≥ 0.96 and RMSEA values ≤ 0.08). Structural analyses of the TCI compassion (vs. revengefulness) scale can be found in Supplementary S2. The balanced wording of the items further did not have an effect on the below-reported results (for separate analyses for the positively worded compassion and the reversed scored revengefulness items, see Supplementary S3). Descriptive statistics and the missing value pattern of the compassion scale are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the study variables.

Genotyping and Imputation

The GWAS for the participants of the YFS was performed in 2009 (Smith et al., 2010). Genomic DNA was extracted from peripheral blood leukocytes using a commercially available kit and Qiagen BioRobot M48 Workstation according to the manufacturer's instructions (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany). Genotyping was done for 2,556 samples using custom-built Illumina Human 670 k BeadChip at Welcome Trust Sanger Institute. Genotypes were called using Illuminus clustering algorithm. Fifty-six samples failed Sanger genotyping pipeline QC criteria (i.e., duplicated samples, heterozygosity, low call rate, or Sequenom fingerprint discrepancy). From the remaining 2,500 samples, one sample failed gender check, three were removed due to low genotyping call rate (<0.95), and 54 samples for possible relatedness (pi-hat > 0.2). SNPs (11,766) were excluded based on the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium test (p ≤ 1e−06), 7,746 SNPs failed the missingness test (call rate <0.95), and 34,596 SNPs failed the frequency test (minor allele frequency <0.01). After quality control, 2,443 samples and 546,677 genotyped SNPs remained. Genotype imputation was performed using SHAPEIT v1 for haplotyping, and SNPTEST v2.2.2 and 1000 Genomes Phase I integrated variant set release v3 reference panel for imputation. The imputation quality of 39,346,532 imputed SNPs was good (i.e., squared correlation between imputed and true genotypes ≥ 0.40). We selected SNPs in the oxytocin and dopamine signaling pathways that are known to be functional in humans. ANKK1/DRD2 rs1800497 (T → C) (Noble et al., 1997; Pohjalainen et al., 1998) and COMT rs4680 (G → A) (Stein et al., 2006) were directly genotyped. OXTR rs2268498 (C → T) (Reuter et al., 2017; Zimmermann et al., 2018), CD38 rs3796863 (C → A) (Jin et al., 2007; Feldman et al., 2012), and DBH rs1611115 (T → C) (Zabetian et al., 2001; Mustapic et al., 2014) were imputed. Chromosome, position, and minor allele frequency are indicated in Table 2. Because complex traits like compassion are shaped by many genes of small effect (Manolio et al., 2009), we summed up the assumed risk alleles to form genetic profiles for putatively high dopamine and low oxytocin levels (see Belsky and Israel, 2014).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNP) in the oxytocin and dopamine signaling pathways known to be functional in humans.

Covariates

The top 10 principal components obtained in a genome-wide association study (GWAS) were included as covariates to account for population stratification (Patterson et al., 2006; Border et al., 2019).

Childhood socioeconomic status (SESC) was assessed when participants were from 3 to 18 years old (1980). SESC was formed based on two indicators: the average of mother's and father's years in education and the annual household income. Adulthood socioeconomic status of the participants (SESA) was assessed with self-reported education and income in 2001 and 2012 (preference was given to the first assessment, if available). Both SES indicators were coded with one point per presence of each of the following: High educational level (tertiary education vs. secondary education or lower) and high income (highest 25% vs. lower). The indicators have a minimum score of 0 (low SES) and a maximum of 2 (high SES).

Other covariates included participant age at baseline (years of birth: 1962, 1965, 1968, 1971, 1974, and 1977) and gender (44.0% men).

Analyses

Whether selective attrition influenced the results was examined by comparing the included participants with those of the initial sample that were excluded with reference to the study variables by means of chi-squared independence tests and independent sample t-tests. Genetic associations were examined utilizing multiple indicators latent growth curve modeling (LGCM) (Bishop et al., 2015; Isiordia and Ferrer, 2018). LGCM allows examining antecedents of individual differences in the initial levels of dispositional compassion for others and the rate of change over time. The combination of repeated measurement occasions and a high number of indicators per occasion maximizes the statistical power of our models (Hertzog et al., 2008; Oertzen et al., 2010). Using LGCM also follows current best practice by accounting for potential measurement error in the phenotype (Border et al., 2019). Furthermore, analyses were conducted with full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimator to handle missing data (Allison, 2012). FIML estimation does not impute missing values, but it uses all information available, namely, all participants that responded to the compassion scale at least once.

First, we ran a developmental model controlling only for the covariates (Model 1). Second, we entered each of the five functional SNPs separately (Model 2, a–e). Next, we estimated the accumulative effects of genetic profiles for high dopamine (Model 2, f) and low oxytocin (Model 2, g) levels. Forth, we tested for gender-sensitive effects to identify potential differences between men and women in the contribution of allelic variation in the oxytocin and dopamine signaling pathways (Model 3, a–g).

Finally, we conducted several robustness checks. To test the independence of associations, we entered all five functional SNPs jointly (Pearce et al., 2017), which did not alter the main interpretation of the results. The findings were further robust in regard to alternative genetic models (additivity vs. dominance and heterozygotic effects) (Dick et al., 2015). Because some authors argue that the pattern of associations is more important than individual signals for a candidate SNP (e.g., Pearce et al., 2017), we also compared empirically the results for the five functional variants with the performance of additional 10 commonly studied SNPs (see Supplementary).

We used a liberal significance threshold by dividing the p-value for nominal significant associations by the number of tested candidate genes, 0.05/5 = 0.01, to account for multiple testing (see Border et al., 2019).

Results

Attrition analysis indicated that included participants were more likely to be women compared with those participants of the initial sample who were excluded from the current study (56.0 vs. 43.0%; p < 0.001). There was no attrition bias in any of the other study variables.

Across models (Table 3), the global fit indices suggested that a linear trajectory fits the data well. RMSEAs of ≤ 0.033 suggested a good fit and CFIs of ≥ 0.913 indicated an acceptable fit.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Association between oxytocin and dopamine pathway genes and the initial levels of dispositional compassion for others and change over time.

The developmental model for dispositional compassion for others indicated that initial levels, b = 3.740 (SE = 0.076; p > 0.001), increased over time, b = 0.302 (SE = 0.049; p > 0.001) (Model 1). Year of birth 1963, b = 0.255 (SE =; p = 0.003), and year of birth 1965, b = 0. 178 (SE = 0082; p = 0.031), were positively associated with a higher initial level of compassion for others. Men, b = −0.215 (SE = 0.050; p < 0.001), and participants with lower adulthood SES, b = −0.171 (SE = 0.067; p = 0.011), had lower initial levels of compassion. There was also a nominally significant association between the 10th principal component, b = 1.475 (SE = 0.689; p = 0.032), and change of dispositional compassion for others over time.

In Model 2, none of the five SNPs with known functionality in the oxytocin and dopamine pathway genes was associated with the initial levels of dispositional compassion for others. However, there was a nominally significant positive association between DBH polymorphism rs1611115 and the change over time of compassion (Model 2e). Individuals who carry one or two copies of the T-allele have increased faster in compassion over time than those homozygotes for the C-allele, b = 0.063 (SE = 0.027; p = 0.018). This association, however, did not survive Bonferroni correction for the number of tested candidate genes.

Furthermore, there was a significant interaction between gender and DBH rs1611115 (p < 0.001). Separate analyses for male and female participants (Model 3) indicated that the effect of the T-allele on the development of compassion over time was largely driven by men, 0.206 (SE = 0.046; p < 0.001), and not significant in women (p = 0.647). This male-specific effect remained significant taking into account multiple testing. Figure 1 illustrates this robust gender-sensitive effect of variation in DBH rs1611115. Male carrier of the T-allele (n = 292; combined due to the small number of T-allele homozygotes, n = 29) showed a similar development of compassion over time compared with women, while CC homozygotes (n = 646), on average, reported at all three observations lower dispositional compassion for others.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Development of dispositional compassion for others over time. Comparing female participants (n = 1,192; gray line) with male participants split by DBH rs1611115 genotype. Continuous black line are CC homozygotes (n = 646), and dotted black line are carriers of the T-allele (n = 292).

Finally, neither of the two genetic profiles nor the additional 10 commonly studied SNPs (see Supplementary Table 2) were associated with dispositional compassion.

Discussion

We examined the association between the development of dispositional compassion for others over the life span and five SNPs previously linked to lower oxytocin (OXTR rs2268498 and CD38 rs3796863) and higher dopamine (ANKK1/DRD2 rs1800497, COMT rs4680, and DBH rs1611115) levels (Marino et al., 2005; Stein et al., 2006; Jin et al., 2007; Feldman et al., 2012). There was no statistically significant association between any of the SNPs and the initial level of compassion. In the dopamine signaling pathway, however, there was a gender-sensitive effect with male participants who carry one or two copies of the T-allele of rs1611115 increasing faster in compassion when growing older compared with those homozygotes for the C-allele. Allelic variation in rs1611115 and changes in compassion over time were not related in female participants when analyzed separately, even though approaching Bonferroni corrected significance levels when men and women were considered together. We further confirmed that men, on average, have lower initial levels of dispositional compassion for others than women (see Hintsanen et al., 2019). Taken together, these findings imply that men may leap up from this lower starting point if they carry the T-allele of DBH rs1611115.

Also, two previous studies found gender-sensitive associations for dopamine pathway genes (Pełka-Wysiecka et al., 2012; Uzefovsky et al., 2014). We speculate that the found male-specific effects could be a joint product of genes and environments. That is, males with specific alleles are more likely to end up in different environments and to develop a higher level of compassion due to the environmental circumstances (see Nettle, 2006; Abramson et al., 2020). This might happen due to well-documented gender differences in personality traits, such as assertiveness or reward dependence (Costa et al., 2001; Miettunen et al., 2007), which are found to be larger in developed and egalitarian countries in which women have more equal opportunities such as Finland (Schmitt et al., 2008). Certainly, there also is a tendency in humans to become more compassionate as we grow older because of aging-related role changes, such as fewer demands of competing for social place, and life events, such as the loss of a beloved person (Cuddy et al., 2005; Hintsanen et al., 2019). The found association between the genetic variation in the dopamine pathway and change in compassion over time is in line with studies that suggest that dopamine signaling is neurologically linked to applying “warrior” over “worrier” strategies (Stein et al., 2006). That this effect was gendered and stronger in male participants implies that functions of the studied neuropeptides are critical for gender differences in compassion development (e.g., Carter et al., 2017).

That we did not find any relationships of dispositional compassion with neither four out of five functional SNPs nor the two genetic profiles conflicts with previous work on compassion-related traits and states (Rodrigues et al., 2009; Gong et al., 2014; Christ et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2017; Pearce et al., 2017; Ru et al., 2017). At the same time, the reviewed empirical literature is heterogeneous, including more null findings for included SNPs than the highlighted significant associations (Comings et al., 2001; Pełka-Wysiecka et al., 2012; Liu et al., 2017; Pearce et al., 2017) and associations pointing into the opposite direction (Laursen et al., 2014). This pairs with general criticism of the candidate gene approach because associations with individual SNPs often cannot be replicated (Hewitt, 2012; Border et al., 2019). The complexity of prosocial behavior also requires neurological mechanisms to protect oneself and close others from potentially threatening individuals or groups (Carter et al., 2017). The role of oxytocin has been implicated in the punishment of those who caused the suffering of a close other and increased ethnocentrism (De Dreu et al., 2011; Pfattheicher et al., 2019). This context dependence might be another reason why we did not find significant associations between compassion and oxytocin pathway genes. Also available GWAS in phenotypes loosely related to compassion did not find associations for any of the suggested candidate genes (Warrier et al., 2018a,b). Another group of researchers has argued that associations for candidate genes are dependent on the social domain under study (Li et al., 2015; Gong et al., 2017; Pearce et al., 2017). In the latter view, the current study presents a novel association. No previous study with comparable statistical power (see Pełka-Wysiecka et al., 2012) has examined the contribution of functional variants in oxytocin and dopamine pathway genes on dispositional compassion for others.

The reversed directional association—contrary to previous work (Hess et al., 2009; Gong et al., 2014)—indicates that higher, and not lower, dopamine levels relate to higher compassion. There are several explanations for this unexpected finding. Oxytocin and dopamine are known to interact (Baskerville and Douglas, 2010; Feldman et al., 2012). Oxytocin release notably increases collaboration between people and prosocial behavior, which in turn might activate the dopaminergic reward centers of the brain (Skuse and Gallagher, 2009). In line with this reasoning, Inagaki et al. (2016) have found that giving social support rather than receiving it is associated with higher well-being, reduced stress-related brain activity, and greater reward-related activity. There further exists a link between compassionate states and dopaminergic reward signaling (Kim et al., 2009). Being compassionate has also been found to associate with better health, psychological well-being, and social functioning (Post, 2005; Steffen and Masters, 2005; Saarinen et al., 2019b). Dispositional compassion for others further predicts higher positive affect, lower negative affect, and more perceived social support assessed 4 and 10 years later (Saarinen et al., 2019b).

Moreover, these putatively higher dopamine levels are a consequence of reduced plasma DBH enzyme activity when converting dopamine to norepinephrine (Zabetian et al., 2001; Marino et al., 2005; Mustapic et al., 2014). Thus, allelic variation based on dopamine signaling activity rather than overall levels may be important for compassion development (Stein et al., 2006; Nikolova et al., 2011). The found association between dopamine signaling activity and compassion is interesting on its own right by illuminating a specific genetic pathway that might explain individual differences in compassion for others. It is also important for informing subsequent gene–environment interaction studies and meta-analyses.

That we did not find significant associations for any of the additionally tested 10 SNPs was somewhat surprising because phenotypes closely related to compassion for others such as empathic concern (Huetter et al., 2016), autonomic arousal while witnessing others suffering (Smith et al., 2014), and physiological and self-reported stress reactivity (Rodrigues et al., 2009) were previously associated with these SNPs. These null findings indicate that there may be differences in the genetic background of dispositional vs. state-like compassion.

Limitations and Strengths

The current study has limitations that need to be considered when interpreting the results. That selective attrition in terms of gender influenced the results limits the generalizability of our findings, especially, because the positive effect of carrying the T-allele of DBH rs1611115 on dispositional compassion for others was stronger in men, while women were somewhat overrepresented. The study was conducted in a Caucasian population, and the findings might not apply in other cultures even though compassion is stated to be a universal disposition (Cloninger et al., 1993; Goetz et al., 2010).

For the OXTR, the second functional SNP rs35062132 (Ma et al., 2013) is, unfortunately, monomorphic in Finnish samples and could not be included in our study. Finally, we cannot rule out the possibility that there might be other candidate genes and genetic variation, such as VNTRs, that contribute to individual differences in compassion (see Pełka-Wysiecka et al., 2012).

Our data only allows to study the development of compassion in individuals between age 20 and 50. It would be interesting to know what happens when the participants grow even older. We are also unable to say if there were differences in the level of compassion due to allelic variation when the participants were younger than in our assessments.

There is a lack of consensus on the definition and measurement of compassion (Goetz et al., 2010; Strauss et al., 2016; Pommier et al., 2019). Using a reliable and validated personality inventory enabled us, contrary to most previous studies, to investigate molecular genetic underpinnings of dispositional compassion for others (Cloninger et al., 1993; Garcia et al., 2017). The focus of this study was on the stable motivational component of compassion, while we did not measure the emotional component or compassionate actions.

Candidate gene approaches have been strongly criticized recently, and current results need to be interpreted with caution pending replication (Hewitt, 2012). Very few, if any, genetically informed cohorts include Cloninger's TCI and have a similar long follow-up, which could be used in replication efforts. We followed the recommendations of Dick et al. (2015) and Border et al. (2019) for reducing the chance of reporting false-positive associations and conducted an extensive set of robustness checks. We achieved sufficient statistical power to detect even small effects by using a longitudinal research design with multiple indicators for measuring compassion for others (Hertzog et al., 2008; Oertzen et al., 2010).

Conclusions

Genetic variation, previously linked to lower oxytocin and higher dopamine levels, was not related to lower dispositional compassion and a slower increase in compassion over time when male and female participants were analyzed jointly. However, males who were shown to have lower average levels of compassion than women seem to reduce this difference to some degree over time if they carry the T-allele of DBH polymorphism rs1611115 and not the CC genotype. The male-specific finding suggests that gender differences should be paid more attention to in future research. The direction of the association further indicates that dopamine signaling activity rather than overall dopamine levels might drive differences in dispositional compassion.

Data Availability Statement

The dataset supporting the conclusions of this article was obtained from the Cardiovascular Risk In Young Finns Study (YFS) which comprises health-related participant data. The use of data is restricted under the regulations on professional secrecy (Act on the Openness of Government Activities, 612/1999) and on sensitive personal data (Personal Data Act, 523/1999, implementing the EU data protection directive 95/46/EC). Due to these restrictions, the data cannot be stored in public repositories or otherwise made publicly available. Data access may be permitted on a case-by-case basis upon request only. Data sharing outside the group is done in collaboration with the YFS group and requires a data-sharing agreement. Investigators can submit an expression of interest to the chairman of the publication committee (Prof. Mika Kähönen, Tampere University, Finland).

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by all participating universities' ethics committees at the beginning of the study in 1980, and the followups were approved by the ethics committee of the University of Turku (vernacular institution name: Varsinais-Suomen sairaanhoitopiirin kuntayhtymä, Eettinen toimikunta, Meeting Number 9/2010; study name, Lasten sepelvaltimotaudin riskitekijät projekti (Laseri) 30-vuotis seurantatutkimus, 25.8.2010). The YFS was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki declaration. Written informed consent was obtained from the participants. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

All the authors have made a substantial contribution to designing or carrying out the research, writing or revising the manuscript, or providing guidance on the execution of the research. All analyses have been conducted by HD. The paper has been read and approved by all the authors.

Funding

This study was supported financially by the Academy of Finland (MH, grant number 308676). The Young Finns Study has been financially supported by the Academy of Finland: grants 286284, 134309 (Eye), 126925, 121584, 124282, 129378 (Salve), 117787 (Gendi), 41071 (Skidi), and 322098; the Social Insurance Institution of Finland; Competitive State Research Financing of the Expert Responsibility area of Kuopio, Tampere and Turku University Hospitals (grant X51001); Juho Vainio Foundation; Paavo Nurmi Foundation; Finnish Foundation for Cardiovascular Research; Finnish Cultural Foundation; The Sigrid Juselius Foundation; Tampere Tuberculosis Foundation; Emil Aaltonen Foundation; Yrjö Jahnsson Foundation; Signe and Ane Gyllenberg Foundation (TL); Diabetes Research Foundation of Finnish Diabetes Association; and EU Horizon 2020 (grant 755320 for TAXINOMISIS and grant 848146 for AITION); and European Research Council (grant 742927 for MULTIEPIGEN project); Tampere University Hospital Supporting Foundation.

Conflict of Interest

L-PL and TL are employed by the company Fimlab Laboratories Oy.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.576346/full#supplementary-material

References

Abelson, J. L., Erickson, T. M., Mayer, S. E., Crocker, J., Briggs, H., Lopez-Duran, N. L., et al. (2014). Brief cognitive intervention can modulate neuroendocrine stress responses to the Trier Social Stress Test: buffering effects of a compassionate goal orientation. Psychoneuroendocrinology 44, 60–70. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2014.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abramson, L., Uzefovsky, F., Toccaceli, V., and Knafo-Noam, A. (2020). The genetic and environmental origins of emotional and cognitive empathy: review and meta-analyses of twin studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 114, 113–133. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2020.03.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albert, P. R. (2011). What is a functional genetic polymorphism? Defining classes of functionality. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 36, 363–365. doi: 10.1503/jpn.110137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allison, P. (2012). Handling Missing Data by Maximum Likelihood. Orlando, FL: Keynote Presentation at the SAS Global Forum.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ando, J., Suzuki, A., Yamagata, S., Kijima, N., Maekawa, H., Ono, Y., et al. (2004). Genetic and environmental structure of Cloninger's temperament and character dimensions. J. Pers. Disord. 18, 379–393. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2004.18.4.379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bachner-Melman, R., Gritsenko, I., Nemanov, L., Zohar, A. H., Dina, C., and Ebstein, R. P. (2005). Dopaminergic polymorphisms associated with self-report measures of human altruism: a fresh phenotype for the dopamine D4 receptor. Mol. Psychiatry 10, 333–335. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baeken, C., Claes, S., and De Raedt, R. (2014). The influence of COMT Val 158 Met genotype on the character dimension cooperativeness in healthy females. Brain Behav. 4, 515–520. doi: 10.1002/brb3.233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baskerville, T. A., and Douglas, A. J. (2010). Dopamine and oxytocin interactions underlying behaviors: potential contributions to behavioral disorders. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 16, e92–e123. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2010.00154.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, D. W., and Israel, S. (2014). Integrating genetics and social science: genetic risk scores. Biodemogr. Soc. Biol. 60, 137–155. doi: 10.1080/19485565.2014.946591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benenson, J. F. (2014). Warriors and Worriers: The Survival of the Sexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Ben-Israel, S., Uzefovsky, F., Ebstein, R. P., and Knafo-Noam, A. (2015). Dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism and sex interact to predict children's affective knowledge. Front. Psychol. 6:846. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, J., Geiser, C., and Cole, D. A. (2015). Modeling latent growth with multiple indicators: a comparison of three approaches. Psychol. Methods 20, 43–62. doi: 10.1037/met0000018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Border, R., Johnson, E. C., Evans, L. M., Smolen, A., Berley, N., Sullivan, P. F., et al. (2019). No support for historical candidate gene or candidate gene-by-interaction hypotheses for major depression across multiple large samples. Am. J. Psychiatry 176, 376–387. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.18070881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calati, R., Porcelli, S., Giegling, I., Hartmann, A. M., Möller, H.-J., De Ronchi, D., et al. (2011). Catechol-o-methyltransferase gene modulation on suicidal behavior and personality traits: review, meta-analysis and association study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 45, 309–321. doi: 10.1016/J.JPSYCHIRES.2010.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, S., Bartal, I. B., and Porges, E. (2017). “The roots of compassion: an evolutionary and neurobiological perspective,” in The Oxford Handbook of Compassion Science, eds E. M. Seppälä, E. Simon-Thomas, S. L. Brown, and M. C. Worline (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 178–188.

Google Scholar

Christ, C. C., Carlo, G., and Stoltenberg, S. F. (2016). Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) single nucleotide polymorphisms indirectly predict prosocial behavior through perspective taking and empathic concern. J. Pers. 84, 204–213. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., and Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 50, 975–990. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240059008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Comings, D., Gade-Andavolu, R., Gonzalez, N., Wu, S., Muhleman, D., Blake, H., et al. (2001). A multivariate analysis of 59 candidate genes in personality traits: the temperament and character inventory. Clin. Genet. 58, 375–385. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-0004.2000.580508.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, P. T., Terracciano, A., and McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: robust and surprising findings. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81, 322–331. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuddy, A. J. C., Norton, M. I., and Fiske, S. T. (2005). This old stereotype: the pervasiveness and persistence of the elderly stereotype. J. Soc. Issues 61, 267–285. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00405.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Dreu, C. K. W., Greer, L. L., Van Kleef, G. A., Shalvi, S., and Handgraaf, M. J. J. (2011). Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 1262–1266. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015316108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Fruyt, F., De Clercq, B. J., Van Wiele, L., and Van Heeringen, K. (2006). The validity of cloninger's psychobiological model versus the five-factor model to predict DSM-IV personality disorders in a heterogeneous psychiatric sample: domain facet and residualized facet descriptions. J. Pers. 74, 479–510. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00382.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dick, D. M., Agrawal, A., Keller, M. C., Adkins, A., Aliev, F., Monroe, S., et al. (2015). Candidate gene–environment interaction research. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 37–59. doi: 10.1177/1745691614556682

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebert, A., Edel, M.-A., Gilbert, P., and Brüne, M. (2018). Endogenous oxytocin is associated with the experience of compassion and recalled upbringing in Borderline Personality Disorder. Depress. Anxiety 35, 50–57. doi: 10.1002/da.22683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebstein, R. P., Knafo, A., Mankuta, D., Chew, S. H., and Lai, P. S. (2012). The contributions of oxytocin and vasopressin pathway genes to human behavior. Horm. Behav. 61, 359–379. doi: 10.1016/J.YHBEH.2011.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 51, 665–697. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.665

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., Eggum, N. D., and Giunta, L. (2010). Empathy-related responding: associations with prosocial behavior, aggression, and intergroup relations. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 4, 143–180. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-2409.2010.01020.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Karbon, M., Smith, M., and Maszk, P. (1996). The relations of children's dispositional empathy-related responding to their emotionality, regulation, and social functioning. Dev. Psychol. 32, 195–209. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.32.2.195

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elovainio, M., Jokela, M., Kivimäki, M., Pulkki-Råback, L., Lehtimäki, T., Airla, N., et al. (2007). Genetic variants in the DRD2 gene moderate the relationship between stressful life events and depressive symptoms in adults: cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. Psychosom. Med. 69, 391–395. doi: 10.1097/psy.0b013e31806bf365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Weisman, O., Schneiderman, I., Gordon, I., Maoz, R., et al. (2012). Sensitive parenting is associated with plasma oxytocin and polymorphisms in the OXTR and CD38 genes. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 175–181. doi: 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2011.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia, D., Lester, N., Cloninger, K. M., and Cloninger, C. R. (2017). “Cooperativeness,” in Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences, eds V. Zeigler-Hill and T. Shackelford (Cham: Springer), 1–3.

Google Scholar

García, Ó., Aluja, A., García, L. F., Escorial, S., and Blanch, A. (2012). Zuckerman-Kuhlman-Aluja Personality Questionnaire (ZKA-PQ) and Cloninger's Temperament and Character Inventory Revised (TCI-R): a comparative study. Scand. J. Psychol. 53, 247–257. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2012.00943.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. (2015). The evolution and social dynamics of compassion. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 9, 239–254. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. (2020). Compassion: from its evolution to a psychotherapy. Front. Psychol. 11:586161. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.586161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, P., Basran, J., MacArthur, M., and Kirby, J. N. (2019). Differences in the semantics of prosocial words: an exploration of compassion and kindness. Mindfulness 10, 2259–2271. doi: 10.1007/s12671-019-01191-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillath, O., Shaver, P. R., Baek, J.-M., and Chun, D. S. (2008). Genetic correlates of adult attachment style. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 1396–1405. doi: 10.1177/0146167208321484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillespie, N. A., Cloninger, C. R., Heath, A. C., and Martin, N. G. (2003). The genetic and environmental relationship between Cloninger's dimensions of temperament and character. Pers. Individ. Dif. 35, 1931–1946. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00042-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gluschkoff, K., Pulkki-Råback, L., Elovainio, M., Saarinen, A., Tammelin, T., Hirvensalo, M., et al. (2019). Is it good to be good? Dispositional compassion and health behaviors. Ann. Behav. Med. 53, 665–673. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D., and Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion: an evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychol. Bull. 136, 351–374. doi: 10.1037/a0018807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goncalves, D. M., and Cloninger, C. R. (2010). Validation and normative studies of the Brazilian Portuguese and American versions of the Temperament and Character Inventory - Revised (TCI-R). J. Affect. Disord. 124, 126–133. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gong, P., Fan, H., Liu, J., Yang, X., Zhang, K., and Zhou, X. (2017). Revisiting the impact of OXTR rs53576 on empathy: a population-based study and a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 80, 131–136. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2017.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gong, P., Liu, J., Li, S., and Zhou, X. (2014). Dopamine beta-hydroxylase gene modulates individuals' empathic ability. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1341–1345. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertzog, C., von Oertzen, T., Ghisletta, P., and Lindenberger, U. (2008). Evaluating the power of latent growth curve models to detect individual differences in change. Struct. Equat. Model. 15, 541–563. doi: 10.1080/10705510802338983

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, C., Reif, A., Strobel, A., Boreatti-Hümmer, A., Heine, M., Lesch, K.-P., et al. (2009). A functional dopamine-β-hydroxylase gene promoter polymorphism is associated with impulsive personality styles, but not with affective disorders. J. Neural Transm. 116, 121–130. doi: 10.1007/s00702-008-0138-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hewitt, J. K. (2012). Editorial policy on candidate gene association and candidate gene-by-environment interaction studies of complex traits. Behav. Genet. 42, 1–2. doi: 10.1007/s10519-011-9504-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hintsanen, M., Gluschkoff, K., Dobewall, H., Cloninger, C. R., Keltner, D., Saarinen, A., et al. (2019). Parent–child-relationship quality predicts offspring dispositional compassion in adulthood: a prospective follow-up study over three decades. Dev. Psychol. 55, 216–225. doi: 10.1037/dev0000633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huetter, F. K., Bachmann, H. S., Reinders, A., Siffert, D., Stelmach, P., Knop, D., et al. (2016). Association of a common oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism with self-reported ‘empathic concern' in a large population of healthy volunteers. PLoS ONE 11:e0160059. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Immordino-Yang, M. H., McColl, A., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A. (2009). Neural correlates of admiration and compassion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 8021–8026. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810363106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inagaki, T. K., Haltom, K. E. B., Suzuki, S., Jevtic, I., Hornstein, E., Bower, J. E., et al. (2016). The neurobiology of giving versus receiving support: the role of stress-related and social reward-related neural activity. Psychos. Med. 78:443. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isiordia, M., and Ferrer, E. (2018). Curve of factors model: a Latent growth modeling approach for educational research. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 78, 203–231. doi: 10.1177/0013164416677143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jern, P., Verweij, K. J. H., Barlow, F. K., and Zietsch, B. P. (2017). Reported associations between receptor genes and human sociality are explained by methodological errors and do not replicate. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, E9185–E9186. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1710880114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, D., Liu, H.-X., Hirai, H., Torashima, T., Nagai, T., Lopatina, O., et al. (2007). CD38 is critical for social behaviour by regulating oxytocin secretion. Nature 446, 41–45. doi: 10.1038/nature05526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keum, S., and Shin, H.-S. (2019). Genetic factors associated with empathy in humans and mice. Neuropharmacology 159:107514. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROPHARM.2019.01.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J.-W., Kim, S.-E., Kim, J.-J., Jeong, B., Park, C.-H., Son, A. R., et al. (2009). Compassionate attitude towards others' suffering activates the mesolimbic neural system. Neuropsychologia 47, 2073–2081. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2009.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Lamm, C., and Singer, T. (2013). Functional neural plasticity and associated changes in positive affect after compassion training. Cereb. Cortex 23, 1552–1561. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Ricard, M., and Singer, T. (2014). Differential pattern of functional brain plasticity after compassion and empathy training. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 873–879. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knafo, A., and Israel, S. (2010). “Genetic and environmental influences on prosocial behavior,” in Prosocial Motives, Emotions, and Behavior: The Better Angels of Our Nature, eds M. Mikulincer and P. R. Shaver (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 149–167. doi: 10.1037/12061-008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knafo, A., and Plomin, R. (2006). Parental discipline and affection and children's prosocial behavior: genetic and environmental links. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90, 147–164. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.1.147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsen, R. J., and Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 132–140. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laursen, H. R., Siebner, H. R., Haren, T., Madsen, K., GrÃ,nlund, R., Hulme, O., et al. (2014). Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene is associated with behavioral and neural correlates of empathic accuracy. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:423. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and Adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Lebowitz, M. S., and Dovidio, J. F. (2015). Implications of emotion regulation strategies for empathic concern, social attitudes, and helping behavior. Emotion 15, 187–194. doi: 10.1037/a0038820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leerkes, E. M., Su, J., Calkins, S., Henrich, V. C., and Smolen, A. (2017). Variation in mothers' arginine vasopressin receptor 1a and dopamine receptor D4 genes predicts maternal sensitivity via social cognition. Genes Brain Behav. 16, 233–240. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J., Zhao, Y., Li, R., Broster, L. S., Zhou, C., and Yang, S. (2015). Association of oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) rs53576 polymorphism with sociality: a meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 10:e0131820. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Gong, P., Li, H., and Zhou, X. (2017). A field study of the association between CD38 gene and altruistic behavior: empathic response as a mediator. Psychoneuroendocrinology 85, 165–171. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2017.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, W.-J., Hashii, M., Munesue, T., Hayashi, K., Yagi, K., Yamagishi, M., et al. (2013). Non-synonymous single-nucleotide variations of the human oxytocin receptor gene and autism spectrum disorders: a case–control study in a Japanese population and functional analysis. Mol. Autism 4:22. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-4-22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manolio, T. A., Collins, F. S., Cox, N. J., Goldstein, D. B., Hindorff, L. A., Hunter, D. J., et al. (2009). Finding the missing heritability of complex diseases. Nature 461, 747–753. doi: 10.1038/nature08494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, M. D., Bourdélat-Parks, B. N., Cameron Liles, L., and Weinshenker, D. (2005). Genetic reduction of noradrenergic function alters social memory and reduces aggression in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 161, 197–203. doi: 10.1016/J.BBR.2005.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melchers, M., Montag, C., Markett, S., Niazy, N., Groß-Bölting, J., Zimmermann, J., et al. (2017). The OXTR gene, implicit learning and social processing: does empathy evolve from perceptual skills for details? Behav. Brain Res. 329, 35–40. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.04.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miettunen, J., Veijola, J., Lauronen, E., Kantojärvi, L., and Joukamaa, M. (2007). Sex differences in Cloninger's temperament dimensions-a meta-analysis. Compr. Psychiatry 48, 161–169. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mustapic, M., Maihofer, A. X., Mahata, M., Chen, Y., Baker, D. G., O'Connor, D. T., et al. (2014). The catecholamine biosynthetic enzyme dopamine β-hydroxylase (DBH): first genome-wide search positions trait-determining variants acting additively in the proximal promoter. Hum. Mol. Genet. 23, 6375–6384. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddu332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nettle, D. (2006). The evolution of personality variation in humans and other animals. Am. Psychol. 61, 622–631. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.6.622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nikolova, Y. S., Ferrell, R. E., Manuck, S. B., and Hariri, A. R. (2011). Multilocus genetic profile for dopamine signaling predicts ventral striatum reactivity. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 1940–1947. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.82

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noble, E. P., Gottschalk, L. A., Fallon, J. H., Ritchie, T. L., and Wu, J. C. (1997). D2 dopamine receptor polymorphism and brain regional glucose metabolism. Am. J. Med. Genet. 74, 162–166.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

O'Connell, G., Whalley, H. C., Mukherjee, P., Stanfield, A. C., Montag, C., Hall, J., et al. (2012). Association of genetic variation in the promoter region of OXTR with differences in social affective neural processing. J. Behav. Brain Sci. 2, 60–66. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2012.21007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oertzen, T., Hertzog, C., Lindenberger, U., and Ghisletta, P. (2010). The effect of multiple indicators on the power to detect inter-individual differences in change. Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol. 63, 627–646. doi: 10.1348/000711010X486633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oveis, C., Horberg, E. J., and Keltner, D. (2010). Compassion, pride, and social intuitions of self-other similarity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 618–630. doi: 10.1037/a0017628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pace, T. W. W., Negi, L. T., Adame, D. D., Cole, S. P., Sivilli, T. I., Brown, T. D., et al. (2009). Effect of compassion meditation on neuroendocrine, innate immune and behavioral responses to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 87–98. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2008.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, N., Price, A. L., and Reich, D. (2006). Population structure and eigenanalysis. PLoS Genet. 2:e190. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.0020190

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearce, E., Wlodarski, R., Machin, A., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2017). Variation in the β-endorphin, oxytocin, and dopamine receptor genes is associated with different dimensions of human sociality. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 5300–5305. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1700712114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearce, E., Wlodarski, R., Machin, A., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2018). The influence of genetic variation on social disposition, romantic relationships and social networks: a replication study. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 4, 400–422. doi: 10.1007/s40750-018-0101-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pełka-Wysiecka, J., Zietek, J., Grzywacz, A., Kucharska-Mazur, J., Bienkowski, P., and Samochowiec, J. (2012). Association of genetic polymorphisms with personality profile in individuals without psychiatric disorders. Prog. Neuro Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry, 39, 40–46. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfattheicher, S., Sassenrath, C., and Keller, J. (2019). Compassion magnifies third-party punishment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 117, 124–141. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000165

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pohjalainen, T., Rinne, J. O., Någren, K., Lehikoinen, P., Anttila, K., Syvälahti, E. K., et al. (1998). The A1 allele of the human D2 dopamine receptor gene predicts low D2 receptor availability in healthy volunteers. Mol. Psychiatry 3, 256–260.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Pommier, E., Neff, K. D., and Tóth-Király, I. (2019). The development and validation of the compassion scale. Assessment 27, 21–39. doi: 10.1177/1073191119874108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Post, S. G. (2005). Altruism, happiness, and health: it's good to be good. Int. J. Behav. Med. 12, 66–77. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm1202_4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poulin, M. J. (2017). “To help or not to help: goal commitment and the goodness of compassion,” in The Oxford Handbook of Compassion Science, eds J. R. Seppälä, E. M., Simon-Thomas, E., Brown, S. L., Worline, M. C., Cameron, and L., Doty (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 355–367.

Google Scholar

Raitakari, O. T., Juonala, M., Rönnemaa, T., Keltikangas-Järvinen, L., Räsänen, L., Pietikäinen, M., et al. (2008). Cohort profile: the cardiovascular risk in Young Finns Study. Int. J. Epidemiol. 37, 1220–1226. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter, M., Frenzel, C., Walter, N. T., Markett, S., and Montag, C. (2011). Investigating the genetic basis of altruism: the role of the COMT Val158Met polymorphism. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 662–668. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter, M., Montag, C., Altmann, S., Bendlow, F., Elger, C., Kirsch, P., et al. (2017). Functional characterization of an oxytocin receptor gene variant (rs2268498) previously associated with social cognition by expression analysis in vitro and in human brain biopsy. Soc. Neurosci. 12, 604–611. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2016.1214174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodrigues, S. M., Saslow, L. R., Garcia, N., John, O. P., and Keltner, D. (2009). Oxytocin receptor genetic variation relates to empathy and stress reactivity in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 21437–21441. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909579106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ru, W., Fang, P., Wang, B., Yang, X., Zhu, X., Xue, M., et al. (2017). The impacts of Val158Met in Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene on moral permissibility and empathic concern. Pers. Individ. Dif. 106, 52–56. doi: 10.1016/J.PAID.2016.10.041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudolph, U., Roesch, S., Greitemeyer, T., and Weiner, B. (2004). A meta-analytic review of help giving and aggression from an attributional perspective: contributions to a general theory of motivation. Cogn. Emot. 18, 815–848. doi: 10.1080/02699930341000248

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saarinen, A., Keltikangas-Järvinen, L., Cloninger, C. R., Veijola, J., Elovainio, M., Lehtimäki, T., et al. (2019a). The relationship of dispositional compassion for others with depressive symptoms over a 15-year prospective follow-up. J. Affect. Disord. 250, 354–362. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.03.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saarinen, A., Keltikangas-Järvinen, L., Pulkki-Råback, L., Cloninger, C. R., Elovainio, M., Lehtimäki, T., et al. (2019b). The relationship of dispositional compassion with well-being: a study with a 15-year prospective follow-up. J. Posit. Psychol. 15, 806–820. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2019.1663251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., and Allik, J. (2008). Why can't a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in big five personality traits across 55 cultures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 94, 168–182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shang, S., Wu, N., and Su, Y. (2017). How oxytocin receptor (OXTR) single nucleotide polymorphisms act on prosociality: the mediation role of moral evaluation. Front. Psychol. 8:396. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skuse, D. H., and Gallagher, L. (2009). Dopaminergic-neuropeptide interactions in the social brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 27–35. doi: 10.1016/J.TICS.2008.09.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. N., Chen, W., Kähönen, M., Kettunen, J., Lehtimäki, T., Peltonen, L., et al. (2010). Longitudinal genome-wide association of cardiovascular disease risk factors in the bogalusa heart study. PLoS Genet. 6:e1001094. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1001094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, K. E., Porges, E. C., Norman, G. J., Connelly, J. J., and Decety, J. (2014). Oxytocin receptor gene variation predicts empathic concern and autonomic arousal while perceiving harm to others. Soc. Neurosci. 9, 1–9. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2013.863223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steffen, P. R., and Masters, K. S. (2005). Does compassion mediate the intrinsic religion-health relationship? Ann. Behav. Med. 30, 217–224. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm3003_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, D. J., Newman, T. K., Savitz, J., and Ramesar, R. (2006). Warriors versus worriers: the role of COMT gene variants. CNS Spectr. 11, 745–748. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900014863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stellar, J. E., Cohen, A., Oveis, C., and Keltner, D. (2015). Affective and physiological responses to the suffering of others: compassion and vagal activity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 108, 572–585. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., Spoor, S., Bohon, C., and Small, D. M. (2008). Relation between obesity and blunted striatal response to food is moderated by TaqIA A1 allele. Science 322, 449–452. doi: 10.1126/science.1161550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, C., Lever Taylor, B., Gu, J., Kuyken, W., Baer, R., Jones, F., et al. (2016). What is compassion and how can we measure it? A review of definitions and measures. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 47, 15–27. doi: 10.1016/J.CPR.2016.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tunbridge, E. M., Bannerman, D. M., Sharp, T., and Harrison, P. J. (2004). Catechol-O-methyltransferase inhibition improves set-shifting serformance and elevates stimulated dopamine release in the rat prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 24, 5331–5335. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1124-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uzefovsky, F., Shalev, I., Israel, S., Edelman, S., Raz, Y., Perach-Barzilay, N., et al. (2014). The Dopamine D4 receptor gene shows a gender-sensitive association with cognitive empathy: evidence from two independent samples. Emotion 14, 712–721. doi: 10.1037/a0036555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vitoratou, S., Ntzoufras, I., Theleritis, C., Smyrnis, N., and Stefanis, N. C. (2015). Temperament and character dimensions assessed in general population, in individuals with psychoactive substance dependence and in young male conscripts. Eur. Psychiatry 30, 474–479. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warrier, V., Grasby, K. L., Uzefovsky, F., Toro, R., Smith, P., Chakrabarti, B., et al. (2018a). Genome-wide meta-analysis of cognitive empathy: heritability, and correlates with sex, neuropsychiatric conditions and cognition. Mol. Psychiatry 23, 1402–1409. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warrier, V., Toro, R., Chakrabarti, B., Børglum, A. D., Grove, J., Hinds, D. A., et al. (2018b). Genome-wide analyses of self-reported empathy: correlations with autism, schizophrenia, and anorexia nervosa. Transl. Psychiatry 8:35. doi: 10.1038/s41398-017-0082-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zabetian, C. P., Anderson, G. M., Buxbaum, S. G., Elston, R. C., Ichinose, H., Nagatsu, T., et al. (2001). A quantitative-trait analysis of human plasma–dopamine β-hydroxylase activity: evidence for a major functional polymorphism at the DBH locus. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 68, 515–522. doi: 10.1086/318198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Q., Ting-Toomey, S., and Oetzel, J. G. (2014). Linking emotion to the Conflict Face-Negotiation Theory: a U.S.-China investigation of the mediating effects of anger, compassion, and guilt in interpersonal conflict. Hum. Commun. Res. 40, 373–395. doi: 10.1111/hcre.12029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmermann, J., Deris, N., Montag, C., Reuter, M., Felten, A., Becker, B., et al. (2018). A common polymorphism on the oxytocin receptor gene (rs2268498) and resting-state functional connectivity of amygdala subregions: a genetic imaging study. Neuroimage 179, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwir, I., Arnedo, J., Del-Val, C., Pulkki-Råback, L., Konte, B., Yang, S. S., et al. (2018). Uncovering the complex genetics of human character. Mol. Psychiatry 25, 2295–2312. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0263-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: oxytocin, prosocial traits, personality development, compassion, dopamine

Citation: Dobewall H, Saarinen A, Lyytikäinen L-P, Keltikangas-Järvinen L, Lehtimäki T and Hintsanen M (2021) Functional Polymorphisms in Oxytocin and Dopamine Pathway Genes and the Development of Dispositional Compassion Over Time: The Young Finns Study. Front. Psychol. 12:576346. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.576346

Received: 21 July 2020; Accepted: 03 March 2021;
Published: 08 April 2021.

Edited by:

Qinghua He, Southwest University, China

Reviewed by:

Jin Li, Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS), China
Paul Gilbert, NHS England, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2021 Dobewall, Saarinen, Lyytikäinen, Keltikangas-Järvinen, Lehtimäki and Hintsanen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Henrik Dobewall, henrik.dobewall@oulu.fi

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.