Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 11 August 2021
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Psychological Process of Stereotyping: Content, Forming, Internalizing, Mechanisms, Effects, and Interventions View all 14 articles

Coronavirus Disease 2019–Related Stigma in China: A Descriptive Study

\nLi ZhaoLi Zhao1Zhen Wang
Zhen Wang1*Jian GuanJian Guan1Panyan ShenPanyan Shen2Wen ZhaoWen Zhao2Guoguo ZuoGuoguo Zuo2
  • 1Department of Social Psychology, Zhou Enlai School of Government, Nankai University, Tianjin, China
  • 2Department of Applied Psychology, School of Law, Southwest University of Science and Technology, Sichuan, China

Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) tremendously impacts the physical and mental health of humans worldwide. Consequently, studies on COVID-19 remain extensive. However, most of them were mainly focused on the pathological mechanisms and treatment methods from medical perspectives. Various reports have indicated that COVID-19 is closely related to stigma and discrimination, but little statistical information has been integrated quantitatively to describe the situation in China. Thus, this study investigated the COVID-19-related stigma of individuals. We collected the online survey data from 1,920 Chinese participants from October to December 2020. Findings showed that 306 (15.94%), 285 (14.84%), 265 (13.80%), and 100 (5.21%) participants endorsed stigma toward individuals in high-risk areas, recovered patients with COVID-19, families of recovered patients with COVID-19, and frontline healthcare providers, respectively. To understand the possible factors that could impact the COVID-19-related stigma, knowledge about COVID-19 was investigated. Generally, knowledge about COVID-19 was negatively associated with COVID-19-related stigma in general, while no significant relationship existed between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the COVID-19-related stigma in the groups who had held COVID-19-related stigma. Ultimately, individuals showed COVID-19-related stigma toward recovered patients and their families, individuals in high-risk areas, and frontline healthcare providers to some extent. The results of this study can provide reference to nations, governments, and organizations in addressing the stigma issues raised by the COVID-19 pandemic.

Introduction

Historically, infectious diseases posed a huge threat to the lives of people (Ackerman et al., 2018). Even at present, deaths from infectious diseases account for roughly one-quarter worldwide (World Health Organization, 2015). To mitigate the threat caused by various diseases, people have evolved the behavior immune system (BIS) (Schaller and Park, 2011; Taylor, 2019). The activation of the BIS has implications for the affective, cognitive, and behavioral responses of people at individual and group levels, such as devoting more visual attention to cues related to diseases (Ackerman et al., 2009; Stone and Potton, 2019), increasing intentions to use condoms in sexual attitudes (Tybur et al., 2011), producing more prejudices against out-groups (Kusche and Barker, 2019), and adopting more socially conservative values (Tybur et al., 2016).

Among these studies, one of the most intriguing and well-studied results of the BIS may be the stigma toward individuals possessing cues related to infectious disease (Ackerman et al., 2018). The BIS has implications for prejudices against individuals who have diseases, such as HIV/AIDS, severe acute respiratory syndrome (SARS), and influenza A virus (H1N1) (Kurzban and Leary, 2001; Oaten et al., 2011; Murray and Schaller, 2016). Specifically, due to the BIS, individuals may—in facing cues related to diseases—trigger disgust, worry, and anger in affective responses, vulnerability beliefs and group stereotypes in cognitive responses, and avoidance and protection in behavioral responses (Ackerman et al., 2018). Therefore, coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19), as a novel infectious disease, may be stigmatized due to the protective function of psychological mechanisms of the BIS. Thus, in this study, whether there exists COVID-19-related stigma in China was investigated.

Stigma

Stigma refers to a devalued social identity, which is associated with attributes and characteristics of stereotype, prejudice, and discrimination (Crocker et al., 1998). Stigma significantly and negatively affects individuals by decreasing self-esteem, interfering with family relationships, and limiting opportunities in the job market (Zolezzi et al., 2018). Therefore, a growing number of researchers focus on the stigma, such as weight stigma (Papadopoulos and Brennan, 2015; Hackman et al., 2016), homosexuality stigma (Preciado et al., 2013; Lelutiu-Weinberger et al., 2019), and illness stigma (Casados, 2017; Norman et al., 2017; Kosyluk et al., 2018; Caqueo-Urízar et al., 2020).

Among various types of stigmas, illness stigma can be divided into two main types. One is the mental illness stigma, such as schizophrenia, depressive, and anxiety disorders. For example, Wang et al. (2012) used an implicit association test and found that individuals tended to associate mental illness-related words (e.g., “depressive disorder”) with negative words (e.g., “dangerous,” “negative,” “evil”) on cognition, emotion, and behavioral tendency, which implied that individuals had an implicit stigma to mental illness. Additionally, studies on explicit stigma to mental illness also found that individuals held stigmatizing attitudes toward mental illness (Peris et al., 2008; Sandhu et al., 2019). Another type of illness stigma is the psychical illness stigma, particularly related to infectious diseases, such as HIV/AIDS, SARS, and TB (Mak et al., 2006; Williams et al., 2011; Wagner et al., 2017). For example, individuals who suffered from or were suspected of having HIV/AIDS had experienced various discriminations induced by stigma, such as disrespect, rejection, and being ignored by families, friends, or strangers (Crandall and Coleman, 1992; Herek, 1999). In 2003, SARS had spread to over 29 countries globally, and it was also associated with various forms of stigma, such as being shunned, insulted, marginalized, and excluded from society (Lee et al., 2005).

Coronavirus Disease 2019–Related Stigma

In recent decades, an increasing body of evidence has indicated that infectious diseases related to stigma have caused many serious social problems, with the emergence of a growing number of novel infectious diseases worldwide (Williams et al., 2011; Dubey et al., 2020). COVID-19 is a novel infectious disease that broke out in Wuhan, China in December 2019. Since then, COVID-19 swept all provinces of China and spread rapidly across the globe. According to the WHO, as of May 27, 2021, there have been 108,361 confirmed cases, with 4,881 deaths, in China. Even worse, 167,492,769 confirmed cases with 3,482,907 deaths have been reported globally (World Health Organization, 2021). To decelerate the spread of COVID-19, in addition to health organizations and governments recommending protective measures, the BIS of individuals triggers their various affective, cognitive, and behavioral responses to protect themselves (Makhanova and Shepherd, 2020). For example, that diseases related to disgust and avoidance behavior were positively associated with COVID-19 concern, which means that the more people perceived COVID-19 serious, the greater disgust they might feel toward COVID-19 and its related cues, the greater avoidance of touching others (Makhanova and Shepherd, 2020; Shook et al., 2020). These negative effects and behaviors conformed to the manifestations of stigma (Link and Phelan, 2001). In this case, as one of the most infectious diseases in history (Upadhyay et al., 2020), we assumed that COVID-19-related stigma exists.

However, as far as we know, few studies have investigated the COVID-19-related stigma based on an empirical study. Those few theoretical studies reported that many individuals, such as frontline healthcare providers and individuals who were living in high-risk zones, might experience negative attitudes caused by COVID-19 (Adja et al., 2020; Singh and Subedi, 2020). Additionally, these individuals suffered different kinds of discrimination, such as isolation, refusal of service, harassment, and bullying (Turner-Musa et al., 2020). Given these negative consequences caused by COVID-19-related stigma, individuals who have or are suspected of COVID-19 infection tend to lag in seeking medical care or even hide their illness, which will threaten the safety of others and increase the difficulty in containing the epidemic (Dubey et al., 2020). Therefore, this study aims to investigate whether and to what extent the COVID-19-related stigma exists in China to provide evidence for the interventions of COVID-19-related stigma and further help control the spread.

We hypothesized that COVID-19-related stigma exists in China. Specifically, individuals endorse stigma toward recovered patients with COVID-19, their families, friends, neighbors, and frontline healthcare providers, and individuals in high-risk areas (H1). Moreover, stigma related to infectious diseases was negatively associated with infectious diseases-related knowledge (Balfour et al., 2010; Farotimi et al., 2015; Fischer et al., 2019). Therefore, a negative relationship between the COVID-19-related stigma and the knowledge about COVID-19 exists (H2).

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedures

The Chinese professional survey website Wenjuanxing (www.wjx.cn, which is similar to SurveyMonkey) was used to collect the data. In this study, a survey, which contained the questionnaire (see details in the “Measures” section), was built on Wenjuanxing and a link was created for it. Then, the link to the survey, accompanied by a brief introduction, was distributed to three participant pools via two social network sites, namely, WeChat and QQ (which are similar to Twitter). If individuals were interested in the survey, they could participate through the Wenjuanxing link and help us share the link and introduction of the survey with others if they decide to. Given that this study aimed to describe the situation of COVID-19-related stigma in China, we surveyed as many participants as possible to collect the data. A total of 2,239 participants from 26 provinces in China participated in the survey. The exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) under 16 years of age; (2) response time was <3 min; (3) there were missing data; and (4) response regularly, which means participants made the same choice for all items. Resultantly, 319 questionnaires were removed, leaving 1,920 questionnaires in the final analyses. The mean age of the sample was 20.51 years (SD = 4.51 years) and ranged from 16 to 54 years. The detailed demographic characteristics are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the sample (N = 1,920).

Measures

The questionnaire used in this study involved three sections:

Demographic Information

Participants were asked to fill in the information regarding their age, gender, hometown province, education level, and subjective social class. The subjective social class was measured using the MacArthur Scale of Subjective Socioeconomic Status (Adler et al., 2000). This scale is a ladder that has 10 rungs. Each rung represents a different level of social class. Participants were told that the bottom of the ladder represents the lowest social class, which means their income, degree of education, and occupation are at the lowest level, and the top of the ladder represents the highest social class, which means their income, degree of education, and occupation are at the highest level. Based on their true social classes, participants were asked to indicate which level of the ladder they belong to.

Coronavirus Disease 2019–Related Stigma

To measure the COVID-19-related stigma, a one-dimensional stigma scale was adopted following the study of Mak et al. (2006). It contains 14 items to assess the COVID-19-related stigma of participants from the following three aspects: affective response (5 items, e.g., “recovered COVID-19 patients are a nuisance” if it is to report the impression to recovered patients with COVID-19. The following examples of items used the same group.), cognitive response (3 items, e.g., “It is only normal that recovered COVID-19 patients are being discriminated against by other people”), and behavioral intention (6 items, e.g., “I will try to keep my distance with recovered COVID-19 patients as much as possible”). Specifically, there were 7 sub-questions corresponding to the 7 target groups under each item in the survey. Moreover, each sub-question had a 6-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). For example, one of the items in the survey is that “I will try to keep my distance with the following groups as much as possible.” Under this item, participants reported their impression toward each target group (7 times in total) on the 6-point scale. After the participants finished the survey, the stigma of participants toward each target group was calculated separately based on the calculation used by Mak et al. (2006). Specifically, after reversing appropriate items (i.e., items 6, 9, 10, 13, and 14), the stigma scores were defined as the mean score of all 14 items, and a higher score represented a higher level of stigma related to COVID-19.

Ultimately, not only infected individuals, but also their families, friends, and neighbors, frontline healthcare providers, and individuals in high-risk areas were vulnerable to stigma (Adja et al., 2020; Singh and Subedi, 2020). To investigate the COVID-19-related stigma, stigma toward these six groups was measured in this study. Additionally, stigma toward individuals who were not related to COVID-19 was measured as the control group. In this study, individuals not related to COVID-19 refer to healthy individuals who were not infected with COVID-19 and were not families, friends, or neighbors of patients with COVID-19. Therefore, a total of seven groups were investigated in this study. In the study of Mak et al. (2006), the values of Cronbach's α of the stigma scale were 0.85, 0.81, and 0.83 for HIV/AIDS, SARS, and TB, respectively. In this study, the internal consistency coefficient of the whole scale was good (Cronbach's α = 0.96). Specifically, for the recovered patients with COVID-19, families of recovered patients with COVID-19, neighbors of recovered patients with COVID-19, friends of recovered patients with COVID-19, frontline healthcare providers, individuals in high-risk areas, and individuals not related to COVID-19, the values of Cronbach's α were 0.80, 0.79, 0.80, 0.80, 0.77, 0.78, and 0.76, respectively.

Knowledge About COVID-19

Two items scored on a 10-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly unfamiliar) to 10 (strongly familiar) were adopted from the study of Khasawneh et al. (2020) to assess the degree of the knowledge of participants about COVID-19 (i.e., “the knowledge of potential sources of transmission of COVID-19” and “the knowledge of potential risk factors and virulence of COVID-19”).

Results

Descriptive Results of the COVID-19-Related Stigma Scores

The descriptive statistics on stigma toward COVID-19, such as recovered patients with COVID-19, families of recovered patients with COVID-19, neighbors of recovered patients with COVID-19, friends of recovered patients with COVID-19, frontline healthcare providers, individuals in high-risk areas, and individuals not related to COVID-19, are shown in Table 2. The same criterion (i.e., score > 3), as used in the study of Mak et al. (2006), was adopted to indicate that participants endorsed stigmatizing perceptions toward target groups. Although the average stigma scores of participants for these seven groups were all <3 in this study, 306 (15.94%), 285 (14.84%), and 265 (13.80%) participants endorsed stigma (score > 3) toward individuals in high-risk areas, recovered patients with COVID-19, and families of recovered patients with COVID-19, respectively. Additionally, only 120 (6.25%) and 100 (5.21%) participants had stigmatizing perceptions toward individuals not related to COVID-19 and frontline healthcare providers, respectively.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive results of the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19)-related stigma scores (N = 1,920).

Correlations Among COVID-19-Related Stigma Scores

The correlations among COVID-19-related stigmas to the seven groups are presented in Table 3. The stigma scores of these seven groups, such as recovered patients with COVID-19, families of recovered patients with COVID-19, neighbors of recovered patients with COVID-19, friends of recovered patients with COVID-19, frontline healthcare providers, individuals in high-risk areas, and individuals not related to COVID-19, had significantly positive relationships with one another (r ranges from 0.65 to 0.99).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlations between different types of COVID-19-related stigma (N = 1,920).

Knowledge and COVID-19-Related Stigma

The descriptive analysis of the knowledge of participants about (i.e., sources and risk factors) COVID-19 showed that the level of knowledge among participants toward possible sources of COVID-19 transmission (M = 6.79, SD = 1.73) and potential risk factors for COVID-19 infection (M = 6.91, SD = 1.78) was not high. The distributions of the knowledge about COVID-19 transmission and infection are shown in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Descriptive results of the knowledge about COVID-19 (N = 1,920).

The correlations between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the COVID-19-related stigma are presented in Table 5. Based on the whole dataset, the possible sources of COVID-19 transmission and the possible risk factors for COVID-19 infection were negatively associated with the stigma toward COVID-19, such as recovered patients with COVID-19, families of recovered patients with COVID-19, neighbors of recovered patients with COVID-19, friends of recovered patients with COVID-19, frontline healthcare providers, individuals in high-risk areas, and individuals not related to COVID-19 (r ranges from −0.12 to −0.15).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Correlations between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the COVID-19-related stigma (N = 1,920).

The average stigma scores of participants for these seven groups were all <3 in this study, which implied that participants did not endorse the COVID-19-related stigma from the perspective of the whole data. Therefore, to identify the possible sources of and factors that impact stigma, a series of new datasets was composed based on the original dataset when a stigma score of the participant was >3. The specific sample size for each group is shown in Table 6.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Correlations between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the COVID-19-related stigma (stigma score > 3).

Based on the series of new datasets, the correlations between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the COVID-19-related stigma are presented in Table 6. The possible sources of COVID-19 transmission and the possible risk factors for COVID-19 infection were not significantly associated with the COVID-19-related stigma.

Given that the results of the whole dataset of stigma differed from those of the data when the stigma scores were >3, the correlations between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the dataset when the stigma scores were ≤3 were analyzed (Table 7). The possible sources of COVID-19 transmission and the possible risk factors for COVID-19 infection were all negatively associated with the COVID-19-related stigma, such as recovered patients with COVID-19, families of recovered patients with COVID-19, neighbors of recovered patients with COVID-19, friends of recovered patients with COVID-19, frontline healthcare providers, individuals in high-risk areas, and individuals not related to COVID-19 (r ranges from −0.12 to −0.15).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Correlations between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the COVID-19-related stigma (stigma score ≤ 3).

Differences of COVID-19-Related Stigma on Demographic Variables

Based on the series of new datasets, the results of analyses on the differences of COVID-19-related stigma on demographic variables are as follows (Table 8): no significant differences existed between men and women on the COVID-19-related stigma, except that men (M = 3.43, SD = 0.34) endorsed a significantly higher level of stigma toward neighbors of recovered patients with COVID-19 than women (M = 3.35, SD = 0.27), t(244) = 2.19, p = 0.03, Cohen's d (i.e., the effect size) = 0.26, which was small based on the Cohen's conventions (Cohen, 1988). No significant differences existed among different ages on the COVID-19-related stigma. No significant differences existed among different education levels on the COVID-19-related stigma, except that significant differences existed among different educations on stigma toward recovered patients with COVID-19, F(3,281) = 4.82, p = 0.003, partial η2 (i.e., the effect size) = 0.05, which was small. The post hoc (Bonferroni) tests revealed that participants with the education level of master's degree and above (M = 3.59, SD = 0.46) endorsed a higher degree of stigma toward recovered patients with COVID-19 than undergraduate participants (M = 3.37, SD = 0.30), p = 0.008. No significant differences existed among different subjective social classes on the COVID-19-related stigma, except that there were significant differences among different subjective social classes on stigma toward neighbors of recovered patients with COVID-19, F(2,243) = 4.79, p = 0.009, partial η2 = 0.04, which was small. The post hoc (Bonferroni) tests revealed that participants who reported that their subjective social classes were 8–10 (M = 3.56, SD = 0.30) endorsed a higher level of stigma toward neighbors of recovered patients with COVID-19 than participants who reported that their subjective social classes were 4–7 (M = 3.36, SD = 0.29), p = 0.046.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. The differences between main variables regarding different individuals-related stigma toward COVID-19.

Discussion

General Description of COVID-19-Related Stigma in China

Coronavirus disease 2019 has jeopardized human lives and societies worldwide. Resultantly, many researchers had conducted medical studies on COVID-19 (Ahn et al., 2020; Chakraborty et al., 2020; Landete et al., 2020; Wan et al., 2020). However, few researchers have conducted empirical studies regarding the COVID-19-related stigma, while various media reports suggested that stigma and discrimination cases related to COVID-19 were common globally, such as America, Nepal, Jordan, India, Italy, and China (Aacharya and Shah, 2020; Chopra and Arora, 2020; Khasawneh et al., 2020; Sahoo et al., 2020; Singh and Subedi, 2020; Turner-Musa et al., 2020). Thus, this study quantified the COVID-19-related stigma in China.

Fundamentally, H1 was supported. It was found that the prevalence of COVID-19-related stigma was low in China (14.84% of participants endorsed stigma toward recovered patients with COVID-19). One of the possible reasons is that the stigma belongs to a social issue, which means that the responses of participants in the questionnaires might be affected by the social desirability effect and concealed their true attitudes toward groups related to stigma. Thus, they might be particularly cautious when they chose options with scores >3 on the stigma scale, indicating that the sample of the stigma data >3 was small. However, the results in this study were worse than on SARS- and TB-related stigma in previous studies (Link and Phelan, 2001; Mak et al., 2006), which indicated that the percentage of participants who endorsed stigmatizing perceptions toward SARS and TB were 3.7 and 4.9%, respectively. This inconsistency might be because, during the COVID-19 outbreak, the regional lockdown programs had been enforced in China to block the possible chains of transmission wherein people were stayed at home to avoid contact with others, which conformed to the behavioral response of stigma (Mak et al., 2006). Moreover, this study showed that participants endorsed similar stigma toward families (13.80%), neighbors (12.81%), and friends (12.40%) of recovered patients with COVID-19 to participants endorsed stigma toward recovered patients. Furthermore, the distributions of COVID-19-related stigma toward recovered patients, and their families, neighbors, friends, and individuals in high-risk areas, had similar structures. The distributions of COVID-19-related stigma to frontline healthcare providers and individuals not related to COVID-19 were similar and had different types of structures. As expected, based on the BIS, one of its characteristics is overgeneralization, which indicates that the BIS has the tendency to be oversensitive or overgeneralized to cues related to diseases, even in cases where disease threats are absent (Ackerman et al., 2018). Just as the “smoke detector principle” (Nesse, 2005), all of the infectious or non-infectious psychical and mental abnormalities are regarded by the BIS as dangerous signals (Ackerman et al., 2018). Thus, although families, neighbors, and friends of recovered COVID-19 patients are not infected with COVID-19, the magnitude of the correlations between recovered patients and their families, neighbors, and friends is strong (r ranges from 0.92 to 0.99), which implied that they might have a higher probability of infection than others who are not related to recovered patients, and then the BIS may trigger psychological responses related to stigmatization, such as disgust and avoidance. These results uncovered that we should not only pay attention to patients with COVID-19 but also focus on their families, neighbors, and friends, who are likely to be ignored in the future.

This study unveiled that participants also reported a higher level of stigma toward individuals in high-risk areas (15.94%) than recovered patients with COVID-19 (14.84%). In addition to the effects of BIS on stigma as mentioned earlier, another possible reason for this may be that patients with COVID-19 who have recovered in this study were perceived by participants to be safer than the individuals in high-risk areas in which many people may be infected with the virus. Therefore, participants might be more afraid of individuals in high-risk areas than recovered patients with COVID-19 and be more likely to avoid them, which might eventually lead to greater stigma scores for individuals in high-risk areas. Additionally, during the COVID-19 pandemic, various media have reported incidents of the stigmatization of frontline healthcare providers worldwide, such as Mexico, Malawi, India, and the United States (Bagcchi, 2020; Grover et al., 2020). In this study, only 100 (5.21%) participants had stigmatizing perceptions of frontline healthcare providers. The reason may be that the traditional Chinese culture emphasizes “do not forget what other people have done for you” (Xu et al., 2018). With that mindset, frontline healthcare providers volunteered and exerted their best to provide healthcare for patients in China (Liu et al., 2020), influencing most Chinese to be grateful for them, instead of stigmatizing and discriminating against them. Future studies should investigate gratefulness (as well as stigma) in order to provide a fuller picture of the perception of healthcare providers. Additionally, future studies should investigate the stigma of individuals toward frontline healthcare providers in other cultural contexts.

To identify the possible factors that impact the COVID-19-related stigma, this study explored the relationship between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the COVID-19-related stigma. H2 was partially supported. The results showed that knowledge was negatively related to COVID-19-related stigma based on the whole dataset. Specifically, the less knowledge about COVID-19 participants had, the more COVID-19-related stigma they endorsed. However, after removing the data of stigma scores that were ≤3, this study found that no significant association existed between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the COVID-19-related stigma, which was inconsistent with previous studies (Svensson and Hansson, 2016; Lopez et al., 2018). Based on previous studies, a significantly negative relationship existed between knowledge and stigma. Given that the results of the whole stigma data differed from those of the stigma data >3, we further analyzed the relationship between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the stigma data ≤3. The results revealed that a significantly negative relationship existed between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the COVID-19-related stigma, which implied that the more knowledge about COVID-19 participants had, the less degree of stigmatization toward COVID-19 they endorsed. In summary, there might be a significantly negative relationship between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the COVID-19-related stigma, and the lack of significant findings on the subset of the data when the stigma scores were larger than 3 may be due to the limited range of scores. However, future studies should be cautious when using this conclusion.

Finally, this study explored whether demographic differences exist in COVID-19-related stigma. The results showed that gender, age, education level, and social class minimally affected the COVID-19-related stigma of individuals. This study found that participants with master's degree or higher education level endorsed higher degrees of stigma toward recovered patients with COVID-19 than undergraduate participants. In addition, participants who reported that their subjective social classes were 8–10 endorsed the higher levels of stigma toward neighbors of recovered patients with COVID-19 than participants who reported that their subjective social classes were 4–7. A higher subjective social class represents individuals who have higher incomes, degrees of education, and occupations. Thus, the two findings implied that people who have a higher level of education may hold a higher level of COVID-19-related stigma, which was inconsistent with the study of Johnco and Rapee (2018), which found that a negative association existed between education and stigma toward patients with diseases. Based on the abovementioned findings, given that a significantly negative relationship existed between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the degree of the COVID-19-related stigma, individuals who have a higher level of education might gain more knowledge about COVID-19 and should have endorsed less COVID-19-related stigma. Thus, we failed to explain the reasons for the results in this study. However, as Williams et al. (2011) mentioned, “disease-related stigma persists in spite of education and often without rationale” (p. 68), the relationship between disease-related stigma and education requires further investigation.

Limitations

This study holds certain limitations. First, based on previous studies on stigma toward mental illness (Michaels and Corrigan, 2013; Latkin et al., 2017), the responses of participants in this study might be affected by the social desirability effect given that this study used questionnaires, which might influence the accuracy of the results. Therefore, to reduce the social desirability effect, the COVID-19-related stigma of participants should be investigated using indirect measures, such as implicit association tests (Greenwald et al., 1998). Second, 120 (6.25%) and 100 (5.21%) participants had stigmatizing perceptions toward individuals not related to COVID-19 and frontline healthcare providers, respectively. The results show the complexity of studying stigma. People somewhat have stigmas toward others. Thus, the values of 6.25 and 5.21% can be used as a baseline to evaluate the situations of COVID-19-related stigma in China. Future studies should pay attention to this baseline. Third, this study used the convenience sampling method, instead of the random sampling method. Most participants were university students, which leads to the uneven distribution of the sample by age and education. Therefore, the generalizability of the results may be limited, and the reliability of the results may be influenced. Future studies are suggested to investigate the COVID-19-related stigma based on children and elder individuals. Fourth, the recovered patients with COVID-19 and their families, friends, and neighbors were studied in this study. Moreover, whether the patients with COVID-19 undergoing rehabilitation and their families, friends, and neighbors experience more stigma should be studied given that they may be considered more dangerous than recovered patients with COVID-19 and their families, friends, and neighbors by other individuals. Furthermore, only two self-assessment questions were used to assess the degree of the knowledge of participants about COVID-19, which may not fully reflect the knowledge of participants about COVID-19 and may further cause the mixed results of the correlation between the knowledge about COVID-19 and its related stigma. Although there were studies measuring the knowledge of participants about medical health using the same two questions (e.g., Sørensen et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2018), future studies are suggested to use more questions and objective indicators to assess the knowledge of people about COVID-19. For example, the questions can be requiring participants to assess their knowledge about dissemination channels of transmission of COVID-19, such as from the air, animal, contaminated food, skin contact, and blood transfusion (Khasawneh et al., 2020). Finally, this study tested only the role of knowledge, but more potentially relevant individual characteristics exist that may impact COVID-19-related stigma in the pandemic. For example, Xu and Cheng (2021) indicated that political ideology, self-control, need for cognition, and risk attitude (health/safety risk-averse) were correlated with the attitudes of individuals toward COVID-19. Additionally, a core cognitive function, namely, working memory, was explored, and the working memory capacity predicted individual differences in social distancing compliance during the COVID-19 pandemic (Xie et al., 2020). Particularly, the higher working memory capacity that participants had, the more social distancing compliance they showed. Thus, these factors should be explored in the future.

Coronavirus disease 2019–related stigma has posed a serious threat to the psychical and mental health of individuals and the whole society. Therefore, we hope that the government, researchers, and general population pay attention to the treatment of COVID-19 itself while focusing on the negative social issues induced by the COVID-19-related stigma, especially its preventions and interventions (Stangl et al., 2019). For example, given that the COVID-19-related stigma is attributable to the unscientific belief and improper understanding of individuals (Bagcchi, 2020), it might be helpful for health professionals to recommend reliable and scientific information related to COVID-19 to expand the knowledge of individuals about COVID-19 to reduce or eliminate the COVID-19-related stigma.

Conclusion

This study examined the COVID-19-related stigma in China. Ultimately, not only recovered patients with COVID-19 were susceptible to stigma, but also families, neighbors, friends of recovered patients with COVID-19, frontline healthcare providers, and individuals in high-risk areas. Moreover, there might be a significantly negative relationship between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the COVID-19-related stigma. Specifically, the knowledge about COVID-19 was negatively associated with the COVID-19-related stigma. A higher level of knowledge about COVID-19 might lead to a lower level of COVID-19-related stigma, although no significant relationship existed between the knowledge about COVID-19 and the COVID-19-related stigma in the groups who have held COVID-19-related stigma.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent from the participants' legal guardian/next of kin was not required to participate in this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

LZ, ZW, and JG: conception and design. PS and ZW: administrative support. LZ, WZ, and PS: provision of study materials. ZW, LZ, GZ, and WZ: collection and assembly of data. LZ, ZW, and JG: data analysis and interpretation. All authors: manuscript writing and final approval of manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported partially by the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (63202002), the Key Projects of Philosophy and Social Sciences Research, Ministry of Education (17JZD043), and the Funds of Philosophy and Social Sciences Research of Nankai University (ZB21BZ0220). The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analyses, the decision to publish, or the preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.694988/full#supplementary-material

References

Aacharya, R. P., and Shah, A. (2020). Ethical dimensions of stigma and discrimination in Nepal during COVID-19 pandemic. Ethics Med. Public Health 14:100536. doi: 10.1016/j.jemep.2020.100536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ackerman, J. M., Becker, D. V., Mortensen, C. R., Sasaki, T., Neuberg, S. L., and Kenrick, D. T. (2009). A pox on the mind: disjunction of attention and memory in the processing of physical disfigurement. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 478–485. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ackerman, J. M., Hill, S. E., and Murray, D. R. (2018). The behavioral immune system: current concerns and future directions. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass. 12:e12371. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12371

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adja, K. Y. C., Golinelli, D., Lenzi, J., Fantini, M. P., and Wu, E. (2020). Pandemics and social stigma: who's next? Italy's experience with COVID-19. Public Health 185, 39–41. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2020.05.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adler, N. E., Epel, E. S., Castellazzo, G., and Ickovics, J. R. (2000). Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychol. 19, 586–592. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahn, D. G., Shin, H. J., Kim, M. H., Lee, S., Kim, H. S., Myoung, J., et al. (2020). Current status of epidemiology, diagnosis, therapeutics, and vaccines for novel coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19). J. Microbiol. Biotechnol. 30, 313–324. doi: 10.4014/jmb.2003.03011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagcchi, S. (2020). Stigma during the COVID-19 pandemic. Lancet Infect. Dis. 20:782. doi: 10.1016/S1473-3099(20)30498-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balfour, L., Corace, K., Tasca, G. A., Best-Plummer, W., MacPherson, P. A., and Cameron, D. W. (2010). High HIV knowledge relates to low stigma in pharmacists and university health science students in Guyana, South America. Int. J. Infect. Dis. 14, e881–e887. doi: 10.1016/j.ijid.2010.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caqueo-Urízar, A., Urzúa, A., Habib, J., Loundou, A., Boucekine, M., Boyer, L., et al. (2020). Relationships between social stigma, stigma experience and self-stigma and impaired quality of life in schizophrenia across three Latin-American countries. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 270, 513–520. doi: 10.1007/s00406-019-01035-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casados, A. T. (2017). Reducing the stigma of mental illness: current approaches and future directions. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 24, 306–323. doi: 10.1111/cpsp.12206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chakraborty, C., Sharma, A. R., Sharma, G., Bhattacharya, M., and Lee, S. S. (2020). SARS-CoV-2 causing pneumonia-associated respiratory disorder (COVID-19): diagnostic and proposed therapeutic options. Eur. Rev. Med. Pharmacol. Sci. 24, 4016–4026. doi: 10.26355/eurrev_202004_20871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chopra, K. K., and Arora, V. K. (2020). COVID-19 and social stigma: role of scientific community. Indian J. Tuberc. 67, 284–285. doi: 10.1016/j.ijtb.2020.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.

Crandall, C. S., and Coleman, R. (1992). AIDS-related stigmatization and the disruption of social relationships. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 9, 163–177. doi: 10.1177/0265407592092001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crocker, J., Major, B., and Steele, C. (1998). “Social stigma,” in The Handbook of Social Psychology, Vol. 2, 4th ed, eds D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, and G. Lindzey (New York, NY: Academic Press), 504–553.

Dubey, S., Biswas, P., Ghosh, R., Chatterjee, S., Dubey, M. J., Chatterjee, S., et al. (2020). Psychosocial impact of COVID-19. Diabetes Metab. Syndr. Clin. Res. Rev. 14, 779–788. doi: 10.1016/j.dsx.2020.05.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farotimi, A. A., Nwozichi, C. U., and Ojediran, T. D. (2015). Knowledge, attitude, and practice of HIV/AIDS-related stigma and discrimination reduction among nursing students in southwest Nigeria. Iran. J. Nurs. Midwifery Res. 20, 705–711. doi: 10.4103/1735-9066.170011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, L. S., Mansergh, G., Lynch, J., and Santibanez, S. (2019). Addressing disease-related stigma during infectious disease outbreaks. Disaster Med. Public Health Prepared. 13, 989–994. doi: 10.1017/dmp.2018.157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., Mcghee, D. E., and Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1464–1480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grover, S., Singh, P., Sahoo, S., and Mehra, A. (2020). Stigma related to COVID-19 infection: are the Health Care Workers stigmatizing their own colleagues? Asian J. Psychiatr. 53:102381. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackman, J., Maupin, J., and Brewis, A. A. (2016). Weight-related stigma is a significant psychosocial stressor in developing countries: evidence from Guatemala. Soc. Sci. Med. 161, 55–60. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.05.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herek, G. M. (1999). AIDS and stigma. Am. Behav. Sci. 42, 1106–1116. doi: 10.1177/0002764299042007004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnco, C., and Rapee, R. M. (2018). Depression literacy and stigma influence how parents perceive and respond to adolescent depressive symptoms. J. Affect. Disord. 241, 599–607. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.08.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khasawneh, A. I., Humeidan, A. A., Alsulaiman, J. W., Bloukh, S., Ramadan, M., Al-Shatanawi, T. N., et al. (2020). Medical Students and COVID-19: knowledge, attitudes, and precautionary measures. A descriptive study from Jordan. Front. Public Health 8:253. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.00253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosyluk, K., Marshall, J., Macias, D. R., Andrus, D., Guerra, D., Robinson, M., et al. (2018). Examining the impact of this is my brave on mental illness stigma and willingness to seek help: a pilot study. Community Ment. Health J. 54, 276–281. doi: 10.1007/s10597-018-0238-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurzban, R., and Leary, M. R. (2001). Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: the functions of social exclusion. Psychol. Bull. 127, 187–208. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kusche, I., and Barker, J. L. (2019). Pathogens and immigrants: a critical appraisal of the behavioral immune system as an explanation of prejudice against ethnic outgroups. Front. Psychol. 10:2412. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landete, P., Quezada Loaiza, C. A., Aldave-Orzaiz, B., Muñiz, S. H., Maldonado, A., Zamora, E., et al. (2020). Clinical features and radiological manifestations of COVID-19 disease. World J. Radiol. 12, 247–260. doi: 10.4329/wjr.v12.i11.247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latkin, C. A., Edwards, C., Davey-Rothwell, M. A., and Tobin, K. E. (2017). The relationship between social desirability bias and self-reports of health, substance use, and social network factors among urban substance users in Baltimore, Maryland. Addict. Behav. 73, 133–136. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S., Chan, L. Y., Chau, A. M., Kwok, K., and Kleinman, A. (2005). The experience of SARS-related stigma at Amoy Gardens. Soc. Sci. Med. 61, 2038–2046. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lelutiu-Weinberger, C., Rendina, H. J., Mirandola, M., Gios, L., Folch, C., Rafila, A., et al. (2019). The role of gay-related stigma in HIV-risk behavior among sexual minority men in Europe. AIDS Behav. 23, 684–694. doi: 10.1007/s10461-018-2306-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Link, B. G., and Phelan, J. C. (2001). Conceptualizing stigma. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 27, 363–385. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.363

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Q., Luo, D., Haase, J. E., Guo, Q., Wang, X. Q., Liu, S., et al. (2020). The experiences of health-care providers during the COVID-19 crisis in China: a qualitative study. Lancet Global Health 8, e790–e798. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(20)30204-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez, V., Sanchez, K., Killian, M. O., and Eghaneyan, B. H. (2018). Depression screening and education: an examination of mental health literacy and stigma in a sample of Hispanic women. BMC Public Health 18:646. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-5516-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mak, W. W. S., Mo, P. K. H., Cheung, R. Y. M., Woo, J., Cheung, F. M., and Lee, D. (2006). Comparative stigma of HIV/AIDS, SARS, and tuberculosis in Hong Kong. Soc. Sci. Med. 63, 1912–1922. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makhanova, A., and Shepherd, M. A. (2020). Behavioral immune system linked to responses to the threat of COVID-19. Pers. Individ. Dif. 167:110221. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michaels, P. J., and Corrigan, P. W. (2013). Measuring mental illness stigma with diminished social desirability effects. J. Mental Health 22, 218–226. doi: 10.3109/09638237.2012.734652

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, D. R., and Schaller, M. (2016). “The behavioral immune system: implications for social cognition, social interaction, and social influence,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 53, eds J. M. Olson and M. P. Zanna (Elsevier), 75–129.

Nesse, R. M. (2005). Natural selection and the regulation of defenses: a signal detection analysis of the smoke detector principle. Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 88–105. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman, R. M. G., Li, Y., Sorrentino, R., Hampson, E., and Ye, Y. (2017). The differential effects of a focus on symptoms versus recovery in reducing stigma of schizophrenia. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 52, 1385–1394. doi: 10.1007/s00127-017-1429-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., and Case, T. I. (2011). Disease avoidance as a functional basis for stigmatization. Philos. Transac. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 366, 3433–3452. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papadopoulos, S., and Brennan, L. (2015). Correlates of weight stigma in adults with overweight and obesity: a systematic literature review. Obesity 23, 1743–1760. doi: 10.1002/oby.21187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peris, T. S., Teachman, B. A., and Nosek, B. A. (2008). Implicit and explicit stigma of mental illness: links to clinical care. J. Nerv. Mental Dis. 196, 752–760. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181879dfd

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preciado, M. A., Johnson, K. L., and Peplau, L. A. (2013). The impact of cues of stigma and support on self-perceived sexual orientation among heterosexually identified men and women. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 477–485. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahoo, S., Mehra, A., Suri, V., Malhotra, P., Yaddanapudi, L. N., Puri, G. D., et al. (2020). Lived experiences of the corona survivors (patients admitted in COVID wards): a narrative real-life documented summaries of internalized guilt, shame, stigma, anger. Asian J. Psychiatry 53:102187. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102187

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandhu, H. S., Arora, A., Brasch, J., and Streiner, D. L. (2019). Mental health stigma: explicit and implicit attitudes of Canadian undergraduate students, medical school students, and psychiatrists. Can. J. Psychiatry 64, 209–217. doi: 10.1177/0706743718792193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaller, M., and Park, J. H. (2011). The behavioral immune system (and why it matters). Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 20, 99–103. doi: 10.1177/0963721411402596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shook, N. J., Sevi, B., Lee, J., Oosterhoff, B., and Fitzgerald, H. N. (2020). Disease avoidance in the time of COVID-19: the behavioral immune system is associated with concern and preventative health behaviors. PloS ONE 15:e0238015. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, R., and Subedi, M. (2020). COVID-19 and stigma: social discrimination towards frontline healthcare providers and COVID-19 recovered patients in Nepal. Asian J. Psychiatry 53:102222. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sørensen, K., Van den Broucke, S., Fullam, J., Doyle, G., Pelikan, J., Slonska, Z., et al. (2012). Health literacy and public health: a systematic review and integration of definitions and models. BMC Public Health 12, 1–13. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-80

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stangl, A. L., Earnshaw, V. A., Logie, C. H., van Brakel, W., Simbayi, L. C., Barré, I., et al. (2019). The health stigma and discrimination framework: a global, crosscutting framework to inform research, intervention development, and policy on health-related stigmas. BMC Med. 17, 1–13. doi: 10.1186/s12916-019-1271-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, A., and Potton, A. (2019). Emotional responses to disfigured faces and disgust sensitivity: an eye-tracking study. J. Health Psychol. 24, 1191–1200. doi: 10.1177/1359105317692856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svensson, B., and Hansson, L. (2016). How mental health literacy and experience of mental illness relate to stigmatizing attitudes and social distance towards people with depression or psychosis: a cross-sectional study. Nordic J. Psychiatry 70, 309–313. doi: 10.3109/08039488.2015.1109140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. (2019). The Psychology of Pandemics: Preparing for the Next Global Outbreak of Infectious Disease. Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

Turner-Musa, J., Ajayi, O., and Kemp, L. (2020). Examining social determinants of health, stigma, and COVID-19 disparities. Healthcare 8:168. doi: 10.3390/healthcare8020168

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tybur, J. M., Bryan, A. D., Lieberman, D., Hooper, A. E. C., and Merriman, L. A. (2011). Sex differences and sex similarities in disgust sensitivity. Pers. Individ. Differ. 51, 343–348. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tybur, J. M., Inbar, Y., Aarøe, L., Barclay, P., Barlow, F. K., De Barra, M., et al. (2016). Parasite stress and pathogen avoidance relate to distinct dimensions of political ideology across 30 nations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 113, 12408–12413. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1607398113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Upadhyay, R., Singh, B., and Singh, U. (2020). Psychological impact of quarantine period on asymptomatic individuals with COVID-19. Soc. Sci. Human. Open 2:100061. doi: 10.1016/j.ssaho.2020.100061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, A. C., Girard, T., Mcshane, K. E., Margolese, S., and Hart, T. A. (2017). HIV-related stigma and overlapping stigmas towards people living with HIV among health care trainees in Canada. AIDS Educ. Prev. 29, 364–376. doi: 10.1521/aeap.2017.29.4.364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wan, D. Y., Luo, X. Y., Dong, W., and Zhang, Z. W. (2020). Current practice and potential strategy in diagnosing COVID-19. Eur. Rev. Med. Pharmacol. Sci. 24, 4548–4553. doi: 10.26355/eurrev_202004_21039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Huang, X., Jackson, T., and Chen, R. (2012). Components of implicit stigma against mental illness among Chinese students. PLoS ONE 7:e46016. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Zhou, X., Leesa, L., and Mantwill, S. (2018). The effect of vaccine literacy on parental trust and intention to vaccinate after a major vaccine scandal. J. Health Commun. 23, 413–421. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2018.1455771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. L., Gonzalez-Medina, D., and Le, Q. V. (2011). Infectious diseases and social stigma. Med. Health Sci. J. 4, 58–70. doi: 10.15208/MHSJ.2011.127

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2015). World Health in 2015: From MDGs, Millennium Development Goals to SDGs, Sustainable Development Goals. Geneva.

World Health Organization (2021). WHO Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) Dashboard. Available online at: https://covid19.who.int/table

Xie, W., Campbell, S., and Zhang, W. (2020). Working memory capacity predicts individual differences in social-distancing compliance during the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 117, 17667–17674. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2008868117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, P., and Cheng, J. (2021). Individual differences in social distancing and mask-wearing in the pandemic of COVID-19: the role of need for cognition, self-control and risk attitude. Pers. Individ. Diff. 175:110706. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110706

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Y., Jiao, L., Xu, R., Feng, Q., Wang, F., Jiang, J., et al. (2018). The process and characteristics of psychological kidnapping: an indigenous model of corruption in China. J. Pacific Rim Psychol. 12, 1–12. doi: 10.1017/prp.2017.24

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zolezzi, M., Alamri, M., Shaar, S., and Rainkie, D. (2018). Stigma associated with mental illness and its treatment in the Arab culture: a systematic review. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 64, 597–609. doi: 10.1177/0020764018789200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19, stigma, knowledge, China, descriptive study

Citation: Zhao L, Wang Z, Guan J, Shen P, Zhao W and Zuo G (2021) Coronavirus Disease 2019–Related Stigma in China: A Descriptive Study. Front. Psychol. 12:694988. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.694988

Received: 14 April 2021; Accepted: 09 July 2021;
Published: 11 August 2021.

Edited by:

Baoshan Zhang, Shaanxi Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Jiuqing Cheng, University of Northern Iowa, United States
Delei Zhao, Harbin Engineering University, China
Magdalena Zawisza, Anglia Ruskin University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2021 Zhao, Wang, Guan, Shen, Zhao and Zuo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Zhen Wang, wangzhen6685@163.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.