Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 30 April 2021
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Appraisal of the Fairness Moral Foundation Predicts the Language Use Involving Moral Issues on Twitter Among Japanese

\r\nAkiko Matsuo*Akiko Matsuo1*Baofa DuBaofa Du2Kazutoshi SasaharaKazutoshi Sasahara3
  • 1Department of Psychology, Tokai Gakuen University, Nagoya, Japan
  • 2Department of Complex Systems Science, Graduate School of Informatics, Nagoya University, Nagoya, Japan
  • 3Department of Innovation Science, School of Environment and Society, Tokyo Institute of Technology, Tokyo, Japan

Moral appraisals are found to be associated with a person’s individual differences (e.g., political ideology), and the effects of individual differences on language use have been studied within the framework of the Moral Foundations Theory (MFT). However, the relationship between one’s moral concern and the use of language involving morality on social media is not self-evident. The present exploratory study investigated that relationship using the MFT. Participants’ tweets and self-reported responses to the questionnaire were collected to measure the degree of their appraisals according to the five foundations of the MFT. The Japanese version of the Moral Foundations Dictionary (J-MFD) was used to quantify the number of words in tweets relevant to the MFT’s five moral foundations. The results showed that endorsement of the Fairness and Authority foundations predicted the word frequency in the J-MFD across all five foundations. The findings suggest that the trade-off relationship between the Fairness and Authority foundations plays a key role in online language communication. The implications and future directions to scrutinize that foundation are discussed.

Introduction

In their Moral Foundations Theory (MFT), Graham et al. (2013) proposed that people utilize the five foundations—(1) Care (not harming), (2) Fairness (not cheating), (3) Ingroup (not betraying), (4) Authority (not subverting), and (5) Purity (not contaminating)—to make moral judgments. The Care and Fairness foundations are referred to as the individualizing moral foundations, which focus on individual rights and autonomy, whereas the Ingroup, Authority, and Purity foundations are called the binding moral foundations, which focus on collective good and group coherence and hierarchy. The five foundations function as the standard for discriminating right from wrong in social situations. The ingroup members determine which of those five foundations are emphasized in organizing the group. People in a group often deal with morality-related situations by exchanging words about interpretations of those situations. In reinforcing the sharedness of the moral standard, language plays a key role in transmitting the set of moral norms shared in the group.

Language is crucial to human morality because it enables moral norms to regulate “should” and “should not” explicitly and the subsequent punishment system (Saucier, 2018; cf. DeScioli and Kurzban, 2018). Today, the linguistic communication of human beings is not limited to face-to-face physical interactions. Social media (e.g., blogs, Facebook, Twitter, and Instagram), in particular, has become a dominant mode of interaction in which physically distant people from various backgrounds can be connected. Cyber space may look different from physical space, but people express their experiences, feelings, policies, and values with language spontaneously in both spaces. Boyd and Pennebaker (2017) advocated that the digital world is where one’s personality would be embedded and that it is a useful platform to conduct personality research combining with self-report measures. For example, Qiu et al. (2012) investigated the relationship between personality and language use in microblogs and showed that personality traits, such as neuroticism, are expressed in linguistic patterns. As for morality, Brady et al. (2020) argued that social media is a place that enables moralized content to be addressed and transmitted because people tend to capture moralized content rapidly and spread moral-emotional content. It is necessary to be familiar with online language in relation to the social media platform that is being interacted with in order to know, understand, and interpret moral issues these days. People sometimes express their position about a moral issues and spread social issues by sharing it with others.

Research on people’s online language use in terms of morality has recently been accumulated with the help of Natural Language Processing and Text Mining (Kao and Poteet, 2007). Graham et al. (2009) developed a tool called the Moral Foundations Dictionary (MFD) to quantify the use of words in written texts referring to virtues and vices that are associated with each moral foundation. The MFD is a list of words that are relevant to each of the five foundations. There are virtue and vice word lists for each foundation. Combined with the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count software (Pennebaker et al., 2001), the MFD enabled past empirical studies to quantify morality from written texts. For example, using this dictionary, Graham et al. (2009) analyzed the online text data of church sermons and found a higher frequency of words related to the Care, Fairness, and Ingroup foundations in sermons made by liberal preachers than those by their conservative counterparts (see also Takikawa and Sakamoto, 2019). Stolerman and Lagnado (2019) analyzed newspaper articles relevant to human rights and found that these articles contained more individualizing-related words (i.e., Care and Fairness). The previous research demonstrated that individual differences in political ideology are associated with language use involving morality. Later, Frimer (2020) replicated Graham et al.’s research and found the differences in language use focusing on political ideology are limited to religious contexts. Even the results that supported Graham et al.’s research showed small effect sizes. Frimer (2020) argues that liberals and conservatives may not speak different languages as much as researchers previously thought.

Although research on people’s language use between Democrats and Republicans has been accumulated, as mentioned above, the relationship between individual differences in terms of moral concern and the everyday online use of language has not been directly explored yet. It is important to cast light on individual differences as factors that affect morality-related language use because people’s political ideologies are associated with their moral concern (DeScioli et al., 2014). Understanding the patterns of the relationship between one’s moral concern as an individual difference and language use should help reveal how people interact publicly with others by using morality-related words in the digital space where they can choose to talk about anything. Individuals who personally rely on a certain moral foundation when making moral judgments can have a distinct pattern in their online language use. If this is the case, online language communication would be systematically framed by the communicator’s moral appraisal.

In addition, the effects of culture on the prioritized moral foundation(s) have been reported (Graham et al., 2011). The cultural effects are reasonable because moral judgment has an ultimate goal: to distinguish good from bad in a culturally correct way (Haidt, 2001; Haidt and Joseph, 2004). For example, people in the United States tend to interpret moral behavior from the view of the individual foundations, while Filipinos employ all of the foundations (Vauclair and Fischer, 2011; see also Vasquez et al., 2001). However, most morality research has been conducted in the so-called WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) cultural groups because many of the experimental measures, such as vignettes and video clips, are only available in English (Henrich et al., 2010; Clifford et al., 2015). The investigation of the use of language from the perspective of morality in a non-WEIRD culture is needed because it is not clear whether the findings obtained in research that was conducted in the WEIRD cultures can be generalized to other cultures.

Therefore, the present study is a preliminary investigation of the relationship between one’s moral concern as an individual difference and the use of language related to moral foundations in their everyday tweets among people in Japan. The newly developed Japanese version of the MFD (J-MFD; Matsuo et al., 2019) enabled the investigation. The present study employs a dataset that links the scores indicating one’s concern for the five moral foundations and their tweets: the first attempt to observe a specific moral concern guiding people to asymmetric language use on social media among Japanese.

Methods

Participants

Macromill, an Internet crowdsourcing service, recruited 386 Japanese participants who took part in our online survey. Among the original sample of 386 participants, 51 reported that they either did not have a Twitter account, had private accounts only, or refused to provide information about their accounts; 31 participants had accounts with zero tweets. The responses from the remaining 304 participants (386 − 51 − 31 = 304) were used for our data analysis (185 men and 119 women). Their ages ranged from 18 to 74 years old (M = 36.05, SD = 12.78).

Materials

Moral Foundations Questionnaire

this questionnaire measures the degree of one’s concern for each of the five moral foundations based on MFT (Graham et al., 2011). To measure Moral Foundations Questionnaire (MFQ) scores in Japanese, we used the 30-item version of the Japanese MFQ (available at www.moralfoundations.org and Kanai, 2013). The Japanese version of the MFQ was found to have a five-factor model as the MFT predicted (Honda et al., 2016). Recently, Murayama and Miura (2019) determined the validity and reliability of the Japanese MFQ with large Japanese samples (855 Japanese participants in Study 1 and 470 in Study 2) and found that the five-factor model was the best fit. The Japanese MFQ has been used in previous research on morality conducted in Japan (e.g., Takamatsu and Takai, 2017; Matsuo et al., 2019). The Japanese MFQ consists of sentences that are rated on a Likert-type scale, ranging from 0 (not at all/almost never) to 5 (very much/almost always). There are six items for each of the five foundations. Responses are summed to create a foundation score for each foundation. A higher score indicates a higher level of concern for that foundation.

The Japanese Version of the MFD

Matsuo et al. (2019) translated the original MFD, which contains 324 English words, into Japanese using a semi-automated method. The authors provided evidence of its validity by comparing the number of the J-MFD words for each moral foundation in the situations that participants described as each foundation being followed and violated. The J-MFD includes 718 Japanese moral words with 11 categories corresponding to “Virtue” or “Vice” (violates); each is associated with one of the five moral foundations (i.e., Care, Fairness, Ingroup, Authority, and Purity) and included a more general or abstract category of morality (i.e., Morality General) (Matsuo et al., 2019). Because each English word in the original MFD possibly had more than one translated word in Japanese, the number of Japanese words in each category of the J-MFD became more than the original MFD. The J-MFD and a computer program for Japanese word segmentation are publicly available online1.

Data Analysis

Looking at their Twitter accounts, we collected the tweets of the 304 participants (all the available user timelines) and applied the J-MFD to these tweets. The number of tweets per person ranged from 68 to 26,754 (Mtweets per person = 4,092.37, SDtweets per person = 2,709.90). The number of word counts per person ranged from 2,440 to 1,202,062 (Mwords per person = 145,565.62, SDwords per person = 133,017.30). We also collected self-reported responses to the MFQ from the same Japanese sample. We computed the “frequency ratio” of appearances of J-MFD words associated with each moral foundation (Virtue and Vice categories combined) as the same way Matsuo et al. (2019) did. We divided the word counts in Twitter by the size of the total words included in one’s tweets to obtain the ratio of J-MFD words in each participant’s tweet [e.g., the number of Care-related words in one’s tweets divided by the total word counts in his/her tweets, which is indicated as (i) here]. To obtain the ratio scores to be used for our data analysis, we divided that ratio of the J-MFD words [(i)] by the total number of dictionary words for each moral foundation [e.g., (i) divided by the number of the Care-related words in the J-MFD]. The relationship between the frequency ratio of J-MFD words and the MFQ scores was examined.

Results

Tables 1, 2 show the means and standard deviations for the total rating scores on the MFQ by subscale and total word frequencies for each moral foundation. The correlations between the word frequencies for the Virtue and Vice categories for each moral foundation are shown in Supplementary Appendix A.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. MFQ correlations with means and standard deviations of its subscales.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlations of word frequencies with means and standard deviations for each foundation.

In Table 3, the five most frequent word in the total tweets by the J-MFD subscale is described.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The five most frequent words in the total tweets by the J-MFD subscale.

A series of multiple regression analyses were performed to investigate how the frequency ratio of appearances of J-MFD words associated with each moral foundation was predicted by one’s moral concern represented by the MFQ scores. As shown in Table 4, there was a significant main effect for the MFQ Fairness score (positive effect) and for the Authority score (negative effect) on the word frequency score for all the foundations. Only for the Purity foundation were the main effects of the MFQ Care score (positive effect) and Purity score (negative effect) additionally observed.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Summary of multiple regression and correlation analyses for variables predicting MFQ scores.

Thus, the results showed that the degree of concern for the Fairness and Authority foundations as the standards for moral judgment among Japanese individuals predicted their use of the words involving all of the five foundations.

A series of post hoc multiple regression analyses were conducted to investigate which Fairness-item(s) in the MFQ predicted the frequency of Fairness-related words in the J-MFD and which Authority-item(s) in the MFQ predicted the frequency of Authority-related words in the J-MFD. The results showed that the only Fairness-item in the MFQ (“When you decide whether something is right or wrong, to what extent is whether or not someone was denied his or her rights relevant to your thinking?”) significantly predicted the frequency of moral-related words across the five foundations on participants’ tweets (all p values < 0.001). Also, the only Authority-related item in the MFQ (“Respect for authority is something all children need to learn”) significantly predicted the frequency of moral-related words across four foundations on their tweets only Purity foundation was (non-significant, p = 0.07).

Discussion

The present study aimed at exploring the effect of one’s emphasis on a moral foundation on their use of language on a social networking site among Japanese. The findings suggest distinct patterns in the interaction of language use and one’s concern for each moral foundation in the online communication setting. We found that one’s endorsement of the Fairness and Authority foundations predicted the frequency of words included in the J-MFD across all the five foundations.

In the present study, we found in our participants’ tweets that the most frequent Vice word for Fairness was “discrimination,” and that the words for Authority, Virtue, and Vice were “control” and “defiant”; these words, from those two foundations, are in conflict. Likewise, the Fairness item and the Authority item in the MFQ that best predicted the moral-relevant word frequency across the five foundations, are about having equal rights as others and the endorsement of authority from early childhood. Thus, the patterns of the results from the present study can be discussed from either of the perspectives of Fairness or Authority foundation because those two foundations are in a trade-off relationship. In fact, some previous research discusses these two foundations as the two sides of the same coin. The findings from Vecina and Piñuela (2017) suggested that endorsement of equal rights (i.e., Fairness foundation) is in conflict with someone else’s dominance (i.e., Authority foundation). Other previous research also discusses that it may be reasonable to regard these two foundations in the trade-off relationship rather than dealing with those foundations separately in isolation (Waytz et al., 2013; Monroe et al., 2020). Given the trade-off relationship, hereafter, we discuss our results from the Fairness perspective because it is generally easier to understand a positive relationship with moral-related online language use than a negative relationship between the concern for the Authority foundation and the use of language.

Our results show that Fairness concern is a strong predictor of moral-relevant word use in online language that occurs spontaneously. Indeed, Piazza et al. (2018) revealed that appraisals of injustice were, compared to the other kinds of appraisals (i.e., harm, disloyalty, authority, and impurity), the only significant predictor of intensity ratings of wrongdoing for all five moral foundations. Our findings may give further evidence to Piazza et al. (2018) by showing the universality of the centrality of Fairness in the MFT with another approach. Or it may be reasonable to suggest that the present results show Westernization among Japanese because Western people were found to be more appreciative of individual rights and justice (i.e., Care and Fairness foundations) than East-Asian people (Graham et al., 2013). Discussions suggest that the Japanese culture is becoming more individualistic than in the past (Matsumoto et al., 1996; Hamamura, 2012). Modern Japanese people may come to value equal rights and individual autonomy in the future. Because the present study recruited participants from all generations and all living areas, the trend of individualization (if any) should be attributed to Japanese society. The correlation between MFQ/MFD scores and age from our results (see Supplementary Appendix B) features the positive correlation between the MFQ Authority score and age; it may be a clue to capture possible differences in cherished moral values across generations in Japan. The present study cast light on the importance of Fairness, which can lead to future cross-cultural and longitudinal research to investigate this foundation.

Concerning the relationship between individual differences and behavior in language communication, Sylwester and Purver (2015) showed that conservative users of Twitter use more group-identity and religious words than liberal users, which reflects liberals’ tendency to emphasize the Ingroup, Authority, and Purity foundations less. As people’s interactions are widely expanded through cyberspace, it would not be surprising that their individual differences, such as moral concern, are disclosed online as shown in the present study that demonstrated that one’s language expressions are reflected by their tendency toward moral concern, which was not self-evident.

The present findings take initial steps toward a more comprehensive picture of how people communicate in moral terms and the effects of such communication on their actual behavior by incorporating traditional moral research ideas and methodology into the practical issue of the gap between inside and outside the lab. The Fairness Foundation is known as one of the moral foundations that are appreciated by liberals in the West (e.g., Graham et al., 2009). Henderson and Dressler (2019) state that the extent to which inequality can be endorsed is considered one of the most significant psychological dimensions that separate conservatives from liberals. Liberals are opposed to inequality, whereas conservatives have a higher tolerance for it because conservatives value authority more than liberals. On the other hand, differences in Japanese political ideology and the behavioral tendencies of the Japanese as a function of political affiliation are not transparent. Because Japanese people do not perceive themselves in a liberal vs. conservative framework, measuring their political ideology can depend on how researchers view Japanese political tendencies (Aoyama, 2019; Murayama and Miura, 2019). Future research will be enriched if the political ideology among the Japanese is captured in a deeper sense taking their unique culture into consideration.

People who are concerned with the Fairness or Authority foundation in Japan may be sensitive to the information about equality (cf. Gollwitzer and Bücklein, 2007). This tendency can be approached by the theory of justice sensitivity. Justice sensitivity is defined as a personality trait that puts importance on injustice and unfairness in everyday life (Schmitt et al., 1995). When this personality trait is connected with the notion of moral conviction, one’s motivational component concerning morality in the form of “oughts” or “shoulds” (Skitka and Mullen, 2002), people may declare what should be done to others. In today’s society, hate speech and SNS flaming exemplify the statement about one’s own “shoulds” for justice issues based on one’s moral conviction, which does not necessarily need reason or evidence. More recently, phenomena such as “refrain police” have been observed during the COVID-19 outbreak in Japan. Those “policemen/women” threaten others who violate the governmental self-restraint request based solely on their moral conviction. People who express their moral conviction specifically to show off their own moral quality by public discussion of morality and politics are referred to as moral grandstanders (Tosi and Warmke, 2016). These tendencies of moral conviction and moral grandstanding may hold their own set of moral norms about equality. It would be beneficial to investigate the mechanism and function of those tendencies within the framework of the MFT. On the other hand, Japanese people may have perceived the concept of fairness differently from Westerns in the first place (Kim and Leung, 2007). Thus, the Fairness foundation in Japan can be worth scrutinizing in future research.

Furthermore, the results showed a negative association between MFQ Authority and MFD Authority and the negative association between MFQ Purity and MFD Purity, which does not fit what one would intuitively expect. Given the zero-order correlations between the MFQ Authority and MFD foundation-related word frequency for all foundations and between the MFQ Purity score and MFD Purity-related word frequency, the MFQ Authority and Purity scores may serve as suppressor variables. If this is the case, at least it can be stated that the variables of the MFQ Authority/Purity scores can play an important role in predicting the MFD Authority/Purity-related word frequency (Knowlden, 2014). A better model with relevant variables will be constructed in future research.

A closer look at the Purity foundation can be another direction. We found a different pattern for Purity; only Purity-related online language use was predicted by one’s endorsement of the Care, Fairness, Authority, and Purity foundations. This finding may suggest that Purity has a distinct nature, compared to the other foundations, which has been explored in the previous research. For example, Kaur and Sasahara (2016) analyzed people’s tweets in English and found that Purity is a unique component separated by the other foundations. Also, Matsuo et al. (2017) discussed the possibility that Japanese people apply their unique lay theory to the interpretation of Shweder’s Divinity ethic (equivalent to Haidt’s Purity foundation). Their findings from the analyses of the participant-made moral transgressions suggest that Divinity violations and part of the Autonomy situations (equivalent to Haidt’s Care and Fairness foundations) are viewed under the same category in the Japanese cultural context. It may be hard to capture the possibly different nature of Purity among Japanese people with the Purity items on the MFQ because those items generally imply Christian concepts, such as the existence of only one God. Concerning this issue, Kitamura and Matsuo (2021) developed a tool to measure Japanese unique purity orientation, called the Purity Orientation-Pollution Avoidance Scale (POPA). The POPA scale measures Japanese people’s psychological pursuit for something pure and their avoidance of something polluted because of the experimental materials invented. This Japanese-customized scale would be a useful tool to quantify unique Japanese concepts of the pure and impure relevant to the Purity foundation in the MFT. Thus, the present study’s results may reflect difference(s) observed across cultures or a culturally unique Purity characteristic. The in-depth investigation of the perceptions of Purity among the Japanese should be awaited.

Overall, in the present study, online language use was found to reflect the users’ individual differences in moral concerns. The present study is a first step toward understanding how morality works among people in a non-Western culture. Using a promising tool such as the J-MFD, researchers interested in morality from any academic field can analyze texts that are filled with people’s mental representations about morality. In addition to controlled lab experiments, observations of people’s spontaneous expressions would be needed to clarify possible tendencies in language use because people do not randomly express their thoughts about morality. An investigation comparing Twitter users in English-speaking and Japanese-speaking cultures would also be beneficial. Any observed differences should be carefully scrutinized because those differences may come from using specific words with different connotations, not from the characteristics of cultures. Future research on morality—related to the sources of the differences—should investigate whether the present study’s findings can be extended with the modification or addition, or both of the original MFD and J-MFD. For example, Garten et al. (2016) attempt to extend the original MFD by combining the original MFD with the pre-trained distributed representations for words. Frimer et al. (unpublished data) developed a new MFD (MFD 2.0) by computing the prototypically of moral-related words that two of the authors selected. Garten et al. (2016) and Frimer et al. (unpublished data) based the selection of the words to be included in the MFD on the experts, which is similar to the development of the original MFD. On the other hand, Hopp et al. (2020) asked non-experts who were recruited through the crowdsourcing service to annotate moral-related words in news articles to extend the original MFD (eMFD). Our results in the present study would be analyzed more deeply (e.g., identifying prototypicality of each word in participants’ tweets) when the J-MFD takes those revisions of the original MFD into consideration. Because the J-MFD is open-access material and research can be revised, cooperating efforts to extend the J-MFD will enrich the analyses of text written in Japanese used for the research in various academic fields. For instance, like the extensions by the aforementioned studies, the J-MFD can be extended using automated and manual methods with the help of not only experts but also the layperson. Such extension will further contribute to identifying the mechanism and functions of human morality in their social life.

Data Availability Statement

The materials, including tweet IDs and analysis programs, are available online at https://osf.io/nd59g/.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval were not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

KS devised the study and collected data. BD and AM analyzed the data. AM wrote the manuscript. KS helped with writing and editing the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The present study was supported by JST PRESTO (Grant number: JPMJPR16D6) and JST CREST (Grant number: JPMJCR17A4 and JPMJCR20D3) to KS.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr. Minoru Karasawa, Dr. Christina Brown, and Yuri Tanaka for critical feedback on earlier drafts of this manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.599024/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://github.com/soramame0518/j-mfd/

References

Aoyama, M. (2019). A psychological approach to exploring japanese ideologies: based on the concept of morality and political orientation in moral foundations theory. Jpn. Soc. Global Soc. Cult. Stud. 16, 12–23.

Google Scholar

Boyd, R. L., and Pennebaker, J. W. (2017). Language-based personality: a new approach to personality in a digital world. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 18, 63–68. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.07.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brady, W. J., Crockett, M. J., and Van Bavel, J. J. (2020). The MAD model of moral contagion: the role of motivation, attention, and design in the spread of moralized content online. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15, 978–1010. doi: 10.1177/1745691620917336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clifford, S., Iyengar, V., Cabeza, R., and Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2015). Moral foundations vignettes: a standardized stimulus database of scenarios based on moral foundations theory. Behav. Res. Methods 47, 1178–1198. doi: 10.3758/s13428-014-0551-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeScioli, P., and Kurzban, R. (2018). “Morality is for choosing sides,” in Atlas of Moral Psychology, eds K. Gray and J. Graham (New York, NY: Guilford Publications), 177–185.

Google Scholar

DeScioli, P., Massenkoff, M., Shaw, A., Petersen, M. B., and Kurzban, R. (2014). Equity or equality? Moral judgments follow the money. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 281:20142112. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.2112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frimer, J. A. (2020). Do liberals and conservatives use different moral languages? Two replications and six extensions of Graham, Haidt, and Nosek’s (2009) moral text analysis. J. Res. Pers. 84:103906. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2019.103906

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garten, J., Boghrati, R., Hoover, J., Johnson, K. M., and Dehghani, M. (2016). “Morality between the lines: detecting moral sentiment in text,” in Proceedings of IJCAI 2016 Workshop on Computational Modeling of Attitudes, New York, NY.

Google Scholar

Gollwitzer, M., and Bücklein, K. (2007). Are “we” more punitive than “me”? Self-construal styles, justice-related attitudes, and punitive judgments. Soc. Justice Res. 20, 457–478. doi: 10.1007/s11211-007-0051-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S. P., et al. (2013). Moral foundations theory: the pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47, 55–130.

Google Scholar

Graham, J., Haidt, J., and Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 96, 1029–1046. doi: 10.1037/a0015141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., and Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 366–385.

Google Scholar

Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychol. Rev. 108, 814–834. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.4.814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haidt, J., and Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: how innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus 133, 55–66. doi: 10.1162/0011526042365555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamamura, T. (2012). Are cultures becoming individualistic? A cross-temporal comparison of individualism–collectivism in the United States and Japan. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 16, 3–24. doi: 10.1177/1088868311411587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, N. L., and Dressler, W. W. (2019). Cultural models of substance misuse risk and moral foundations: cognitive resources underlying stigma attribution. J. Cogn. Cult. 19, 78–96. doi: 10.1163/15685373-12340049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature 466:29. doi: 10.1038/466029a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honda, S., Ishimaru, S., Utsunomiya, S., Yamane, T., Oda, M., Sakamoto, K., et al. (2016). Considering the relation between the Haidt’s moral foundations theory and Japanese morality through developing a new scale. Bull. Fac. Educ. Yamaguchi Univ. 66, 95–106.

Google Scholar

Hopp, F. R., Fisher, J. T., Cornell, D., Huskey, R., and Weber, R. (2020). The extended moral foundations dictionary (eMFD): development and applications of a crowd-sourced approach to extracting moral intuitions from text. Behav. Res. Methods 53, 232–246. doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01433-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanai, R. (2013). Nou ni Kizamareta Moraru no Kigen: Hito wa Naze zen wo Motomerunoka. [The Origin of Morality Engraved in the Brain: Why do People Pursue Goodness?]. Tokyo: Iwanami Shoten.

Google Scholar

A. Kao and S. R. (eds.) Poteet (2007). Natural Language Processing and Text Mining. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media.

Google Scholar

Kaur, R., and Sasahara, K. (2016). “Quantifying moral foundations from various topics on Twitter conversations,” in Proceedings of the 2016 IEEE International Conference on Big Data (Big Data), (New York, NY: IEEE), 2505–2512.

Google Scholar

Kim, T. Y., and Leung, K. (2007). Forming and reacting to overall fairness: a cross-cultural comparison. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Processes 104, 83–95. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2007.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kitamura, H., and Matsuo, A. (2021). Development and validation of the purity orientation-pollution avoidance scale: a study with Japanese sample. Front. Psychol. 12:590595. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.590595/full

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knowlden, A. P. (2014). Role of suppressor variables in primary prevention obesity research: examples from two predictive models. Int. Schol. Res. Notices 2014:567523.

Google Scholar

Matsumoto, D., Kudoh, T., and Takeuchi, S. (1996). Changing patterns of individualism and collectivism in the United States and Japan. Cult. Psychol. 2, 77–107. doi: 10.1177/1354067x9621005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuo, A., Karasawa, M., and Norasakkunkit, V. (2017). “Why do people think of the situation as moral violation? Perceptions of Shweder’s “Big 3” morality in Japan and the U.S,” in Poster Presented at the Annual International Convention of Psychological Science Conference, Vienna.

Google Scholar

Matsuo, A., Sasahara, K., Taguchi, Y., and Karasawa, M. (2019). Development and validation of the Japanese moral foundations dictionary. PLoS One 14:e0213343. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monroe, A. E., Wyngaarden, J. B. III, and Plant, E. A. (2020). They should have followed the rules”: trade-offs between fairness and authority values predict judgments of social justice protests. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1177/1948550620923854

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murayama, A., and Miura, A. (2019). Validation of the Japanese version of the moral foundation questionnaire: investigating the relationship with ideologies. Jpn. J. Psychol. 90, 156–166. doi: 10.4992/jjpsy.90.17234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennebaker, J. W., Francis, M. E., and Booth, R. J. (2001). Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count: LIWC 2001. Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 71.

Google Scholar

Piazza, J., Sousa, P., Rottman, J., and Syropoulos, S. (2018). Which appraisals are foundational to moral judgment? Harm, injustice, and beyond. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 10, 1–11. doi: 10.4324/9781315146911-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qiu, L., Lin, H., Ramsay, J., and Yang, F. (2012). You are what you tweet: personality expression and perception on Twitter. J. Res. Pers. 46, 710–718. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.08.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saucier, G. (2018). Culture, morality and individual differences: comparability and incomparability across species. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 373, 1–9.

Google Scholar

Schmitt, M. J., Neumann, R., and Montada, L. (1995). Dispositional sensitivity to befallen injustice. Soc. Justice Res. 8, 385–407. doi: 10.1007/bf02334713

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skitka, L. J., and Mullen, E. (2002). The dark side of moral conviction. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 2, 35–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-2415.2002.00024.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stolerman, D., and Lagnado, D. (2019). The moral foundations of human rights attitudes. Polit. Psychol. 41, 439–459. doi: 10.1111/pops.12539

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sylwester, K., and Purver, M. (2015). Twitter language use reflects psychological differences between democrats and republicans. PLoS One 10:e0137422. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takamatsu, R., and Takai, J. (2017). Validation of the Japanese version of moral expansiveness scale. Jpn. J. Interpers. Soc. Psychol. 17, 93–102.

Google Scholar

Takikawa, H., and Sakamoto, T. (2019). The moral–emotional foundations of political discourse: a comparative analysis of the speech records of the US and the Japanese legislatures. Qual. Quant. 54, 547–566. doi: 10.1007/s11135-019-00912-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tosi, J., and Warmke, B. (2016). Moral grandstanding. Philos. Public Aff. 44, 197–217. doi: 10.1111/papa.12075

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasquez, K., Keltner, D., Ebenbach, D. H., and Banaszynski, T. L. (2001). Cultural variation and similarity in moral rhetorics: voices from the Philippines and the United States. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 32, 93–120. doi: 10.1177/0022022101032001010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vauclair, C.-M., and Fischer, R. (2011). Do cultural values predict individuals’ moral attitudes? A cross-cultural multilevel approach. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 41, 645–657. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.794

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vecina, M. L., and Piñuela, R. (2017). Relationships between ambivalent sexism and the five moral foundations in domestic violence: is it a matter of fairness and authority? J. Psychol. 151, 334–344. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2017.1289145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waytz, A., Dungan, J., and Young, L. (2013). The whistleblower’s dilemma and the fairness–loyalty tradeoff. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 1027–1033. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: moral judgment, SNS, moral foundations dictionary, culture, Japan, Twitter, language, morality

Citation: Matsuo A, Du B and Sasahara K (2021) Appraisal of the Fairness Moral Foundation Predicts the Language Use Involving Moral Issues on Twitter Among Japanese. Front. Psychol. 12:599024. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.599024

Received: 26 August 2020; Accepted: 19 March 2021;
Published: 30 April 2021.

Edited by:

Ana-Maria Bliuc, University of Dundee, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

S. Adil Saribay, Kadir Has University, Turkey
Damien L. Crone, University of Pennsylvania, United States

Copyright © 2021 Matsuo, Du and Sasahara. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Akiko Matsuo, matsuo-a@tokaigakuen-u.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.