Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 January 2022
Sec. Perception Science

Skin Color and Attractiveness Modulate Empathy for Pain: An Event-Related Potential Study

\r\nDi Yang,Di Yang1,2Xiong Li,Xiong Li3,4Yinya Zhang,Yinya Zhang1,2Zuoshan Li,Zuoshan Li1,2Jing Meng,*\r\nJing Meng1,2*
  • 1Key Laboratory of Applied Psychology, Chongqing Normal University, Chongqing, China
  • 2School of Education, Chongqing Normal University, Chongqing, China
  • 3Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
  • 4Key Laboratory of Basic Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China

Although racial in-group bias in empathy for pain has been reported, empathic responses to others’ pain may be influenced by other characteristics besides race. To explore whether skin color and attractiveness modulate empathy for pain, we recorded 24 participants’ reactions to painful faces from racial in-group members with different skin color (fair, wheatish, or dark) and attractiveness (more or less attractive) using event-related potentials (ERPs). Results showed that, for more attractive painful faces, dark skin faces were judged as less painful and elicited smaller N2 amplitudes than fair- and wheatish-skinned faces. However, for less attractive faces, there were no significant differences among the three skin colors. Our findings suggest that empathy for pain toward racial in-group members may be influenced by skin color and attractiveness.

Introduction

Empathy refers to a complicated psychological construct that reflects the ability to understand and share others’ emotional states (Arditte Hall et al., 2018). When an individual observes pain or injury of others, they often perceive pain and negative experiences as their own (Levy et al., 2018). This ability is called empathy for pain (Han, 2018; Meng et al., 2019). Empathy helps individuals to avoid potential hazards and promotes empathic behavior in others (Shi et al., 2015; Webb et al., 2017).

The neurophysiological mechanisms underlying empathy for pain can be measured using event-related potentials (ERPs), which have high temporal resolution and can be used in experimental paradigms to record some subjective responses (Cascio et al., 2015; Hu and Iannetti, 2019). Empathic neural responses to others’ pain have been observed in several ERP components following the onset of painful pictures (Jing et al., 2017; Han, 2018; Luo et al., 2018; Meng et al., 2019). For example, an early ERP component (N2) over the frontal-central area of the cortex is related to affective empathy to others’ pain and positively correlates with participants’ personal negative emotional reactions (Mokhtari et al., 2020). What’s more, prior studies found that N2 amplitudes might be the key component in empathy and could predict adults’ affective empathy, with greater N2 amplitudes correlated with more immediate affective empathy (Kerr-Gaffney et al., 2019). Later ERP components [e.g., P3 and late positive complex (LPC), over the central-parietal cortex] represents cognitive empathy and has been shown to correlate with pain intensity judgment of others’ pain (Chen and Liu, 2016; Xia et al., 2016; Meng et al., 2019).

In addition, as principal ERP components elicited after task-relevant visual stimuli, Hong et al. (2016) similarly found the two important time temporal stages of processing painful attractive and unattractive faces pictures (Hong et al., 2016), including an early negativity (N2) around 200–300 ms after the onset of a stimulus and late LPC which peak at the parietal electrodes later than 300 ms post-stimulus. Additionally, researchers have conducted numerous ERP studies on faces with race characteristics. One representative ERP components have been investigated in these studies, namely N170. The N170 is an early negative-going potential over occipito-temporal regions usually peaking at around 170 ms post-stimulus (Beckes et al., 2013; Recio et al., 2014), and is considered to be an indicator of automatic or unconscious processing of facial features (Hajcak et al., 2010). Some researchers found the N170 amplitudes elicited by viewing faces of other races (vs. own-race) were larger (Ito and Urland, 2005; Stahl et al., 2008) which suggests that the N170 may represent social categorization processes.

Although people use various social cues for racial categorization, skin color is one of the most salient race-related phenotypic features (Ebner et al., 2011). Skin color is considered a racial feature that helps to rapidly identify whether an individual belongs to a certain race (Pereira et al., 2019). Indeed, previous studies have shown that distinguishing racial identity according to others’ skin color occurs spontaneously and automatically affects subsequent interpersonal thoughts, feelings, and behavior in observers (Nguyen et al., 2018; Deska et al., 2020).

Racial in-group bias in empathy has been widely reported in previous studies, shown by greater empathy for pain toward racial in-group members compared with that of racial out-group members (e.g., Han, 2018; Luo et al., 2018). One study showed that when Chinese participants were presented with painful facial pictures of Chinese (wheatish skin) and Caucasian (fair skin) models, their empathic neural responses to painful facial pictures of Caucasian models were lower than those of Chinese models (Feng et al., 2015). A similar pattern was found by Fabi and Leuthold (2018), where they compared electroencephalogram (EEG) responses of Caucasian participants (fair skin) to painful pictures of fair- and dark-skinned hands and found that Caucasian participants showed decreased empathic responses toward dark-skinned hands than fair-skinned hands (Fabi and Leuthold, 2018). These studies suggest that individuals exhibit greater empathic responses to racial in-group members’ pain than to racial out-group members’ pain. This effect is explained by racial in-group bias in empathy (Avenanti et al., 2010; Magariño et al., 2020). However, racial identity used in these studies was mainly represented by the skin color of parts of the body (e.g., faces and hands), and models with a similar skin color as participants were perceived as racial in-group members, whereas those with dissimilar skin color were perceived as racial out-group members. Furthermore, it is possible that skin color also represents physical fitness and attractiveness of an individual (Stepanova and Strube, 2012; Niesta Kayser et al., 2016; Visconti et al., 2018; Freitas et al., 2020). Thus, others’ skin color may play a crucial role that may currently be underestimated. It remains unclear whether empathic responses to others’ pain could be influenced by the skin color of racial in-group members.

Skin color, especially facial skin color, plays an important role in judgments of physiological health, which include fitness, immunity, and fertility (Bixley et al., 2018). There have been numerous studies that have suggested that individuals’ perceptions of physical fitness are influenced by skin color (Carrito et al., 2016; Dias, 2020), and physical fitness has important implications for resisting potential threat and harm (Ogunjimi et al., 2020). One study showed that when participants were asked to select the healthiest person from photographs of individuals with different skin color, they consistently chose dark skin over fair or wheatish skin (Cairns et al., 2020). Furthermore, individuals with slightly dark skin (which may indicate more efficient blood circulation) were considered more attractive and healthy (Jones, 2018). This may be because individuals with dark skin are considered to have low risk of sunburn and skin diseases (Stepanova and Strube, 2012; Desai et al., 2020; Freitas et al., 2020).

The “beauty-is-good” stereotype (Little et al., 2006) supposes that facial attractiveness is a marker of biological quality that signals fertility and health and that it plays a significant role in interpersonal interactions in daily life (Nakamura and Watanabe, 2020). To date, there have been no consistent conclusions regarding the influence of attractiveness on empathy for pain. One study showed that attractiveness facilitates empathy for pain, whereby greater empathic responses were elicited for more attractive than less attractive faces (Meng et al., 2020). However, another study revealed that physical attractiveness inhibits children’s empathy for pain (Fisher and Ma, 2014). Therefore, to investigate the effect of others’ skin color on empathic responses to others’ pain and the interaction between skin color and attractiveness, we considered the modulation effect of both skin color and attractiveness in the present study.

Based on previous findings showing that individuals with dark skin are perceived as healthy and having better physical fitness (Ogunjimi et al., 2020) and the “beauty-is-good” stereotype (Little et al., 2006), we hypothesized that empathy for pain would be influenced by others’ attractiveness and skin color and that both behavioral and neural responses to more attractive and dark-skinned individuals in pain would be inhibited.

Materials and Methods

Twenty-four adults (13 women) from the Chongqing Normal University participated in this study as paid volunteers. None of the participants had been previously diagnosed with a psychiatric, medical, or neurological disorder. All participants were right-handed Chinese adults between the ages of 18 and 24 years [mean = 21.8 years, standard deviation (SD) = 2.4 years]. Written informed consent was provided by all participants prior to participation in the experiment in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures were approved by Chongqing Normal University research ethics committee. The procedures were performed in accordance with ethical guidelines and regulations.

After conducting the experiment, a post hoc power analysis using Gpower 31 (Faul et al., 2007) was conducted using a conservative average of the moderate effect sizes from previous sharing empathy for pain studies (within factors, F-test, Cohen’s d = 0.79–0.44; Rütgen et al., 2015a,b). We used sample size of 24 participants to calculate a power of 1−ß = 0.99 at a standard error probability of α = 0.05 with moderate effect size of d = 0.4.

Stimuli

The stimuli (see Figure 1 for examples) were 480 digital pictures of Chinese faces, which were revised from a picture database that had been validated and used in previous studies (Li et al., 2020; Meng et al., 2020). The database comprised pictures of 40 more attractive faces (20 female faces and 20 male faces) and 40 less attractive faces (20 female faces and 20 male faces). Painful pictures depicted the model having a syringe needle penetrating their cheek, and non-painful pictures depicted a soft object (Q-tip) gently touching the model’s cheek. The skin color of each face was transformed into three different skin colors (fair, wheatish, and dark) using the Adobe Photoshop CS2 (Adobe Systems Incorporated, CA, United States) software. Luminance, contrast, and color were matched across painful and non-painful pictures. Moreover, to reduce interference caused by repetitive stimuli, all pictures were mirror flipped once.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Examples of pictures used in the study. Examples of more (left panel) and less (right panel) attractive faces and painful (top panel) and non-painful (bottom panel) faces with fair, wheatish, and dark skin. Pictures were revised from a picture database that had been validated and used in previous studies Li et al. (2020) and Meng et al. (2020).

Before the experiment, skin color (1 = fair, 5 = wheatish, and 9 = dark), attractiveness (1 = not at all attractive, 9 = most attractive), and emotional valence (1 = very happy, 5 = neutral, and 9 = very unhappy) of the pictures were assessed using a 9-point Likert scale by 51 undergraduate students (25 women, aged 18–26 years, mean = 24.22 years, SD = 3.4 years) who did not participate in the experiment. Detailed descriptive statistics of this assessment are summarized in Supplementary Table 1.

Experimental Procedure

Participants were seated in a quiet and comfortable room with an ambient temperature of ∼23°C. Participants were instructed to determine whether the model in picture was experiencing pain. As shown in Figure 2, at the start trial, a 500 ms white fixation cross was presented on a black screen, followed by a blank black screen that was presented for 800–1,500 ms. A picture was then presented, and participants were instructed to respond as accurately and as quickly as possible by pressing a key (either “1” or “2”) to judge whether the presented face in the picture was experiencing pain. The keys pressed were counterbalanced across participants to control for order effects. The picture disappeared from the screen as soon as the participant responded. If the participant did not respond, the next trial was automatically carried out after 3,000 ms. The order of picture presentation was randomized. Presentation of pictures was controlled using the E-Prime 3.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, PA, United States). The entire experimental procedure comprised four blocks, with 240 trials per block and an inter-trial interval of 500 ms. Each picture was presented once during the experiment. A training session was conducted before the formal experiment to allow participants to familiarize themselves with the experimental procedure. EEG data were recorded throughout the experimental procedure.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Flowchart describing the procedure of the experiment.

Following the EEG recording session, participants were asked to rate each picture based on four attributes on a 9-point Likert scale, which included pain intensity (1 = no sensation, 4 = pain threshold, and 9 = unbearable pain), skin color (1 = fair, 5 = wheatish, and 9 = dark), and attractiveness (1 = not at all attractive, 9 = more attractive) of the model in the pictures, and their subjective emotional reaction (1 = very unhappy, 5 = neutral, and 9 = very happy).

Electroencephalogram Recording

Electroencephalogram data were recorded from 64 scalp sites using tin electrodes mounted on an actiCHamp system (Brain Vision LLC, Morrisville, NC, United States; bandpass: 0.01–100 Hz; sampling rate: 1,000 Hz). The electrode on the medial frontal aspect was used as the ground electrode. All electrode impedances remained below 5 kΩ.

Electroencephalogram Data Analysis

Electroencephalogram data were pre-processed and analyzed using MATLAB R2014a (MathWorks, United States) and the EEGLAB v13.6.5b toolbox (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). We used left-right mastoids as analyzing reference. Continuous EEG signals were bandpass filtered (0.01–30 Hz). Time windows of 200 ms before and 800 ms after the onset of stimuli were extracted from continuous EEG data, and the extracted window was baseline-corrected by a 200 ms time interval prior to stimulus onset. EEG epochs were visually inspected and trials containing significant noise from gross movements were removed. Electro-oculogram artifacts were corrected using an independent component analysis (ICA) algorithm (Jung et al., 2001). Epochs with amplitude values exceeding 80 μV at any electrode were excluded from the presented average. In our experimental tasks, EEG signals were impaired by participants’ movement, violent blinking, electromyography, or other interference during the experiment. We excluded some of bad trials. These excluded trials only constituted 6 ± 4.1% of the total number of trials.

We confirmed scalp topographies in both single-participant and group-level ERP waveforms. The ERP components selected in this study included early components (N1, P2, and N2) and late components (P3 and LPC) based on previously studies of empathy for pain (Sessa and Meconi, 2015; Hu and Iannetti, 2016; Meng et al., 2020). We also included N170, which is induced by face stimuli (Itier and Taylor, 2004). ERP components were extracted from following electrode sites: N1 (FCz, FC1, FC2, Cz, C1, and C2) within N1 latency intervals of 100–120 ms; P2 and N2 (AFz, AF3, AF4, Fz, F1, F2, FCz, FC1, and FC2) within P2 latency intervals of 180–200 ms and N2 latency intervals of 200–220 ms; P3 and LPC (CPz, CP1, CP2, Pz, P1, P2, POz, PO3, and PO4) within P3 latency intervals of 290–310 ms and the LPC was extracted within a time window of 400–600 ms; N170 (P7, P8, PO7, and PO8) within latency intervals of 160–180 ms.

To obtain genuine neural responses of empathy for pain, differential ERP waves were also used in the present study, which were obtained by subtracting the ERP waves of non-painful pictures from those of painful pictures (Ibáñez et al., 2011; Meng et al., 2012; Cui et al., 2016). Amplitudes of differential ERP waveforms were calculated at the same electrode sites and time windows as the original ERP components (i.e., N1, N2, P2, P3, LPC, and N170). Amplitudes of differential ERP waveforms were described as D_(ERP component); for example, D_N2 = N2 amplitude of painful pictures minus N2 amplitude of non-painful pictures.

Statistical Analysis

Behavioral Data

Behavioral data, which included accuracies (ACCs), reaction times (RTs), and subjective ratings of pictures (i.e., pain intensity, attractiveness, skin color, and subjective emotional reaction ratings), were compared using a three-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), with within-participant factors of “pain” (painful, non-painful), “attractiveness” (more attractive, less attractive), and “skin color” (fair, wheatish, and dark). For significant interaction effects (p < 0.05), we performed simple effect analyses. The p-values of the main and interaction effects were corrected using the Greenhouse-Geisser method (Jessen and Kotz, 2011).

Event-Related Potential Data

Amplitudes of differential ERP waveforms between painful and non-painful pictures were compared using two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, with within-participant factors of “attractiveness” (more attractive, less attractive) and “skin color” (fair, wheatish, and dark). For significant interaction effects (p < 0.05), we performed simple effect analyses. The p-values of the main and interaction effects were corrected using the Greenhouse-Geisser method (Jessen and Kotz, 2011).

Results

Behavioral Data

The descriptive and statistical analysis results of the behavioral data are shown in Table 1 and Supplementary Table 2, respectively. Pain intensity ratings were modulated by the main effect of “pain” (F1, 23 = 153.66, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.87), which showed that participants judged painful pictures as more painful than non-painful pictures (painful: 6.07 ± 0.46, non-painful: 4.33 ± 0.24). Pain intensity ratings were significantly modulated by the interaction of “pain,” “attractiveness,” and “skin color” (F2, 22 = 6.39, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.22). Simple effects analyses indicated that for more attractive painful faces, dark-skinned faces were judged as less painful than fair-skinned (dark: 6.13 ± 0.22, fair: 6.92 ± 0.28; F2, 22 = 5.79, p = 0.024, ηp2 = 0.20) and wheatish-skinned (wheatish: 7.35 ± 0.32; F2, 22 = 8.34, p = 0.008, ηp2 = 0.27) faces. Pain intensity ratings did not differ between the three kinds of skin faces in the other conditions (p > 0.05 for all comparisons; Figure 3).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of repeated-measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) results of behavioral data.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Bar charts of pain intensity ratings. Bar charts of the more attractive (right panel) or less attractive (left panel) faces with fair (red), wheat (blue), and dark (gray) skin with non-painful (linear) or painful (solid) cues. Data are expressed using means ± standard error of the mean. ns: p > 0.05, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Attractive ratings were significantly modulated by the main effect of “attractiveness” (F1, 23 = 9.48, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.29), which indicated that participants judged more attractive faces as having higher attractiveness than less attractive faces (more attractive: 5.62 ± 0.13, less attractive: 5.13 ± 0.08). Skin color ratings were modulated by “skin color” (F2, 22 = 205.32, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.89), which suggested that participants were able to accurately judge the three skin colors (fair: 3.51 ± 0.11, wheatish: 4.82 ± 0.13, and dark: 6.67 ± 0.09). Subjective emotional reactions were modulated by the main effects of “pain” (F1, 23 = 20.07, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.54) and “attractiveness” (F1, 23 = 40.09, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.70), which indicated that participants expressed more negative emotions to painful pictures than to non-painful pictures (painful: 4.71 ± 0.08, non-painful: 5.15 ± 0.06) and more positive emotions to the more attractive faces relative to the less attractive faces (less attractive: 4.58 ± 0.08, more attractive: 5.29 ± 0.06). No other main effects or interactions were significant (p > 0.05 for all comparisons).

Reaction times were significantly modulated by the interaction of “pain” and “skin color” (F2, 22 = 5.79, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.20). For painful pictures, participants judged dark-skinned faces slower than they judged wheatish-skinned (dark: 674.75 ± 24.32 ms, wheatish: 692.01 ± 28.09 ms; F2, 22 = 5.55, p = 0.027, ηp2 = 0.19) and fair-skinned (fair: 694.74 ± 27.86 ms; F2, 22 = 5.72, p = 0.025, ηp2 = 0.20) faces. No differences were found in any of the other conditions (p > 0.05 for all comparisons).

Accuracies were modulated by the main effects of “pain” (F1, 23 = 8.92, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.28) and “attractiveness” (F1, 23 = 9.18, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.29). Participants judged painful pictures less accurately than they judged non-painful pictures (painful: 97.7 ± 6.3%, non-painful: 98.5± 5.4%) and judged more attractive faces less accurately than they judged less attractive faces (more attractive: 97.9 ± 5.7%, less attractive: 98.35 ± 4.6%). No other significant main effects or interactions were found (p > 0.05 for all comparisons).

Event-Related Potential Data

Grand average ERP waveforms and scalp topographies of painful and non-painful pictures with different skin colors are shown in Figure 4 (high attractiveness faces) and Figure 5 (low attractiveness faces). These pictures elicited N1, N2, and P2 over frontal-central electrodes, N170 over occipito-temporal electrodes, and P3 and LPC at central-parietal electrodes.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Event-related potential (ERP) waveforms and scalp topography distributions to more attractive faces. ERP waveforms, bar charts, and scalp topographies elicited by the more attractive faces with fair (red), wheat (blue), and dark (gray) skin. These pictures had either non-painful (linear) or painful (solid) cues. Electrodes used to estimate ERP amplitudes are marked by black squares on their respective topographic distributions. Data in the bar charts are ERP amplitudes expressed as means ± standard error of the mean.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Event-related potential (ERP) waveforms and scalp topography distributions to less attractive faces. ERP waveforms, bar charts, and scalp topographies elicited by the less attractive faces with fair (red), wheat (blue), and dark (gray) skin. These pictures had either non-painful (linear) or painful (solid) cues. Electrodes used to estimate ERP amplitudes are marked by black squares on their respective topographic distributions. Data in the bar charts are ERP amplitudes expressed as means ± standard error of the mean.

Differential ERP waveforms between painful and non-painful pictures are shown in Figure 6. Amplitudes of D_N2 were significantly modulated by the interaction of “skin color” and “attractiveness” (F2,22 = 3.69, p = 0.036, ηp2 = 0.14). Simple effects analyses indicated that for more attractive faces, D_N2 amplitudes to dark-skinned faces (0.32 ± 0.33 μV) were significantly less negative (smaller amplitudes) than to fair- (−1.37 ± 0.49 μV, F1, 23 = 9.83, p < 0.005, ηp2 = 0.29) and wheat-skinned (−0.64 ± 0.37 μV, F1, 23 = 5.22, p < 0.032, ηp2 = 0.19) faces. There were no differences between the three skin colors for the less attractive faces (F2, 22 = 0.82, p = 0.092, ηp2 = 0.01). No other main effects or interactions were found (p > 0.05 for all comparisons). Statistical analysis results are summarized in Table 2 and detail statistic results of amplitudes of the dominant ERP components were summarized in Supplementary Table 3.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Differential event-related potential (ERP) waveforms between painful and non-painful pictures. Differential ERP waveforms (bottom panel) of more (solid) and less (dotted) attractive faces with fair (red), wheat (blue), and dark (gray) skin. The D_N2 amplitudes are shown in bar charts and are expressed as means ± standard error of the mean (top panel). ns: p > 0.05, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of statistical analyses of amplitudes of differential event-related potential (ERP) waveforms.

Discussion

The present study explored whether empathic responses to others’ pain are affected by others’ skin color and attractiveness. Our results showed that dark-skinned faces were judged as less painful and elicited smaller N2 amplitudes than fair- and wheatish-skinned faces. However, this effect was specific to more attractive painful faces, and not to less attractive faces. These results suggested that empathy for pain to more attractive people may be modulated by skin color and that empathic responses to more attractive dark-skinned painful faces may be inhibited.

The behavioral data analysis showed that participants expressed higher pain intensity ratings and more negative emotional reactions toward painful faces than toward non-painful faces. These results are in line with previous studies using painful pictures that exhibited injuries of the hands and feet (Chen et al., 2012; Fabi and Leuthold, 2017), faces (Meng et al., 2020) as well as painful expressions (Jie et al., 2017). Thus, our findings suggest that in the present study, participants’ affective and cognitive empathy was successfully elicited by the stimuli. In addition, consistent with previous findings that more positive emotional reactions are evoked by more attractive faces than they are by less attractive faces (Shang et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2018), our findings confirmed that emotional reactions were significantly modulated by the main effect of “attractiveness.”

Consistent with a previous ERP study of empathy for others’ facial pain (Meng et al., 2020), in our study, others’ painful faces elicited larger ERP amplitudes than did non-painful faces, which included the frontal-central N1 and the central-parietal P3 and LPC. Given that the N1 is thought to reflect early bottom-up processes, and the P3 and LPC are thought to be linked to top-down cognitive evaluation processes of empathy for pain (Fan and Han, 2008; Sessa and Meconi, 2015; Li et al., 2019), it appears that more mental processing resources to others’ pain were recruited during these time windows for both automatic and controlled processes of empathy for pain. Similarly to previous studies (Sessa et al., 2014; Meng et al., 2020), we also found a main effect of “pain” in LPC amplitude in the pain judgment task, with painful pictures eliciting more positive LPC waves than non-painful pictures. These results may suggest that LPC are sensitive to others’ pain cues, independently of facial skin color and attractiveness. As LPC over the posterior parietal cortical area have been thought to link to a cognitive evaluation component of empathy (Xia et al., 2016; Meng et al., 2019), it appears that processing resources of evaluation of others’ pain were recruited automatically in LPC time windows even though the pain cues in the pain judgment Task were related to attractiveness and skin color. In addition, N170 was not modulated by the blending effects between facial skin color and attractiveness of the faces. About the N170, previous results are regard to the sensitivity of this component for race-specific appearance (Ito and Urland, 2005; Stahl et al., 2008) and the existence of other-race faces elicited the enhanced N170 component. But, the different facial skin colors in this study may not have made participants aware of racial identity differences. This effectively controls for the interference of the irrelevant variable of ethnic identity.

To reduce the influence of the empathy-irrelevant distractor, we calculated differential ERP waveforms between painful and non-painful pictures to reveal the underlying neural processing for empathy for pain, for which the method has been used widely in previous studies (Ibáñez et al., 2011; Meng et al., 2012; Cui et al., 2016). In the present study, we found a significant interaction of “skin color” and “attractiveness” in the differential N2 amplitudes to others’ pain, whereby more attractive dark-skinned faces elicited smaller N2 amplitudes than more attractive fair- and wheatish-skinned faces. However, empathic responses to the less attractive faces were not influenced by skin color. Given that the frontal N2 component is thought to be related to the affective components of empathy for pain (Chen et al., 2012; Luo et al., 2018) and N2 amplitudes have shown to be positively correlated to the degree of empathic responses to others’ pain (Mella et al., 2012; Fabi and Leuthold, 2017), decreased affective empathy is likely to be elicited toward people with more attractive dark-skinned faces. In addition, pain intensity ratings to the more attractive dark-skinned faces were lower than to the more attractive fair- and wheatish-skinned faces. Our results suggest that skin color modulates empathy for pain toward more attractive individuals. Moreover, relative to more attractive people with fair and wheatish skin, empathic responses to pain of more attractive people with dark skin are inhibited.

One possible explanation for the present findings is that empathic responses to others’ pain might be sensitive to their physical condition (Fisher and Ma, 2014). Empathy for pain to healthier (Carrito et al., 2016; Bixley et al., 2018; Dias, 2020) and younger (Tamm et al., 2017) individuals have shown to be reduced. It is possible that people with more attractive dark-skinned faces may be considered healthier and younger than other people, and thus be perceived as having better physical fitness. From an evolutionary perspective, physical fitness has important implications in terms of resisting potential threat and harm (Visconti et al., 2018). Previous work has shown that higher physical fitness is linked to a decreased risk of physical pain (Xiang et al., 2018; Chen et al., 2020), and an antecedent to empathy toward another person is the perception or awareness that the person is in need of help (Bershad et al., 2018). Thus, people with more attractive dark-skinned faces may be perceived as having better physical fitness and being less susceptible to painful feelings; thus, empathic responses to their pain may be inhibited. Interestingly, empathy for less attractive painful faces was not influenced by skin color. Possible explanation for this result would be that the facial attractiveness might influence our process to skin color of faces. Compared more attractive faces, Less attractive faces may not attract too much attention from participants and be processed facial features effectively and fast (Chen et al., 2012). Previous studies on the early perception of attractiveness demonstrated that attractive faces would activate a rapid and automatic perception in a very short time (Sui and Liu, 2009; Rellecke et al., 2011). The facial features of less attractiveness faces are spent more long time for participants to perception (Olson and Marshuetz, 2005). Thus, when participants pay attention to the less attractive faces, empathy for less attractive painful faces was more difficult to be influenced by skin color.

Despite these possible implications, several limitations of the present study should also be addressed. Firstly, both female and male faces were used in the study, and the effects of gender may influence results. Secondly, painful pictures depicted a syringe needle penetrating a model’s cheek. Whether these pictures reflect painful situations in daily life requires further investigation. Finally, the interaction between skin color and attractiveness on empathy for pain was induced experimentally, but the generalizability of the results to real life situations requires further investigation.

Conclusion

We used pictures of faces within participants’ racial in-group to examine whether empathy for pain is affected by skin color and attractiveness of others’ faces. Results suggested that both behavioral and neural empathic responses to more attractive dark-skinned painful faces are lower relative to those to more attractive fair- or wheat-skinned faces, whereas empathy for less attractive painful faces were not influenced by skin color. Thus, empathy for pain may be influenced by the interplay between others’ skin color and attractiveness.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found in the article/Supplementary Material.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Local Research Ethics Committee of Chongqing Normal University. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in the study.

Author Contributions

DY: conceptualization, methodology, software, data curation, and writing—original draft preparation. XL: methodology and software. YZ: data curation and writing—original draft preparation. ZL: supervision. JM: conceptualization, methodology, and writing—reviewing and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Ministry of Education in China, Humanity and Social Science Youth Foundation Project (19YJC190016).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.780633/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://stats.idre.ucla.edu/other/gpower/

References

Arditte Hall, K. A., Joormann, J., Siemer, M., and Timpano, K. R. (2018). The impact bias in self and others: affective and empathic forecasting in individuals with social anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 106, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2018.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avenanti, A., Sirigu, A., and Aglioti, S. M. (2010). Racial bias reduces empathic sensorimotor resonance with other-race pain. Curr. Biol. 20, 1018–1022. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckes, L., Coan, J., and Morris, J. (2013). Implicit conditioning of faces via the social regulation of emotion: ERP evidence of early attentional biases for security conditioned faces. Psychophysiology 50, 734–742. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bershad, A. K., Ruiz, N. A., and De Wit, H. (2018). Effects of buprenorphine on responses to emotional stimuli in individuals with a range of mood symptomatology. Intern. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 21, 120–127. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyx077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bixley, G. S., Clark, K. M., and James, A. P. (2018). Skin colour predicts fruit and vegetable intake in young caucasian men: a cross-sectional study. J. Nutr. Intermed. Metab. 12, 20–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jnim.2018.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cairns, P., Ozakinci, G., and Perrett, D. I. (2020). Reactions to an online demonstration of the effect of Increased fruit and vegetable consumption on appearance: survey study. J. Med. Internet Res. 22:e15726. doi: 10.2196/15726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrito, M. D. L., Santos, I. M. B. D., Lefevre, C. E., Whitehead, R. D., Silva, C. F. D., and Perrtt, D. I. (2016). The role of sexually dimorphic skin colour and shape in attractiveness of male faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 37, 125–133.

Google Scholar

Cascio, C. J., Gu, C., Schauder, K. B., Key, A. P., and Yoder, P. (2015). Somatosensory event-related potentials and association with tactile behavioral responsiveness patterns in children with ASD. Brain Topogr. 28, 895–903. doi: 10.1007/s10548-015-0439-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J., Zhong, J., Zhang, Y., Li, P., Zhang, A., Tan, Q., et al. (2012). Electrophysiological correlates of processing facial attractiveness and its influence on cooperative behavior. Neurosci. Lett. 517, 65–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.02.082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, P. H., Chen, W., Wang, C. W., Yang, H. F., Huang, W. T., Huang, H. C., et al. (2020). Association of physical fitness performance tests and anthropometric indices in taiwanese adults. Front. Physiol. 11:583692. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2020.583692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, W., and Liu, L. (2016). The effect of context on empathy. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 24:91. doi: 10.3724/sp.j.1042.2016.00091

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, F., Zhu, X. R., Duan, F. Y., and Luo, Y. J. (2016). Instructions of cooperation and competition influence the neural responses to others’ pain: an ERP study. Soc. Neurosci. 11, 289–296. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1078258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desai, S., Moore, K., and Hartman, R. I. (2020). Skin cancer prevention counseling in populationwith elevated risk: an analysis of the health information national trends survey 5cycle 2. J. Am. Acad. Dermatol. 85, 242–245. doi: 10.1016/j.jaad.2020.08.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deska, J. C., Kunstman, J. W., Bernstein, M. J., Ogungbadero, T., and Hugenberg, K. (2020). Black racial phenotypicality shapes social pain and support judgments. J. Exper. Soc. Psychol. 90:103998. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2020.103998

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dias, F. A. (2020). How skin color, class status, and gender intersect in the labor market: evidence from a field experiment. Res. Soc. Stratif. Mobil. 65:100477. doi: 10.1016/j.rssm.2020.100477

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebner, N. C., He, Y., Fichtenholtz, H. M., McCarthy, G., and Johnson, M. K. (2011). Electrophysiological correlates of processing faces of younger and older individuals. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 526–535. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabi, S., and Leuthold, H. (2017). Empathy for pain influences perceptual and motor processing: evidence from response force, ERPs, and EEG oscillations. Soc. Neurosci. 12, 701–716. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2016.1238009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabi, S., and Leuthold, H. (2018). Racial bias in empathy: do we process dark- and fair-colored hands in pain differently? an EEG study. Neuropsychologia 114, 143–157. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, Y., and Han, S. (2008). Temporal dynamic of neural mechanisms involved in empathy for pain: an event-related brain potential study. Neuropsychologia 46, 160–173. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statisticalpower analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.1088/1755-1315/148/1/012022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, C., Li, Z., Feng, X., Wang, L., Tian, T., and Luo, Y. J. (2015). Social hierarchy modulates neural responses of empathy for pain. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 485–495. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, R. J., and Ma, Y. (2014). The price of being beautiful: negative effects of attractiveness on empathy for children in need. J. Consum. Res. 41, 436–450. doi: 10.1086/676967

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freitas, R., Martins, A., Silva, J., Alves, C., Pinteus, S., Alves, J., et al. (2020). Highlighting the biological potential of the brown seaweed fucus spiralis for skin applications. Antioxidants 9:611. doi: 10.3390/antiox9070611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hajcak, G., MacNamara, A., and Olvet, D. M. (2010). Event-related potentials, emotion, and emotion regulation: an integrative review. Dev. Neuropsychol. 35, 129–155. doi: 10.1080/87565640903526504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, S. (2018). Neurocognitive basis of racial in-group bias in empathy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 400–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, X., Liu, Y., Sun, J., and Tong, S. (2016). Age-related differences in the modulation of small-world brain networks during a Go/NoGo task. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:100. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Iannetti, G. D. (2016). Issues in pain prediction beyond pain and gain. Trends Neurosci. 39, 640–642. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Iannetti, G. D. (2019). Neural indicators of perceptual variability of pain across species. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 1782–1791. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1812499116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ibáñez, A., Hurtado, E., Lobos, A., Escobar, J., Trujillo, N., Baez, S., et al. (2011). Subliminal presentation of other faces (but not own face) primes behavioral and evoked cortical processing of empathy for pain. Brain Res. 1398, 72–85. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itier, R. J., and Taylor, M. J. (2004). N170 or N1? Spatiotemporal differences between object and face processing using ERPs. Cereb. Cortex 14, 132–142. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, T. A., and Urland, G. R. (2005). The influence of processing objectives on the perception of faces: an ERP study of race and gender perception. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 5, 21–36. doi: 10.3758/cabn.5.1.21

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, S., and Kotz, S. A. (2011). The temporal dynamics of processing emotions from vocal, facial, and bodily expressions. Neuroimage 58, 665–674. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jie, J., Zhuang, M., Luo, P., and Zheng, X. (2017). Hot topics on the research in empathy from the perspective of neuroscience. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 25:1922.

Google Scholar

Jing, J., Gao, C., and Niu, G. (2017). The effect of internet use on empathy. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 25, 652–661. doi: 10.3724/sp.j.1042.2017.00652

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, A. L. (2018). The influence of shape and colour cue classes on facial health perception. Evol. Hum. Behav. 39, 19–29.

Google Scholar

Jung, T. P., Makeig, S., Westerfield, M., Townsend, J., Courchesne, E., and Sejnowski, T. J. (2001). Analysis and visualization of single-trial event-related potentials. Hum. Brain Mapp. 14, 166–185. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerr-Gaffney, J., Harrison, A., and Tchanturia, K. (2019). Cognitive and affective empathy in eating disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychiatry 10:102. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, J., Goldstein, A., Pratt, M., and Feldman, R. (2018). Maturation of pain empathy from child to adult shifts from single to multiple neural rhythms to support interoceptive representations. Sci. Rep. 8:1810. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-19810-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Li, Z., Xiang, B., and Meng, J. (2020). Empathy for pain in individuals with autistic traits influenced by attention cues: evidence from an ERP study. Acta Psychol. Sinica 52, 294–306.

Google Scholar

Li, X., Zhang, Y., Xiang, B., and Meng, J. (2019). Differences between empathy for face and body pain: cognitive and neural responses. Brain Sci. Adv. 5, 256–264. doi: 10.26599/BSA.2019.9050022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., and Perrett, D. I. (2006). What is good is beautiful: face preference reflects desired personality. Pers. Individ. Differ. 41, 1107–1118.

Google Scholar

Luo, S., Han, X., Du, N., and Han, S. (2018). Physical coldness enhances racial in-group bias in empathy: electrophysiological evidence. Neuropsychologia 116, 117–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magariño, L. S., Evans, M. C., Duong, J. B., Villodas, F., and Villodas, M. T. (2020). Racial group differences in parenting attitudes among at risk emerging adults: the roles of adversity, relationship quality, and caregiver involvement and attitudes. Child Abuse Neglect. 10:104810. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mella, N., Studer, J., Gilet, A. L., and Labouvie-Vief, G. (2012). Empathy for pain from adolescence through adulthood: an event-related brain potential study. Front. Psychol. 3:501. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meng, J., Hu, L., Shen, L., Yang, Z., Chen, H., Huang, X. T., et al. (2012). Emotional primes modulate the responses to others’ pain: an ERP study. Exper. Brain Res. 220, 277–286. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3136-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meng, J., Li, X., Peng, W., Li, Z., and Shen, L. (2020). The interaction between pain and a-ttractiveness perception in others. Sci. Rep. 10:5528.

Google Scholar

Meng, J., Shen, L., Li, Z., and Peng, W. (2019). Top-down attention modulation on the perception of others’ vocal pain: an event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia 133:107177. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mokhtari, T., Ren, Q., Li, N., Wang, F., Bi, Y., and Hu, L. (2020). Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation in relieving neuropathic pain: basic mechanisms and clinical applications. Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 24:14. doi: 10.1007/s11916-020-0846-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura, K., and Watanabe, K. (2020). A new data-driven mathematical model dissociates attractiveness from sexual dimorphism of human faces. Sci. Rep. 10:16588. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-73472-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, T. T., Vable, A. M., Glymour, M. M., and Nuru-Jeter, A. (2018). Trends for reported discrimination in health care in a national sample of older adults with chronic conditions. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 33, 291–297. doi: 10.1007/s11606-017-4209-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niesta Kayser, D., Agthe, M., and Maner, J. K. (2016). Strategic sexual signals: women’s display versus avoidance of the color red depends on the attractiveness of an anticipated nteraction partner. PLoS One 11:e0148501. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0148501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogunjimi, A. T., Carr, J., Lawson, C., Ferguson, N., and Brogden, N. K. (2020). Micropore closure time is longer following microneedle application to skin of color. Sci. Rep. 10:18963. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-75246-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olson, I. R., and Marshuetz, C. (2005). Facial attractiveness is appraised in a glance. Emotion 5, 498–502. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pereira, F., Guimarães, R. M., Lucidi, A. R., Brum, D. G., Paiva, C., and Alvarenga, R. (2019). A systematic literature review on the European, African and Amerindian genetic ancestry components on Brazilian health outcomes. Sci. Rep. 9:8874.

Google Scholar

Recio, G., Schacht, A., and Sommer, W. (2014). Recognizing dynamic facial expressions of emotion: specificity and intensity effects in event-related brain potentials. Biol. Psychol. 96, 111–125. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rellecke, J., Bakirtas, A. M., Sommer, W., and Schacht, A. (2011). Automaticity in attractive face processing: brain potentials from a dual task. Neuroreport 22, 706–710. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834a89ad

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rütgen, M., Seidel, E.-M., Riečanský, I., and Lamm, C. (2015a). Reduction of empathy for pain by placebo analgesia suggests functional equivalence of empathy and firsthand emotion experience. Neuroscience 35, 8938–8947. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3936-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rütgen, M., Seidel, E.-M., Silani, G., Riečanský, I., Hummer, A., Windischberger, C., et al. (2015b). Placebo analgesia and its opioidergic regulation suggest that empathy for pain is grounded in self pain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, E5638–E5646. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1511269112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sessa, P., and Meconi, F. (2015). Perceived trustworthiness shapes neural empathic responses toward others’ pain. Neuropsychologia 79, 97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.10.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sessa, P., Meconi, F., Castelli, L., and Dell’Acqua, R. (2014). Taking one’s time in feeling other-race pain: an event-related potential investigation on the time-course of cross-racial empathy. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 454–463. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shang, J., Chen, W., and Ji, L. (2018). The role of facial attractiveness in cognitive process and its neural mechanism. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 26:241. doi: 10.3724/sp.j.1042.2018.00241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, Y., Xu, F., Wang, W., Li, Y., and Liu, C. (2015). Empathetic social pain: evidence from neuroimaging. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 23:1608. doi: 10.3724/sp.j.1042.2015.01608

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stahl, J., Wiese, H., and Schweinberger, S. R. (2008). Expertise and own-race bias in face processing: an event-related potential study. Neuroreport 19, 583–587. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f97b4d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stepanova, E. V., and Strube, M. J. (2012). The role of skin color and facial physiognomy in racial categorization: moderation by implicit racial attitudes. J. Exper. Soc. Psychol. 48, 867–878. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.02.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sui, J., and Liu, C. H. (2009). Can beauty be ignored? Effects of facial attractiveness on covert attention. Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 16, 276–281. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamm, S., Nilsonne, G., Schwarz, J., Lamm, C., Kecklund, G., Petrovic, P., et al. (2017). The effect of sleep restriction on empathy for pain: an fMRI study in younger and older adults. Sci. Rep. 7:12236. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12098-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visconti, A., Duffy, D. L., Liu, F., Zhu, G., Wu, W., Chen, Y., et al. (2018). Genome-wide association study in 176,678 Europeans reveals genetic loci for tanning response to sun exposure. Nat. Commun. 9:1684. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-04086-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, G., Chen, J., and Zhang, K. (2018). The perception of emotional facial expressions by children with autism using hybrid multiple factorial design and eye-tracking. Chin. Sci. Bull. 63, 3204–3216. doi: 10.1360/n972018-00553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, C. E., Romero, T., Franks, B., and De Waal, F. B. M. (2017). Long-term consistency in chimpanzee consolation behaviour reflects empathetic personalities. Nat. Commun. 8:292. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-00360-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xia, X. L., Peng, W. W., Iannetti, G. D., and Hu, L. (2016). Laser-evoked cortical responses in freely-moving rodents reflect the activation of C-fibre afferent pathways. Neuroimage 128, 209–217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.12.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiang, Y., Wang, Y., Gao, S., Zhang, X., and Cui, R. (2018). Neural mechanisms with respect to different paradigms and relevant regulatory factors in empathy for pain. Front. Neurosci. 12:507. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: empathy, pain, skin color, attractiveness, event-related potentials

Citation: Yang D, Li X, Zhang Y, Li Z and Meng J (2022) Skin Color and Attractiveness Modulate Empathy for Pain: An Event-Related Potential Study. Front. Psychol. 12:780633. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.780633

Received: 22 September 2021; Accepted: 02 December 2021;
Published: 04 January 2022.

Edited by:

Paola Ricciardelli, University of Milano-Bicocca, Italy

Reviewed by:

Harald C. Traue, University of Ulm, Germany
Tessa Marzi, University of Florence, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Yang, Li, Zhang, Li and Meng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jing Meng, qufumj@qq.com; orcid.org/0000-0002-7083-5551

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.