Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 05 July 2021
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Reading and Writing Skills: Cognitive, Emotional, Creative, and Digital Approaches View all 41 articles

Validation of the Writing Strategies Questionnaire in the Context of Primary Education: A Multidimensional Measurement Model

  • 1Department of Psychology, Sociology and Philosophy, University of Leon, Leon, Spain
  • 2Ponferrada Associated Centre, National University of Distance Education (UNED), Leon, Spain
  • 3Research Institute for Child Development and Education, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands

Research has shown that writers seem to follow different writing strategies to juggle the high cognitive demands of writing. The use of writing strategies seems to be an important cognitive writing-related variable which has an influence on students' writing behavior during writing and, therefore, on the quality of their compositions. Several studies have tried to assess students' writing preferences toward the use of different writing strategies in University or high-school students, while research in primary education is practically non-existent. The present study, therefore, focused on the validation of the Spanish Writing Strategies Questionnaire (WSQ-SP), aimed to measure upper-primary students' preference for the use of different writing strategies, through a multidimensional model. The sample comprised 651 Spanish upper-primary students. Questionnaire data was explored by means of exploratory (EFA) and confirmatory (CFA) factor analysis. Through exploratory factor analysis four factors were identified, labeled thinking, planning, revising, and monitoring, which represent different writing strategies. The confirmatory factor analysis confirmed the adequacy of the four-factor model, with a sustainable model composed of the four factors originally identified. Based on the analysis, the final questionnaire was composed of 16 items. According to the results, the Spanish version of the Writing Strategies Questionnaire (WSQ-SP) for upper-primary students has been shown to be a valid and reliable instrument, which can be easily applied in the educational context to explore upper-primary students' writing strategies.

Introduction

Writing has been defined as a problem-solving task that places multiple cognitive demands on the writer (Hayes, 1996). As Flower and Hayes indicated in the first cognitive model of writing (Flower and Hayes, 1980), writers have to manage several cognitively costly processes such as planning what to say, translating and transcribing those plans into written text, and revising either the plans or the written text (Alamargot and Chanquoy, 2001; Hayes, 2012). The use of these processes, especially in young writers, in whom basic transcription skills are not yet automated (Pontart et al., 2013; Alves et al., 2016; Limpo et al., 2017; Llaurado and Dockrell, 2020), consumes much of the capacity of their working memory as these processes recursively interact during composition (McCutchen, 2011).

Following a comprehensive literature review, Graham and Harris (2000) concluded that writing development seems to depend on the automation of transcription skills and the acquisition of high-levels of self-regulation in order to handle high-level processes such as planning and revision. Self-regulation, represented by the use of writing strategies, is a critical aspect of writing as it enables writers to achieve their writing goals (Zeidner et al., 2000; Santangelo et al., 2016; Puranik et al., 2019). These strategies may reduce cognitive overload as they allow writers to divide, sequence, and regulate the attention paid to the different writing processes (Kieft et al., 2006; Beauvais et al., 2011). Empirical research has shown that writers' strategic behavior during composition strongly predicts the quality of “novices” and “experts” texts (Beauvais et al., 2011; Graham et al., 2017a, 2019; Wijekumar et al., 2019). Accordingly, the use of writing strategies has been generally considered to be a critical individual writing-related variable (Kieft et al., 2008), and is a major focus of research in writing instruction (Harris et al., 2010; Graham and Harris, 2018) from the earliest stages of education (Arrimada et al., 2019). Exploring students' use of different writing strategies during composition seems to be a critical aspect and should be considered in the fields of writing and writing instructional research.

Several studies have attempted to explore how writers differ in the use of different writing strategies (Torrance et al., 1994, 1999, 2000; Biggs et al., 1999; Lavelle et al., 2002; Kieft et al., 2006, 2007, 2008). These studies identified two main writing strategies, related with the processes identified in the first seminal cognitive model of writing (Flower and Hayes, 1980), such as planning and revising. According to these studies, writers who follow a planning strategy tend to plan before beginning to write, whereas writers who prefer the revising strategy tend to plan by writing a rough draft first and then revising it. Despite the high-value of these studies, it is important to note that they only focused on analyzing the writing strategies in undergraduate (Torrance et al., 1994, 1999, 2000; Biggs et al., 1999; Lavelle et al., 2002; Arias-Gundín and Fidalgo, 2017; Robledo Ramón et al., 2018) and secondary-school students (Kieft et al., 2006, 2008). To our knowledge, just one study has explored the use of different writing strategies with upper-primary Flemish students (De Smedt et al., 2018). In this study, the authors implemented the Writing Strategies Questionnaire initially developed by Kieft et al. (2006, 2008) and identified four factors by means of exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis which were labeled thinking, planning, revising and controlling. The planning and revising strategies were consistent with those identified in previous studies with secondary school students (Kieft et al., 2006, 2008). However, in that study the authors found two additional factors. The controlling factor was defined as students' tendency to check the content or structure of their text, whereas the thinking factor make reference to the extent to which students first think about the content of their text and about their writing approach before they start writing. Thus, according to this study, it seems to be that the questionnaire assesses writing strategies in a more comprehensive way than initially intended by Kieft et al. (2006, 2008).

Additionally, it is important to consider that in all the previously reported studies, data were collected independently of the writing task through questionnaires, which may have led to biases due to self-reported estimates of writing strategies (Fidalgo and García, 2009). However, it is difficult to think of a feasible alternative for exploring writing strategies which would allow researchers to collect data from a representative sample size. Therefore, it is vitally important to conduct studies to explore the psychometric properties and the validity of these questionnaires. The advantages of exploring these aspects of the Writing Strategies Questionnaire would be the possibility of capturing students' strategy preferences non-intrusively, exploring some aspects that remain unclear about writing style (i.e., stability), and the possibility of comparing student outcomes according to their writing strategy preference in intervention studies as one key individual feature of writers at different ages (Kieft et al., 2008).

Therefore, the main goal of the present study is to analyze the factor structure and validity of a Spanish version of the Writing Strategies Questionnaire (WSQ-SP) (Kieft et al., 2006, 2008) implemented with Spanish upper-primary students, analyzing the adjustment of the factorial model proposed based on the scientific literature (De Smedt et al., 2018), which consists of four interrelated factors taking into account the recursive nature of the writing process: Thinking, Planning, Revision, and Monitoring (see Figure 1). Additionally, the traditional two-factor model initially found (Kieft et al., 2006, 2008) will also be explored to test which is the most appropriate scale structure for the questionnaire.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Hypothesized model of the factor structure of the WSQ-SP, composed of four interrelated factors.

Moreover, a second goal of the study is to analyze the factorial invariance of the proposed model by considering different variables such as gender and grade.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The sample comprised 651 Spanish primary school students in 16 fourth-grade (N = 178, 27%), 16 fifth-grade (N = 246; 38%), and 14 sixth-grade classes (N = 227; 35%). Students' ages ranged from 9 to 13 (Mage = 9.5 years, SD = 0.55 for fourth graders; Mage = 10.4 years, SD = 0.52 for fifth graders; Mage = 11.5 years, SD = 0.54 for sixth graders) and with similar proportions of boys and girls (47.19% girls in 4th grade; 48.37% girls in 5th grade; 55.07% girls in 6th grade). The students came from seven public and four semi-private schools in the city of Ponferrada, finding students from families with a high diversity of socio-economic status. However, it should be noted that most students came from families with medium to high incomes.

The criterion for choosing the participants of the study was that they should be students in 5th or 6th grade of elementary education and that Spanish should be their first language. Students in their final years of primary education were considered for developmental reasons. According to the studies of Berninger et al. (1992, 1994, 1996), planning and revision skills appear progressively during the primary education stage, with the last processes appearing in the last grades (5th and 6th). Additionally, although students with learning disabilities participated in the study, their data was not considered for the analysis. This was done on the basis of previous studies, which have shown differences in the use of high-level cognitive processes between upper-primary students with and without learning disabilities (García and Fidalgo, 2008; Graham et al., 2017b).

Procedure

Prior to the implementation of the study, consent was requested from the Consejería de Educación de Castilla y León [Regional Department of Education of Castilla and Leon], the autonomous community in which the study was carried out. Once the study was approved by the expert committee of the regional department of Education, the researchers contacted all the schools in Ponferrada and surrounding areas. Subsequently, a meeting was held with the heads of the schools to inform them in detail about the study and the procedure to be followed during its execution. Those schools that decided to participate in the study sent the parents an information letter in which the research aims were presented, asking them for informed consent for their children to participate in the study. They were given the opportunity to express concerns and to request that their children's data not be included in the study. Following that, the study was undertaken with participation from only those students whose parents had given informed consent. The study was conducted following the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) (Williams, 2008).

Data was collected in a natural context within regular Spanish language classes. Students were asked to complete the Spanish WSQ and writing a narrative text in a 50-min session. The questionnaire was administered by one of the researchers in this study who has a degree in Psychology and experience in administering similar kind of tests. Additionally, she received specific training on the implementation of the questionnaire. Moreover, the assessment session was audio-recorded to make sure that the assessment procedure occurred as intended.

Measures

Students' Writing Strategies

In this study, we began with the 26-item questionnaire measuring students' writing strategies that has been used in previous studies (Kieft et al., 2006, 2008). Students rate their agreement with each item on a five-point scale (1–5).

For the translation of the questionnaire, we combined direct and inverse translation of the items. The questionnaire was translated from Dutch to English by a Dutch researcher who was also fluent in English and Spanish. Then this researcher and a member of the Spanish team each separately translated the English version into Spanish, in order to compare the two versions. The two Spanish translations were compared and discussed, looking for possible discrepancies.

Following that, an expert-panel assessed the suitability of the questionnaire according to the age of the target population. This panel of experts was made up of five schoolteachers with extensive experience in education (three in primary education, one in early childhood education and one in special needs education). Some changes were made to the wording to improve the understanding of the meaning of some items.

The first version of the questionnaire was then trialed with a small sample of upper-primary students to identify possible mistakes and assess general understanding. Students had no issues with it, hence no changes were made, and this produced the final version of the questionnaire (see Supplementary Material).

Data Analysis

In order to explore the psychometric properties of the questionnaire, we first analyzed the normal distribution of each item, verifying that they gave kurtosis and skewness indices between ±7 and ±3, respectively (Kline, 2011). The magnitude and direction of the relationship between items was also analyzed using Pearson's correlation coefficient.

The validity of the factor structure was analyzed in two steps. First, we conducted exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with the aim of determining whether the items saturated the two factors of the original version or the four factors proposed in the present study (see Figure 1). Second, we performed a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA).

The maximum likelihood method was used to estimate the model using the covariance matrix of the items in order to analyze the fit of the proposed model. In order to investigate the model's goodness of fit, a number of statistics were considered: (a) absolute indices such as the Chi-square ratio and degrees of freedom (X2/df) and the goodness-of-fit index (GFI); (b) the comparative fit index (CFI) as an incremental fit index; (c) the adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI) and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) as parsimony adjustment indices. The goodness-of-fit of the model was assessed according to the following rules: (a) the X2/df ratio is <3; (b) values above 0.90 for the goodness-of-fit index (GFI), comparative goodness-of-fit index (CFI) and adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI) are acceptable; (c) values below 0.08 for the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) indicate acceptable model fit (Browne and Cudeck, 1993; Hoyle, 1995; Kline, 1998; Hu and Bentler, 1999; Valdés et al., 2019).

Finally, the factorial invariance of the proposed model was analyzed by testing the fit of the model using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and composite reliability considering the variables gender and school year.

Results

First, the results of the exploratory factor analysis (EFA) of the WSQ-SP are provided in order to check the factor structure of the proposed model, as well as the loading of the items on each of the factors. Second, the results of the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) are presented showing the fit of the proposed model, as well as a comparison with the traditional two-dimensional model. Finally, the results are presented with respect to the factorial invariance of the WSQ-SP questionnaire considering gender and grade.

Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA)

All of the items exhibited values within the range of normal distribution (asymmetry: ranging between −1.37 and 1.20; kurtosis: raging between −0.96 and 0.82), hence the hypothesis of univariate normality was rejected in all cases (Kline, 2011).

An exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was carried out using the Maximum Likelihood extraction method and Oblimin rotation. The data showed a good fit for this kind of model, evidenced by Bartlett's sphericity test (χ2 (171) = 2216.68, p < 0.000) and the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) value of 0.83 (Lloret-Segura et al., 2014). As a criterion for item inclusion, factor weights >0.30 were considered for only one of the factors, reflecting the theoretical soundness of the scale (Hair et al., 1999). Ten items were excluded because they did not match the different factors (items 5, 6, 7, 9, 10, 12, 15, 16, 20, and 24). The results showed that the 16 items of the scale are grouped into four factors which were theoretically identified and retained. These factors were labeled revising, monitoring, thinking, and planning and together explain 32.08% of the variance. The first factor, monitoring, corresponds to how much students checked the content or structure of their text during composition. This factor consisted of six items explaining 18.0% of the variance and had a composite reliability of 0.82. The second factor, revising, is related to how much students revised the content of their text once the text was written. This factor included three items explaining 7.9% of the variance and had a composite reliability of 0.85. The third factor, planning, is related to how much students thought about the content of their text in advance, using external planning devices such as a draft sheet. This factor included three items explaining 3.8% of the variance and had a composite reliability of 0.75. Finally, the fourth factor, thinking, corresponds to how much students needed to have a clear idea of the content or structure of the text in their minds before they started to write. This factor consisted of four items explaining 2.4% of the variance and a composite reliability of 0.79 (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) of the WSQ-SP.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA)

We performed CFA for the 16 items in the WSQ-SP using Amos software in SPSS. We used Maximum Likelihood (ML) factor analysis with the CFA command. The results of the CFA suggest that overall, the model had a good fit to the data according to the indices (χ2/df = 2.23; GFI = 0.96; AGFI = 0.95; CFI = 0.93; RMSEA = 0.04 CI (0.03–0.05).

The values of the regression coefficients suggest that the factors explained an acceptable part of the variance of the items (see Figure 2). The correlation between the factors indicated that the factors were related but did not present problems of collinearity.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Path diagram of the hypothesized model. Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the questionnaire.

Considering that the proposed model was corroborated by the results, it was compared with the traditional two-dimensional structure identified in previous studies (Kieft et al., 2006, 2008). The model proposed in this study exhibited the best factorial fit (see Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Goodness of fit indices for each model of the CFA of the WSQ-SP (N = 651).

Factor Invariance Analysis

To check that the effectiveness of the model was not significantly affected by the features of the sample, the proposed model was subjected to CFA by selecting the sample based on gender and grade. These two variables were chosen for the following reasons. Gender was considered because some studies have shown it to be a variable that can influence student learning and achievement in general (e.g., Reilly et al., 2019) and specifically in the use of cognitive writing strategies (e.g., Berninger et al., 1992; Jones, 2011). Additionally, grade was chosen because it is during this period of schooling that higher-level cognitive processes related to textual planning and revision appear following different rates of development (Berninger et al., 1992, 1994, 1996). The aim was to ensure that the questionnaire is reliable regardless of gender or grade.

As Table 3 shows, the composite reliability of each factor in all of the proposed models, based on the characteristics of the sample and their combinations, is high (ranging between: 0.81 and 0.92 for the monitoring factor; 0.70 and 0.93 for the thinking factor; 0.70 and 0.82 for the planning factor; and 0.81 and 0.91 for the revising factor). The model shows a good overall fit for the gender and grade variables, with the indicators meeting the established parameters. There was just one exception for the adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI) in the case of 4th grade students (0.88), which was very close to the desired value (0.90). When the model was analyzed based on the interaction of gender and grade, the absolute index, Chi-square ratio and degrees of freedom, and the RMSEA as the parsimony adjustment index, demonstrated acceptable model fit, with the remaining indicators being close to the desired value (0.90). However, it is important to note that when the model was analyzed based on gender-grade interaction, the sample shrank considerably. This influenced the results, given that CFA is sensitive to sample size. The literature recommends performing CFA analysis with samples of more than 200 participants (Valdés et al., 2019). In all of the cases analyzing the model with samples smaller than 200 students, some indicators did not give the desired values, as Table 3 shows.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Goodness-of-Fit Indices for the proposed model of the questionnaire based on sample features.

Discussion

The main goal of the present study was to analyze the factor structure and validity of the Spanish WSQ-SP with upper-primary students. An additional goal was to analyze the factorial invariance of the proposed model by considering different variables such as gender and grade.

With regard to the first goal of the study, the results relating to the questionnaire's factor structure were in line with the previous study carried out with Flemish upper-primary students (De Smedt et al., 2018) in which four factors were identified; planning, revising, monitoring and thinking. In addition, on comparing this model with the two-factor model (i.e., planning and revising), generally identified in previous studies with more expert writers (Kieft et al., 2006, 2008), the four-factor model demonstrated a better match with the questionnaire structure.

This four-factor model is consistent with the differentiation of planning and revision processes that have generally been considered in terms of their occurrence during the process of writing a text (Berninger et al., 1994). As planning and revision can occur before or during translating, a distinction was made between advanced and online planning, post-translation and online revision. In this way, the thinking and planning factors were related to the two different, but complementary, ways of planning. According to previous studies, writers differ in how they plan. While some writers make an outline in note form before drafting, others plan without producing an outline. This latter form of planning has been called “mental planning” (Kellogg, 1988; Torrance et al., 2000). Thus, the thinking factor would correspond to mental planning while the planning factor would correspond to outline planning. Similarly, the revising and monitoring factors can be interpreted according to when revision occurs. According to Berninger and Swanson (1994) considering the timing of revision it is possible to differentiate between online revision (i.e., revision that takes place during composition) and post-translation revision (i.e., revision that takes place after composition). Thus, the revising factor would correspond with post-translation revision while the monitoring factor would correspond with online revision. In other words, the results of the present study indicate that the questionnaire is not only exploring students' use of planning and revising strategies in a general way, but rather also assessing different types of planning and revision strategies depending on when they take place when students are writing a text. These results are in line with the arguments presented by Kieft et al. (2007) and Tillema et al. (2011), who pointed out that the revising scale was composed not only of items related to post-translation revision but also to monitoring. Moreover, the better fit of the four-factor model can be explained based on the fact that these processes seem to have different rates of development (Berninger et al., 1992, 1994, 1996). Based on the implementation of cross-sectional studies with students aged between 6 and 15 years old, the authors found that online planning and revision seems to appear at around ages 6–9 (1st−3rd grades). The authors also found that advanced planning and post-translation revision were the last processes to appear around the last years of primary school (ages 9–12; 4th−6th grades). This would clearly explain why the four-factor model has a better fit to the data from primary school pupils. Here, it is also important to consider that the four factors were shown to exhibit correlation but no problems of collinearity were found. This result is in line the view of writing as a recursive activity in which one process may interrupt others during composition (Flower and Hayes, 1980).

In terms of the second goal of the study, analyzing the factorial invariance of the proposed model by considering different variables such as gender and grade, the results showed that the questionnaire structure was independent of the feature of the sample. The results of the present study seem to be generalizable to upper-primary students regardless of gender or grade.

In summary, the major contribution of this study is the validation of the WSQ-SP with upper-primary students, as validation is a critical step for the development of reliable measurement tools in all scientific domains (Muñiz and Fonseca-Pedrero, 2019). From this study, we can conclude that the questionnaire provides more precise information than initially expected and it is a suitable tool for easily, reliably assessing upper-primary students' writing.

The validation of this questionnaire is a first step toward a reliable analysis of this variable, which will continue with analyzing aspects that have not yet been investigated, such as its stability, the moderating effect it has on writing intervention in upper-primary students (Kieft et al., 2006, 2008), and the effect of instruction itself on writing. Having a validated questionnaire will also make it possible to analyze the relationship between students' use of strategies and other important writing-related variables such as reading (Fidalgo et al., 2014; Qin and Liu, 2021), motivation (Rocha et al., 2019), and students' knowledge (Wijekumar et al., 2019). It would also be interesting to analyze the relationship between the results provided by this scale and the writing processes students follow through the use of online measures such as the triple task (García and Fidalgo, 2008; Fidalgo et al., 2014) and thinking aloud (López et al., 2019).

Finally, as an educational contribution, this instrument may be a useful tool that will help provide teachers with information about their students' strategies and consequently help them to adapt the writing instruction according to their needs. All of this, without a doubt, will have a positive impact on students' writing performance, not only in initial educational levels (e.g., López et al., 2017), but also in later educational stages, such as at University, where students often find it difficult to write academic texts (Connelly et al., 2005).

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, under request to the corresponding author, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Consejería de Educación de Castilla y León. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the design of the work, analysis and data interpretation, drafting and revising it critically, and approved it for publication.

Funding

This research has been supported by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness through a project EDU2015-67484-P (MINECO/FEDER).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank staff and students at the Peñalba, Flores del Sil, Espíritu Santo, San Antonio, Jesús Maestro, La Borreca, Navaliegos, Valentín García Yebra, Concepcionistas, Asunción, and San Ignacio schools for their assistance in completing this study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.700770/full#supplementary-material

References

Alamargot, D., and Chanquoy, L. (2001). Studies in Writing Series: Vol. 9. Through the Models of Writing. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic. doi: 10.1007/978-94-010-0804-4

CrossRef Full Text

Alves, R. A., Limpo, T., Fidalgo, R., Carvalhais, L., Pereira, L. Á., and Castro, S. L. (2016). The impact of promoting transcription on early text production: effects on bursts and pauses, levels of written language, and writing performance. J. Educ. Psychol. 108, 665–679. doi: 10.1037/edu0000089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arias-Gundín, O., and Fidalgo, R. (2017). El perfil escritor como variable moduladora de los procesos involucrados en la composición escrita en estudiantes universitarios [Writer profile as a modulating variable of processes involved in written composition in undergraduate students]. EJIHPE 7, 59–68. doi: 10.30552/ejihpe.v7i1.195

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arrimada, M., Torrance, M., and Fidalgo, R. (2019). Effects of teaching planning strategies to first-grade writers. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 89, 670–688. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauvais, C., Olive, T., and Passerault, J. M. (2011). Why are some texts good and others not? Relationship between text quality and management of the writing processes. J. Educ. Psychol. 103, 415–428. doi: 10.1037/a0022545

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berninger, V. W., Cartwright, A. C., Yates, C. M., Swanson, H. L., and Abbott, R. D. (1994). Developmental skills related to writing and reading acquisition in the intermediate grades: shared and unique functional systems. Read. Writ. 6, 161–196. doi: 10.1007/BF01026911

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berninger, V. W., and Swanson, H. L. (1994). “Modifying Hayes and Flower's model of skilled writing to explain beginning and developing writing,” in Children's Writing: Toward a Process Theory of the Development of Skilled Writing, Vol. 2, ed E. C. Butterfield (Greenwich, CT: JAI Press), 57–81.

Berninger, V. W., Whitaker, D., Feng, Y., Swanson, H. L., and Abbott, R. D. (1996). Assessment of planning, translating, and revising in junior high writers. J. Sch. Psychol. 34, 23–52. doi: 10.1016/0022-4405(95)00024-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berninger, V. W., Yates, C. M., Cartwright, A. C., Rutberg, J., Remy, E., and Abbott, R. D. (1992). Lower-level developmental skills in beginning writing. Read. Writ. 4, 257–280. doi: 10.1007/BF01027151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggs, J., Lai, P., Tang, C., and Lavelle, E. (1999). Teaching writing to ESL graduate students. A model and an illustration. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 69, 293–306. doi: 10.1348/000709999157725

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, M., and Cudeck, R. (1993). “Alternative ways of assessing model fit,” in Testing Structural Equation Models, eds K. A. Bollen and J. S. Long (Newbury Park, CA: SAGE Publicatons), 136–162.

Google Scholar

Connelly, V., Dockrell, J. E., and Barnett, J. (2005). The slow handwriting of undergraduate students constrains overall performance in exam essays. Educ. Psychol. 25, 99–107. doi: 10.1080/0144341042000294912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Smedt, F., Merchie, E., Barendse, M., Rosseel, Y., De Naeghel, J., and Van Keer, H. (2018). Cognitive and motivational challenges in writing: studying the relation with writing performance across students' gender and achievement level. Read. Res. Q. 53, 249–272. doi: 10.1002/rrq.193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fidalgo, R., and García, J. N. (2009). La evaluación de la metacognición en la composición escrita [Evaluating metacognition in written composition]. Estud. Psicol. 30, 51–72. doi: 10.1174/021093909787536290

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fidalgo, R., Torrance, M., Arias-Gundín, O., and Martínez-Cocó, B. (2014). Comparison of reading-writing patterns and performance of students with and without reading difficulties. Psicothema 26, 442–448. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2014.23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flower, L., and Hayes, J. R. (1980). “The dynamics of composing: making plans and juggling constraints,” in Cognitive Processes in Writing, eds L. W. Gregg and E. R. Steinberg (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 31–49.

Google Scholar

García, J. N., and Fidalgo, R. (2008). Orchestration of writing processes and writing products: a comparison of sixth-grade students with and without learning disabilities. Learn. Disabil. Contemp. J. 6, 77–98.

Google Scholar

Graham, S., Collins, A. A., and Rigby-Wills, H. (2017a). Writing characteristics of students with learning disabilities and typically achieving peers: a meta-analysis. Except. Child. 83, 199–218. doi: 10.1177/0014402916664070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, S., and Harris, K. (2018). “Evidence-based writing practices: a meta-analysis of existing meta-analysis,” in Design Principles for Teaching Effective Writing: Theoretical and Empirical Grounded Principles, eds R. Fidalgo, K. Harris, and M. Braaksma (Leiden: Brill Editions), 13–37. doi: 10.1163/9789004270480_003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, S., and Harris, K. R. (2000). The role of self-regulation and transcription skills in writing and writing development. Educ. Psychol. 35, 3–12. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3501_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, S., Harris, K. R., Fishman, E., Houston, J., Wijekumar, K., Lei, P. W., et al. (2019). Writing skills, knowledge, motivation, and strategic behavior predict students' persuasive writing performance in the context of robust writing instruction. Element. Sch. J. 119, 487–510. doi: 10.1086/701720

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, S., Harris, K. R., Kiuhara, S. A., and Fishman, E. J. (2017b). The relationship among strategic writing behavior, writing motivation, and writing performance with young, developing writers. Element. Sch. J. 118, 82–104. doi: 10.1086/693009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J., Anderson, R., Tatham, R., and Black, W. (1999). Análisis multivariante, 5 Edn. Madrid: Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Harris, K. R., Santangelo, T., and Graham, S. (2010). “Metacognition and strategies instruction in writing,” in Metacognition, Strategy Use, and Instruction, eds H. S. Waters and W. Schneider (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 226–256.

Google Scholar

Hayes, J. R. (1996). “A new framework for understanding cognition and affect in writing,” in The Science of Writing: Theories, Methods, Individual Differences, and Applications, eds C. M. Levy and S. Ransdell (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 1–27.

Hayes, J. R. (2012). “Evidence from language bursts, revision, and transcription for translation and its relation to other writing processes,” in Translation of Thought to Written Text While Composing: Advancing Theory, Knowledge, Research, Methods, Tools, and Applications, eds M. Fayol, D. Alamargot, and V. W. Berninger (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 15–25.

Google Scholar

Hoyle, R. H. (1995). Structural Equation Modeling: Concepts, Issues, and Applications. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equat. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, S. (2011). Mapping the landscape: gender and the writing classroom. J. Writ. Res. 3, 161–179. doi: 10.17239/jowr-2012.03.03.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellogg, R. T. (1988). Attentional overload and writing performance: effects of rough draft and outline strategies. J. Exp. Psychol.14, 355–365. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.14.2.355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kieft, M., Rijlaarsdam, G., Galbraith, D., and Van den Bergh, H. (2007). The effects of adapting a writing course to students' writing strategies. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 77, 565–578. doi: 10.1348/096317906X120231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kieft, M., Rijlaarsdam, G., and Van den Bergh, H. (2006). Writing as a learning tool: testing the role of students' writing strategies. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 12, 17–34. doi: 10.1007/BF03173567

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kieft, M., Rijlaarsdam, G., and van den Bergh, H. (2008). An aptitude-treatment interaction approach to writing-to-learn. Learn. Instruct. 18, 379–390. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, P. (1998). The New Pychometrics: Science, Psychology, and Measurement. London: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2011). “Convergence of structural equation modeling and multilevel modeling,” in Handbook of Methodological Innovation in Social Research Methods, eds M. Williams and W. P. Vogt (London: Sage), 562–589. doi: 10.4135/9781446268261.n31

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavelle, E., Smith, J., and O'Ryan, L. (2002). The writing approaches of secondary students. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 72, 399–418. doi: 10.1348/000709902320634564

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Limpo, T., Alves, R. A., and Connelly, V. (2017). Examining the transcription-writing link: effects of handwriting fluency and spelling accuracy on writing performance via planning and translating in middle grades. Learn. Individ. Diff. 53, 26–36. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.11.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llaurado, A., and Dockrell, J. E. (2020). The impact of orthography on text production in three languages: catalan, English, and Spanish. Front. Psychol. 11:878. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lloret-Segura, S., Ferreres-Traver, A., Hernández-Baeza, A., and Tomás-Marco, I. (2014). El análisis factorial exploratorio de los ítems: una guía práctica, revisada y actualizada. Ann. Psychol. 30, 1151–1169. doi: 10.6018/analesps.30.3.199361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López, P., Torrance, M., and Fidalgo, R. (2019). The online management of writing processes and their contribution to text quality in upper-primary students. Psicothema 31, 311–318. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2018.326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López, P., Torrance, M., Rijlaarsdam, G., and Fidalgo, R. (2017). Effects of direct instruction and strategy modeling on upper-primary students' writing development. Front. Psychol. 8:1054. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCutchen, D. (2011). From novice to expert: implications of language skills and writing-relevant knowledge for memory during the development of writing skill. J. Writ. Res. 3, 51–68. doi: 10.17239/jowr-2011.03.01.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muñiz, J., and Fonseca-Pedrero, E. (2019). Diez pasos para la construcción de un test [Ten steps for test development]. Psicothema 31, 7–16. doi: 10.18682/pd.v1i1.854

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pontart, V., Bidet-Ildei, C., Lambert, E., Morisset, P., Flouret, L., and Alamargot, D. (2013). Influence of handwriting skills during spelling in primary and lower secondary grades. Front. Psychol. 4:818. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puranik, C. S., Boss, E., and Wanless, S. (2019). Relations between self-regulation and early writing: domain specific or task dependent?. Early Child. Res. Q. 46, 228–239. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.02.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qin, J., and Liu, Y. (2021). The influence of reading texts on L2 reading-to-write argumentative writing. Front. Psychol. 12:655601. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.655601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., and Andrews, G. (2019). Gender differences in reading and writing achievement: evidence from the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP). Am. Psychol. 74:445. doi: 10.1037/amp0000356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robledo Ramón, P., Arias-Gundín, O., Palomo, M., Andina, E., and Rodríguez, C. (2018). Perfil escritor y conocimiento metacognitivo de las tareas académicas en los estudiantes universitarios. Publicaciones 48, 243–270. doi: 10.30827/publicaciones.v48i1.7335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rocha, R. S., Filipe, M., Magalhães, S., Graham, S., and Limpo, T. (2019). Reasons to write in grade 6 and their association with writing quality. Front. Psychol. 10:2157. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santangelo, T., Harris, K., and Graham, S. (2016). “Self-regulation and writing,” in Handbook of Writing Research, 2nd Edn, eds C. A. MacArthur, S. Graham, and J. Fitzgerald (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 174–193.

Google Scholar

Tillema, M., van den Bergh, H., Rijlaarsdam, G., and Sanders, T. (2011). Relating self-reports of writing behaviour and online task execution using a temporal model. Metacogn. Learn. 6, 229–253. doi: 10.1007/s11409-011-9072-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrance, M., Thomas, G. V., and Robinson, E. J. (1994). The writing strategies of graduate research students in the social sciences. High. Educ. 27, 379–392. doi: 10.1007/BF03179901

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrance, M., Thomas, G. V., and Robinson, E. J. (1999). Individual differences in the writing behaviour of undergraduate students. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 69, 189–199. doi: 10.1348/000709999157662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrance, M., Thomas, G. V., and Robinson, E. J. (2000). Individual differences in undergraduate essay writing strategies. A longitudinal study. High. Educ. 39, 181–200. doi: 10.1023/A:1003990432398

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdés, A. A., García, F. I., Torres, G. M., Urías, M., and Grijalva, C. S. (2019). Medición en Investigación Educativa con Apoyo del SPSS y el AMOS [Measurement in Educational Research with Support of SPSS and AMOS]. Clave Editorial.

Google Scholar

Wijekumar, K., Graham, S., Harris, K. R., Lei, P. W., Barkel, A., Aitken, A., et al. (2019). The roles of writing knowledge, motivation, strategic behaviors, and skills in predicting elementary students' persuasive writing from source material. Read. Writ. 32, 1431–1457. doi: 10.1007/s11145-018-9836-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. R. (2008). Revising the declaration of Helsinki. World Med. J. 54, 120–125.

Zeidner, M., Boekaerts, M., and Pintrich, P. R. (2000). “Self-regulation: directions and challenges for future research,” in Self-Regulation: Theory, Research, and Applications, eds M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, and M. Zeidner (Orlando, FL: Academic Press), 749–768. doi: 10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50052-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: writing strategies, questionnaire, upper-primary education, psychometrics, validity

Citation: Arias-Gundín O, Real S, Rijlaarsdam G and López P (2021) Validation of the Writing Strategies Questionnaire in the Context of Primary Education: A Multidimensional Measurement Model. Front. Psychol. 12:700770. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.700770

Received: 26 April 2021; Accepted: 17 May 2021;
Published: 05 July 2021.

Edited by:

María Isabel de Vicente-Yagüe Jara, University of Murcia, Spain

Reviewed by:

Inmaculada Clotilde Santos Díaz, University of Malaga, Spain
Ester Trigo-Ibáñez, Universidad de Cádiz, Spain

Copyright © 2021 Arias-Gundín, Real, Rijlaarsdam and López. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Paula López, plopg@unileon.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.