Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 10 January 2022
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Mental Fatigue and Sport: From the Lab to the Field View all 6 articles

Mental Fatigue and Basketball Performance: A Systematic Review

  • 1Faculty of Educational Studies, Universiti Putra Malaysia, Serdang, Malaysia
  • 2Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, Universiti Putra Malaysia, Serdang, Malaysia
  • 3Department of Physical Education, Wuhan Sports University, Wuhan, China

Mental fatigue (MF) is a psycho-biological state that impairs sports-related performances. Recently, it has been proved that MF can affect basketball performance. However, a systematic overview detailing the influences of MF on basketball performance is still lacking. This study aims to investigate the effects of MF on the physical, technical, tactical, and cognitive performance of basketball. We used the databases of PubMed, Web of Science, SPORTDiscus, Scopes, and CKNI for articles published up to 31 May 2021. The articles included in this study were projected to test whether MF influences basketball athlete performance. Only experimental design studies were selected, and the control condition was without MF. Finally, seven articles fit the inclusion criteria. The results imply that MF impairs the technical aspects of basketball (free throws, three-point shots, and total turnover) and the players' cognitive [take-the-first (TTF) heuristics and decision-making] performance, which results in athletes not using their techniques skillfully and being unable to make practical decisions during critical points in the game. In addition to that, the influences of MF on physical and tactical performance have not been studied. Further studies should look into comprehensive research on the influences of MF on basketball performance, especially on a player's physical and tactical performance.

Systematic Review Registration: [https://inplasy.com/] [INPLASY2021100017].

Introduction

Mental fatigue (MF) is a psychobiological state caused by prolonged, demanding cognitive activity, which has implicated many aspects of daily life. This condition causes an acute feeling of tiredness and a decreased cognitive ability (Boksem and Tops, 2008; Marcora et al., 2009; Van Cutsem et al., 2017). Along with the investigation of MF on cognitive performance, an increasing number of researchers focus on the potential impact MF has on one's physical performance. The most recent research aimed at understanding the phenomenon of MF found that it has a negative impact on athletic performance (Van Cutsem et al., 2017; Pageaux and Lepers, 2018; Habay et al., 2021), including endurance (cycling, running, yo-yo), motor skills (accuracy, speed, time taken to finish), and decision-making (errors, slower response time). MF harms the sports-specific psychomotor performances (SSPP) of different sports, including soccer, badminton, table tennis, basketball, and cricket (Habay et al., 2021). On the other hand, maximal force production (countermovement jumps, maximal voluntary contractions) was not affected by MF (Pageaux and Lepers, 2018).

According to the multiple resource model (Wickens, 1980), MF attracts peoples' attention as the secondary task which may simultaneously compete with the limited resource and potentially interfere with hazard recognition and take-over performance (Naujoks et al., 2017). In the higher demand condition, peoples' self-regulation mainly exhibited in the main task (Wandtner et al., 2016) and the adjustment in the interaction with the secondary task (Lin et al., 2019). On a control level, people regulate the current secondary task processing by disengagement with the secondary task if necessary, in which self-regulation was mainly manifested by peoples' attention allocation between main task and secondary tasks (Lin et al., 2019). As for sports area, sometimes, it is hard for athletes to perform to the best of their capabilities in fierce competitions, especially when success or failure has essential meaning. They have a higher cognitive burden to bear in those situations, which affects their performance (Nieuwenhuys and Oudejans, 2012). Studies suggest that the pressure of competition can induce anxiety and interfere with one's attention span, thus impairing one's athletic performance (Oudejans et al., 2011; Englert and Bertrams, 2012). Therefore, self-regulation was exerted to regulate their anxiety and better control their focus (Wilson et al., 2009; Englert and Bertrams, 2012; Baumeister et al., 2016; Englert, 2016), which can facilitate the execution of desired behaviors and task-relevant actions, thus leading to athletes being closer to their goal or highest standard of performance (Baumeister et al., 2016; Englert, 2016). However, exerting self-control may result in higher chances of failure in future efforts, called ego depletion, or MF (Baumeister et al., 2016).

Concerning basketball, people usually evaluate it by physical, technical, tactical and cognitive performance (Klusemann et al., 2013; Scanlan et al., 2014; Conte et al., 2018). Physical demands, such as stand-walk, jog, run, sprint, shuffle and jumps, are basic ability for playing basketball (Klusemann et al., 2013). Technical and tactical performance, such as the percentage of shot, rebounds, ball reversals and post entries are key factor between winning and losing teams (Conte et al., 2018). Cognitive components, such as perceptual and decision-making elements, influences basketball players' performance (Scanlan et al., 2014). Numerous studies have researched the influences of physical fatigue on players' performances and found that passing accuracy, ball speed and shooting technique significantly decrease when players are under the influences of physical fatigue compared to their non-fatigued states (Erculj and Supej, 2009; Li et al., 2021). However, these consequences are considered restricted to physical fatigue, and only a few articles have paid attention to the psychological aspect of basketball performance. During basketball games, players operate in high-intensity environments, which force them to conduct psychological operations to meet competition needs (Sighinolfi, 2020). Players who play at higher levels tend to have higher commitments, challenges, and confidence levels (Zarić et al., 2021). Therefore, due to basketball's cognitive and psychological demands, their cognitive engagement may cause MF.

An increasing number of articles have focused on the influences of MF in a variety of sports areas, including systematic reviews (Van Cutsem et al., 2017; Pageaux and Lepers, 2018; Kunrath et al., 2020; Habay et al., 2021). However, a systematic review investigating the influences of MF in the sport of basketball is lacking. Hence, this systematic review aims to identify the influences of MF on the physical, technical, cognitive, and tactical performance of basketball athletes.

Methods

This systematic review was done following the PRISMA (2009) guidelines (Kosa et al., 2018). This title has already been registered on International Platform of Registered Systematic Review and Meta-analysis Protocols, and the registration number is INPLASY2021100017. It involved three steps: (1) a search on existing literature (including the selection of search terms, databases, and inclusion criteria); (2) a screening based on title; (3) a screening based on the article's abstract. The search was conducted on the 18 May, 2021, and the following databases were used: PubMed, Web of Science, SPORTDiscus, Scopes, and China National Knowledge Infrastructure (CNKI). The search terms were (“mental fatigue” OR “cognitive fatigue” OR “mental effort” OR “cognitive effort” OR “mental exertion” OR “ego depletion” AND “basketball”). In each database, a search was conducted by title. In addition to that, the related reference lists in the included articles were screened. Finally, there is not the language limitation.

PICOS (population, intervention, comparison, outcome, study designs) criteria were used as the inclusion criteria, is presented in Table 1. Studies had to fulfill following inclusion criteria: (1) an evaluation of basketball-specific tests performed after the MF-inducing intervention was required, and, in the control group, the MF should not have been induced, or at least have triggered less MF than the intervention task; (2) the intervention was used to induce MF, and the sample population was comprised of basketball players; (3) measurements were implemented in a basketball-specific context (i.e., jump shots, dribbling, and passing); (4) the outcomes encompassed any form of basketball performance; and (5) randomized controlled trials (RCTs), non-randomized controlled trials (nRCTs), and non-randomized non-controlled trials (nRnCTs) had to be included. The data-collection process, based on PRISMA, is presented in Figure 1 (Tan et al., 2020).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Inclusion criteria according to the PICOS conditions.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Systematic review search and screening procedure.

The results (titles and/or abstracts) of studies retrieved using the search strategy and the titles and/or abstracts of studies from other sources will be independently screened by two review authors to identify studies that may meet the above inclusion criteria. The reviewers will review these studies according to the standard of population, intervention, comparison, outcome, and study design. The two review authors will extract data independently, and the differences will be determined and resolved through discussion (discuss with the third author if necessary).

“QualSyst” was used to assess the methodology quality (Kmet and Lee, 2004). It contained 14 items (see Table 2). The score was set according the degree to which the certain criteria were met (yes = 2, partial = 1, no = 0). “NA” was marked when the items did not apply to the study design and excluded from the total calculation of score. A score of ≥75% indicated strong quality, a score of 55–75% indicated moderate quality, and a score of ≤ 55% indicated weak quality.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Quality assessment “Qualsyst”.

Results

This study screened 999 articles, and 951 articles were remained after duplicates. After titles and abstracts were screened, 6 articles remained, and only 1 related article was found in screening the reference lists of the 6 articles (Figure 1). Overall, 7 articles were finally selected for the present systematical review. The result of quality assessment showed that 5 articles were of strong quality, 1 was moderate, and 1 was weak (Table 2). We did not exclude the article deemed to be of weak quality, because the number of articles in present systematic review was already limited.

General Study Characteristics

Table 3 shows information on the study characteristics, which based on follow aspects: (1) Sample size: In totally, the seven articles had 258 subjects. The sample sizes ranged from 18 (Bahrami et al., 2020) to 72 (Shaabani et al., 2020) participants, and the mean sample size was 36.9 participants (SD = 23.4). (2) Gender: Most samples only contained males, but one sample (Hepler and Kovacs, 2017) had both genders. The percentage of female was 13.2% and male was 86.8%. (3) Population classification: Five articles involved trained athletes (Englert et al., 2015; López et al., 2017; Moreira et al., 2018; Bahrami et al., 2020; Shaabani et al., 2020), but other two included undergraduate students (Hepler and Kovacs, 2017) and amateurs (Filipas et al., 2021) respectively.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Overview of the mental fatigue—inducing interventions.

Mental Fatigue-Inducing Interventions and Instruments

Two studies used a Stroop task to induce MF. The Stroop task was incongruent (Moreira et al., 2018; Shaabani et al., 2020) or combined with a Stroop software exercise involving math tests (Bahrami et al., 2020). Other studies used other forms of demanding cognitive tasks, such as watching a basketball tactical video (Filipas et al., 2021), N-Back tasks (López et al., 2017) and transcribing a neutral text with conditions (Englert et al., 2015). Hepler and Kovacs (2017) used mental serial subtraction to induce mental stress. Mental stress can induce anxiety and decrease attention. In those situations, self-control was exerted to regulate the pressure experienced and better focus their attention, which increases the possibility of self-control failure in the future, thus leading to ego depletion or MF (Shaabani et al., 2020). The duration of the intervention was different across studies, though most studies ranged from 6 to 120 mins (Englert et al., 2015; Moreira et al., 2018; Bahrami et al., 2020; Shaabani et al., 2020; Filipas et al., 2021). One article, however, only intervened for 30 s (Hepler and Kovacs, 2017), and another did not make mention of the duration (López et al., 2017).

The control conditions used by the included studies were varied. Englert et al. (2015) asked participants to transcribe the neutral text without any instructions. Moreira et al. (2018) used an easy cognitive task in which subjects sat in front of the computer screen for 10 mins and remained relaxed in the room for 20 mins. López et al. (2017) chose the oddball version of the control task. In this task, players had to press a button when a specific number was displayed onscreen. Hepler and Kovacs (2017) gave a two-digit number and asked participants to count backwards from it for 30 s in the control group. Shaabani et al. (2020) used a congruent-modified Stroop color-word task as the control condition. An overview of the MF-inducing interventions can be found in Table 3.

Several types of instruments were applied to test the effectiveness of reducing MF before and after cognitive tasks. First of all, five studies used one or more subjective manipulation checks: two studies used a visual analog scale (VAS) to assess MF and motivation (Bahrami et al., 2020; Filipas et al., 2021). Englert et al. (2015) applied a 4-item manipulation check to test whether the intervention induces the ego depletion. López et al. (2017) used the National Aeronautics and Space Administration-task load index (NASA-TLX) questionnaire to measure perceived effort and measure frustration workload score (0–100). Shaabani et al. (2020) used an ego-depletion manipulation check (EDMC) (four-item, 7-point Likert-type scale) to assess the depletion conditions between groups. All manipulation checks indicated an increase in MF in the experimental group. On the other hand, Moreira et al. (2018) used a behavioral manipulation check, in which reaction time and accuracy of the Stroop task were assessed. Finally, Hepler and Kovacs (2017) used a physiological manipulation check, in which a heart rate monitor (Polar H7—chest strap) was used to measure heart rate variability (HRV).

Effects of MF on Basketball Performance

Basketball performance is divided into physical, technical, cognitive, and tactical performance for this section (Klusemann et al., 2013; Scanlan et al., 2014; Conte et al., 2018). Table 4 gives an overview of the mental fatigue on basketball performance.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Overview of the mental fatigue on basketball performance.

Physical Performance

The articles selected for the present study did not involve any MF on physical performance in basketball.

Technical Performance

Regarding technical performance, the six articles used the players' efficiency, total turnover in small-sided-games (SSGs), three-point shots, and free throws to evaluate a player's technical performance. Moreira et al. (2018) mentions an unclear difference in player efficiency between the two groups, but the total turnover increased in the intervention group, compared to the control group. Bahrami et al. (2020) says there was a significant decrease in the scores of the three-point shots from pre-test to post-test in the experimental group, but there was no noticeable difference in the control group. It also states that there was a significant difference in the scores of the three-point shots between the two groups involved in the post-test. As for the influences of MF on free throws, Englert et al. (2015), López et al. (2017), Shaabani et al. (2020), and Filipas et al. (2021) found that the percentage of free throws was lower in the experimental group than in the control group.

Cognitive Performance

Decision-making and take-the-first (TTF) heuristics were used to evaluate cognitive performance. Hepler and Kovacs (2017) found no significant difference in TTF frequency, the number of options generated, the first optional quality, or the final decision quality between two groups, but there was a noticeable difference between the first option generation and the final decision speed.

Tactical Performance

The articles selected in the present study did not involve the effects of MF on tactical performance in basketball.

Discussion

In this study, we aim to sum the current extent of knowledge on the influences of MF on basketball players' performances. In order to achieve the aim, MF must first be successfully induced. Therefore, we reviewed the different methods that were used to attempt to cause MF. Overall, this review shows that MF can harm basketball players' performances in terms of efficiency, total turnover, free throws, take-the-first heuristics, decision-making, and three-point shots.

Mental Fatigue-Inducing Interventions

In this study, the seven articles analyzed used six different tasks to induce MF. Whether the MF was induced successfully or not is very important.

Moreira et al. (2018) and Shaabani et al. (2020) used incongruent-modified Stroop color-word tasks of 30 mins to induce mental stress. Stroop tasks are a common way of inducing MF. For instance, Filipas et al. (2019) and Weerakkody et al. (2020) also used them in their studies of football and cycling (Filipas et al., 2019; Weerakkody et al., 2020). Rauch and Schmitt (2009) showed that a 15 mins Stroop task with 50% congruent and incongruent trials could induce MF. Englert et al. (2015) required participants to transcribe a neutral German text from the computer screen on paper for 6 mins as fast as they could. The participants in the experimental group had to omit all letters “e” and “n,” which are the most common letters in German. They had to override their writing habits, so that much self-control was needed. Another study also proved that the method was successful in inducing MF (Englert et al., 2015). Filipas et al. (2021) asked participants to watch a basketball tactical video to induce MF, and the results showed a difference between the experimental and control group. Video watching is relevant because it is expected of basketball players to perform video analysis conferences before competitions (Filipas et al., 2021). Therefore, making the experimental session similar to the actual basketball match should be considered in future studies. Bahrami et al. (2020) used the Stroop software and math tests to induce MF, but the duration was for 120 mins. It has been proposed that the different durations and difficulties of the mentally exerting tasks might have different influences on people (Van Cutsem et al., 2017). Hagger et al. (2016) states that sufficient duration and intensity are essential in inducing fatigue. López et al. (2017) used an N-Back test to induce MF, but the duration is not stated in the study. N-Back tests are tasks of continuous processing, and are also a standard method of inducing MF. Tanaka et al. (2009) proved that a 30 mins 2-Back test can cause MF (Tanaka et al., 2009). Hepler and Kovacs (2017) used mental serial subtraction to cause MF. In this task, participants needed to count backwards by seven from a 4-digit number, and they needed to complete as many correct answers a possible. If they gave a wrong answer, they had to start over from the first number. Previous studies proved that similar mental arithmetic could induce MF (Diller et al., 2011).

In summary, Stroop tasks, N-back tests, transcribing a neutral text with conditions and mental serial subtraction are not basketball-specific tasks. However, these tests require critical cognitive skills to achieve a high level of performance. Basketball belongs to the category of open skills, which requires players to react in unpredictable and changing, externally paced environments (Coyne et al., 2018). Basketball as a sport has some unique qualities. For instance, unlike soccer, the restrictions of the court and rules result in a high number of accelerations and decelerations, and more high-intensity displacements for the players (Halouani et al., 2014; Hoffmann et al., 2014). In addition to that, players also have to perform more offense-defense conversions in competitions, which means they need to frequently combine a series of skills (e.g., screen, fast break) and tactics (e.g., screen, fast break) in competitions (Pino-Ortega et al., 2021). Basketball may require better visual attention, action execution, and decision-making skills (Overney et al., 2008; Yarrow et al., 2009; Nakata et al., 2010), based on which MF can manifest subjectively, behaviorally, and physiologically. Nevertheless, little information about behaviors (e.g., reaction time and accuracy) were made mention to in cognitive tasks (Moreira et al., 2018). Hence, cognitive tasks should be considered in greater detail in future studies.

Mental Fatigue and Basketball Performance

In order to discuss the subsequent basketball performance at a mentally fatigued state, a distinction was made between physical, technical, cognitive, and tactical performance.

Physical Performance

According to the cognitive load theory (Sweller, 1988), cognitive load refers to the used amount of working memory resources and heavy cognitive load has negative effects on task completion. Previous study reported that MF affected the information resources allocation of working memory, especially in the frontal and parietal regions which were closely related to working memory (Yang et al., 2021). Therefore, MF can increase the cognitive load leading to the reduction of performance.

In basketball competitions, physical performance includes stand-walks, jogging, running, sprinting, and low-, medium-, and high-intensity shuffles and jumps (Klusemann et al., 2013). The effects of MF on one's physical performance in basketball were not investigated. But Previous studies prove that MF impairs one's endurance, manifesting in increased completion time, decreased time before exhaustion, and self-selected power output/velocity (Van Cutsem et al., 2017). However, maximal strength, power, and anaerobic work are not affected by MF (Van Cutsem et al., 2017). Basketball combines aerobics and anaerobic exercises (Mancha-Triguero et al., 2020). Research shows that the average movement made by the world's best basketball centers is about 5,000 m per game, and the average movement of the world's best power forwards is about 6,000 m per game. An excellent attacking guard needs to move about 6,400 m to play a whole game. The moving distance of the China Basketball Association's (CBA's) leading players for a whole game range between 3,700 and 5,500 m, and the average moving distance per unit time per minute is 117–135 m per minute. There are significant differences among players in different positions, and the activity range of inside players is much smaller than those of outside players (Liu et al., 2012). Hence, whether the different positions the players play result in different degrees of MF should be investigated in the future.

Although the effects of MF on one's physical performance in basketball were not investigated in the chosen studies, other studies have proven its effects on the physical performance of other sports. For instance, Smith et al. (2015b) proved that the total distance covered on a treadmill, and shorter distances covered at lower speeds (Smith et al., 2015b), were decreased in mentally fatigued persons. Smith et al. (2015a) also verified that a yo-yo intermittent recovery test was performed at a decreased rate of 16.3% by mentally fatigued people (Smith et al., 2015a).

In conclusion, investigating whether MF impairs physical performance in basketball is necessary.

Technical Performance

In sports, technology refers to various processes, operation methods, and skills developed according to practical production experience and natural science principles. Basketball technology is a particular action done to achieve the goals of basketball, such as rebounds, assists, and scoring shots (Lan, 2001; Conte et al., 2018).

Six studies assessed the influences of MF on free throws, three-point shots, and total turnovers (Englert et al., 2015; Hepler and Kovacs, 2017; López et al., 2017; Moreira et al., 2018; Bahrami et al., 2020; Filipas et al., 2021). Those studies show the adverse influences of MF on technical basketball performance. The results show that the free throws and three-point shots in the experimental group were significantly lower than the control group, and Bahrami et al. (2020) added on to these results to state that there was no noticeable difference in the control group from pre-test to post-test. Comparatively, one article used small-sided-games (SSGs) to observe participants' technical performance (Moreira et al., 2018). SSGs are particularly relevant among the training methodologies used in basketball. SSGs can develop the physical, physiological, and technical-tactical aspects required in competitions (Klusemann et al., 2012; Delextrat and Martinez, 2013; Clemente, 2016). Moreira et al. (2018) recorded videos of its participants and used a formula to calculate the total turnovers of the two groups, thus proving that the total turnovers of participants in the experimental group were lower compared with control group.

From reviewing these articles, it was evident that none of the articles researched the effects of MF on technical performance in an official competition. To be specific, five articles researched the effects of MF on free throws and three-pointers during training (Englert et al., 2015; Hepler and Kovacs, 2017; López et al., 2017; Bahrami et al., 2020; Filipas et al., 2021), and one article researched it in SSGs (4 vs 4 in a court size of 28 × 15, in four sets of 2 mins and 30 s per set) (Moreira et al., 2018). Players have different mentalities and face different environments in training and in competition. Hence, the effects of MF on technical performance in official basketball competitions should be researched.

Cognitive Performance

Cognitive functions include a lot of basic mental operations, such as attention, memory, and executive functions involving working memory, decision-making, and multitasking (Lorenzo Calvo et al., 2021). Among these functions, attention is the one mainly defined as allocating cognitive resources to internal or external stimuli, which is key for sports performance (Furley and Wood, 2016).

Only one study assessed the effects of MF on TTF heuristics and decision-making outcomes (Shaabani et al., 2020). TTF heuristics are vital in sports (Johnson and Raab, 2003; Raab and Johnson, 2007). TTF refers to decisions made based on the first idea that springs to mind. As a result of the sequential order of option-generation, earlier options are better than options generated later in the process (Hepler and Kovacs, 2017). Hepler and Kovacs (2017) shows that MF does not affect the essential tenets of TTF. To be precise, in the study, participants were likely to choose their first choice in both experimental and control groups, and the number of options generated was identical. Therefore, MF did not affect the first option nor the subsequent options generated. As for decision outcomes, decision quality was not affected by MF, but the option-generation speed and final decision speed were significantly slower in the experimental group than in the control group. One study suggests that mental stress could inhibit reaction time (Van Gemmert and Galen, 1997).

Slimani et al. (2018) proved that MF had a negative effect on selection attention in concentration performances and increases the number of errors made (Slimani et al., 2018). When MF increased, brain activity gradually changed from negative to positive, which means that the inhibition of irrelevant information decreased the brain information system (Faber et al., 2012). Attention focus is a vital component of sports because there are many stimuli to which an athlete must attend. Attention can significantly impact performance when an athlete focuses (Milley and Ouellette, 2021). Therefore, researchers should research more into how MF influences cognitive performance in basketball.

Tactical Performance

Tactics refer to the “principles and methods of combat.” Basketball tactics are the principles and methods guiding individual skill and coordination among athletes in a basketball game. Usually, people divide tactics into offensive tactics (e.g., on-ball screen, off-ball screen) and defensive tactics (e.g., take the position, slide through) (Lan, 2001).

No articles have researched it in the present systematical review. However, a study proved that MF could impair tactical behaviors in soccer (Kunrath et al., 2020). Tactical performance has a crucial effect on basketball competitions. Winning teams have are more likely to have a higher number of ball reversals and post entries than losing teams (Conte et al., 2018), so investigating the influences of MF on tactical performance is vital.

To sum up the results, although the influences of MF on physical and tactical performance have not been researched, this systematic review shows that MF impairs technical and cognitive performance in basketball, which means that the percentage of shots will be decreased and the players' decision in competition will be affected when mentally fatigued. All these factors will likely lead to them losing the game.

Physical, technical, cognitive, and tactical performance are essential factors in basketball. Physical performance, such as running, sprinting, and shuffling, are the foundations on which a player operates their skills and tactics (McInnes et al., 1995), but the influences of MF on it in basketball have not been researched. On the other hand, players need to use unique techniques to execute strategies in basketball competition (Conte et al., 2015). In this regard, Basketball techniques, such as defensive rebounds and assists, are critical factors to win the game (Gomez et al., 2008). In this systematic review, although most of the reviewed articles researched the effects of MF on techniques, they did not do so during official competitions. Finally, cognitive performance, such as attention, anxiety, and motivation, can affect technical and tactical performance, which are also crucial in basketball competitions (Faber et al., 2012; Slimani et al., 2018; Milley and Ouellette, 2021), but only one of the six articles researched the influences of MF on cognitive performance.

In addition to studying the effects of MF on basketball performance, Moreira et al. (2018) also examined the effects of MF on salivary testosterone (T), cortisol (C), and alpha-amylase (sAA) responses. The increase of T and sAA concentrations were attenuated in the experimental group compared to the control group, but there was no change in C concentration between the two groups. The study suggests that the mentally fatigued state limits the increase of T and sAA. The T response might be relevant to the increase in errors during the small-sided-games (SSGs) (Boksem and Tops, 2008) and sAA is a biomarker for stress (Kivlighan and Granger, 2006; Silverman et al., 2010). C response is relevant to the effects of environmental and social-evaluative elements. In the cognitive task, or SSGs, these conditions were not present, which might be the reason that C concentration did not change between the two groups. On the other hand, Filipas et al. (2021) proved the negative effect a combination of MF and sleep deprivation has on technical performance for the first time (Filipas et al., 2021).

Finally, just one of the six articles mentioned a recovery strategy for MF (Shaabani et al., 2020). The study investigated mindfulness intervention effects (done for 15 mins) on mentally fatigued basketball players' free-throw performances. The participants in the control group listened to an audio-book segment on natural history, which was 15 mins long, and the results indicated that a brief mindfulness intervention could decrease the negative influence s of MF on basketball free-throws. Kabat-Zinn described mindfulness as “paying attention in a particular way, on purpose, in the present moment, and nonjudgmentally” (Sierpina, 2005). Mindfulness has been used in many sports to reduce mental stress. Reis Coimbra et al. (2021) proved that mindfulness interventions effectively attenuated MF in volleyball athletes.

Besides mindfulness training, there are other methods used for mental recovery in sports. For instance, Lorenzo Calvo et al. (2021) states that the intake of caffeine could improve attention (Lorenzo Calvo et al., 2021). Listening to self-selected music would be a suitable method of reducing MF in endurance performances (Lam and Phillips, 2019).

In conclusion, Shaabani et al. (2020) investigated mindfulness on free-throw tasks, but future studies should look at its effects on one's technical, physical, cognitive, and tactical performance (Shaabani et al., 2020). On the other hand, the recovery strategies used in basketball are limited, so further studies should also focus on finding more methods for basketball players to recover from MF.

Limitations

Firstly, this review only included published articles. Therefore, the results might be affected by publication bias. Furthermore, two of the seven articles selected for the present study combined MF with sleep restriction and neuroendocrine respectively, which might have interfered with the results. Finally, the lack of articles on physical and tactical performance limits the overall understanding of knowledge on the effects of MF on basketball performance.

Conclusion

By reviewing the results of seven published studies, the present study presents experimental evidence that most of the articles that investigated MF have concluded that it impairs technical performance (free throws, three-point shots) and cognitive performance (TTF heuristics and decision-making) in basketball. As a result of this, athletes may not perform to their utmost capability, which could lead to them losing the game. However, none of the articles investigated the effects of MF on physical and tactical performance in basketball. Hence, further studies should pay more attention to that. As the studies selected examined the influences of MF on basketball tests (e.g., free-throw tests and three-point shot tests), studies should be done on whether the influences of MF would be different in an actual competition. Finally, one article researched and proved a recovery strategy for impaired basketball performance, focusing only on free-throw performance. For the sake of helping athletes recover from MF, more methods should be investigated in the future.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

The literature search and selection of studies were performed by SC and SG. Following an initial screen of titles and abstracts by SC, full scrutiny of potentially eligible studies was independently screened by SC and SG using the specific inclusion criteria. SR arbitrated any disagreements in study inclusion. Study quality assessment was performed by SC. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Dr. Zubaidah Ibrahim for their assistance with the searching strategy.

References

Bahrami, A., Moradi, J., and Etaati, Z. (2020). The effect of mental fatigue on three-point shot performance in skilled basketball players. Int. J. Motor Control Learn. 2, 4–10. doi: 10.29252/ijmcl.2.4.4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., and Tice, D. M. (2016). The strength model of self-control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 351–355. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boksem, M. A., and Tops, M. (2008). Mental fatigue: costs and benefits. Brain Res. Rev. 59, 125–139. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clemente, F (2016). Small-sided and conditioned games in basketball training: a review. Strength Cond. J. 38, 49–58. doi: 10.1519/SSC.0000000000000225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conte, D., Favero, T., Lupo, C., Francioni, F. M., Capranica, L., and Tessitore, A. (2015). Time-motion analysis of italian elite women's basketball games: individual and team analyses. J. Strength Condition. Res. 29, 144–150. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0000000000000633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conte, D., Tessitore, A., Gjullin, A., Mackinnon, D., Lupo, C., and Favero, T. (2018). Investigating the game-related statistics and tactical profile in NCAA division I men's basketball games. Biol. Sport 35:137. doi: 10.5114/biolsport.2018.71602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coyne, J. O. C., Gregory Haff, G., Coutts, A. J., Newton, R. U., and Nimphius, S. (2018). The current state of subjective training load monitoring-a practical perspective and call to action. Sports Med. Open 4:58. doi: 10.1186/s40798-018-0172-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delextrat, A., and Martinez, A. (2013). Small-sided game training improves aerobic capacity and technical skills in basketball players. Int. J. Sports Med. 35, 385–391. doi: 10.1055/s-0033-1349107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diller, J. W., Patros, C. H., and Prentice, P. R. (2011). Temporal discounting and heart rate reactivity to stress. Behav. Processes 87, 306–309. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2011.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Englert, C (2016). The strength model of self-control in sport and exercise psychology. Front. Psychol. 7:314. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Englert, C., and Bertrams, A. (2012). Anxiety, ego depletion, and sports performance. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 34, 580–599. doi: 10.1123/jsep.34.5.580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Englert, C., Bertrams, A., Furley, P., and Oudejans, R. R. D. (2015). Is ego depletion associated with increased distractibility? Results from a basketball free throw task. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 18, 26–31. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erculj, F., and Supej, M. (2009). Impact of fatigue on the position of the release arm and shoulder girdle over a longer shooting distance for an elite basketball player. J. Strength Condition. Res. 23, 1029–1036. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0b013e3181a07a27

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faber, L. G., Maurits, N. M., and Lorist, M. M. (2012). Mental fatigue affects visual selective attention. PLoS ONE 7:e48073. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filipas, L., Ferioli, D., Banfi, G., La Torre, A., and Vitale, J. A. (2021). Single and combined effect of acute sleep restriction and mental fatigue on basketball free-throw performance. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 16, 415–420. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.2020-0142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filipas, L., Gallo, G., Pollastri, L., and La Torre, A. (2019). Mental fatigue impairs time trial performance in sub-elite under 23 cyclists. PLoS ONE 14:e0218405. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0218405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furley, P., and Wood, G. (2016). Working memory, attentional control, and expertise in sports: a review of current literature and directions for future research. J. Appl. Res. Memory Cogn. 5, 415–425. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomez, M. A., Lorenzo, A., Barakat, R., Ortega, E., and Palao, J. M. (2008). Differences in game-related statistics of basketball performance by game location for men's winning and losing teams. Percept. Mot. Skills 106, 43–50. doi: 10.2466/pms.106.1.43-50

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Habay, J., Van Cutsem, J., Verschueren, J., De Bock, S., Proost, M., De Wachter, J., et al. (2021). Mental fatigue and sport-specific psychomotor performance: a systematic review. Sports Med. 51, 1527–1548. doi: 10.1007/s40279-021-01429-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Alberts, H., Anggono, C. O., Batailler, C., Birt, A. R., et al. (2016). A Multilab Preregistered Replication of the Ego-Depletion Effect. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 11, 546–573. doi: 10.1177/1745691616652873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halouani, J., Chtourou, H., Gabbett, T., Chaouachi, A., and Chamari, K. (2014). Small-sided games in team sports training: a brief review. J. Strength Cond. Res. 28, 3594–3618. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0000000000000564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hepler, T. J., and Kovacs, A. J. (2017). Influence of acute stress on decision outcomes and heuristics. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness 57, 305–312. doi: 10.23736/S0022-4707.16.06554-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffmann, J. J. Jr., Reed, J. P., Leiting, K., Chiang, C. Y., and Stone, M. H. (2014). Repeated sprints, high-intensity interval training, small-sided games: theory and application to field sports. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 9, 352–357. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.2013-0189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, J. G., and Raab, M. (2003). Take the first: option-generation and resulting choices. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 91, 215–229. doi: 10.1016/s0749-5978(03)00027-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kivlighan, K. T., and Granger, D. A. (2006). Salivary alpha-amylase response to competition: relation to gender, previous experience, and attitudes. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 703–714. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klusemann, M. J., Pyne, D. B., Foster, C., and Drinkwater, E. J. (2012). Optimising technical skills and physical loading in small-sided basketball games. J. Sports Sci. 30, 1463–1471. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2012.712714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klusemann, M. J., Pyne, D. B., Hopkins, W. G., and Drinkwater, E. J. (2013). Activity profiles and demands of seasonal and tournament basketball competition. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 8, 623–629. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.8.6.623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kmet, L., and Lee, R. (2004). Standard quality assessment criteria for evaluating primary research papers from a variety of fields: AHFMRHTA Initiative 20040213. HTA Initiative 2:R37M04F16. doi: 10.7939/R37M04F16

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosa, D., Monize, J., D'Souza, M., Joshi, A., Philip, K., Reza, S., et al. (2018). PRISMA 2009 Checklist. Amsterdam: Elseiver.

Google Scholar

Kunrath, C. A., Cardoso, F. D. S. L., Calvo, T. G., and da Costa, I. T. (2020). Mental fatigue in soccer: a systematic review. Rev. Bras. Med. Esporte 26, 172–178. doi: 10.1590/1517-869220202602208206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, H. K. N., and Phillips, S. (2019). “The effects of music on mental fatigue and endurance performance,” in The Edinburgh Sport and Exercise Medicine Conference.

Google Scholar

Lan, X (2001). The discussion on the tactical system of basketball. China Sport Sci. Technol. 12, 16–18. doi: 10.16470/j.csst.2001.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, F., Knjaz, D., and Rupcic, T. (2021). Influence of fatigue on some kinematic parameters of basketball passing. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:700. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18020700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, R., Liu, N., Ma, L., Zhang, T., and Zhang, W. (2019). Exploring the self-regulation of secondary task engagement in the context of partially automated driving: a pilot study. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 64, 147–160. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2019.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Cheng, L., and Jianhua, X. (2012). Inspirations from the research results of basketball game load characteristics to stamina training. J. Phys. Educ. 19, 108–122. doi: 10.16237/j.cnki.cn44-1404/g8.2012.05.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López, F., Ureña, N., and Vélez, D. (2017). La fatiga mental deteriora el rendimiento en el tiro libre en baloncesto. 26, 33–36.

Google Scholar

Lorenzo Calvo, J., Fei, X., Dominguez, R., and Pareja-Galeano, H. (2021). Caffeine and cognitive functions in sports: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutrients 13:868. doi: 10.3390/nu13030868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mancha-Triguero, D., Garcia-Rubio, J., Antunez, A., and Ibanez, S. J. (2020). Physical and physiological profiles of aerobic and anaerobic capacities in young basketball players. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:1409. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17041409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcora, S. M., Staiano, W., and Manning, V. (2009). Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. J Appl. Physiol. 106, 857–864. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.91324.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McInnes, S. E., Carlson, J. S., Jones, C. J., and McKenna, M. J. (1995). The physiological load imposed on basketball players during competition. J. Sports Sci. 13, 387–397. doi: 10.1080/02640419508732254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milley, K. R., and Ouellette, G. P. (2021). Putting attention on the spot in coaching: shifting to an external focus of attention with imagery techniques to improve basketball free-throw shooting performance. Front. Psychol. 12:645676. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.645676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreira, A., Aoki, M. S., Franchini, E., da Silva Machado, D. G., Paludo, A. C., and Okano, A. H. (2018). Mental fatigue impairs technical performance and alters neuroendocrine and autonomic responses in elite young basketball players. Physiol. Behav. 196, 112–118. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakata, H., Yoshie, M., Miura, A., and Kudo, K. (2010). Characteristics of the athletes' brain: evidence from neurophysiology and neuroimaging. Brain Res. Rev. 62, 197–211. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2009.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naujoks, F., Befelein, D., Wiedemann, K., and Neukum, A. (2017). A review of non-driving-related tasks used in studies on automated driving. Adv. Intell. Syst. Comput. 597, 525–537. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-60441-1_52

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieuwenhuys, A., and Oudejans, R. R. (2012). Anxiety and perceptual-motor performance: toward an integrated model of concepts, mechanisms, and processes. Psychol. Res. 76, 747–759. doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0384-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oudejans, R. R., Kuijpers, W., Kooijman, C. C., and Bakker, F. C. (2011). Thoughts and attention of athletes under pressure: skill-focus or performance worries? Anxiety Stress Coping 24, 59–73. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2010.481331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Overney, L. S., Blanke, O., and Herzog, M. H. (2008). Enhanced temporal but not attentional processing in expert tennis players. PLoS ONE 3:e2380. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pageaux, B., and Lepers, R. (2018). The effects of mental fatigue on sport-related performance. Prog. Brain Res. 240, 291–315. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pino-Ortega, J., Rojas-Valverde, D., Gomez-Carmona, C. D., and Rico-Gonzalez, M. (2021). Training design, performance analysis, and talent identification-a systematic review about the most relevant variables through the principal component analysis in soccer, basketball, and rugby. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:2642. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18052642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raab, M., and Johnson, J. G. (2007). Expertise-based differences in search and option-generation strategies. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 13, 158–170. doi: 10.1037/1076-898x.13.3.158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rauch, W., and Schmitt, K. (2009). Fatigue of Cognitive Control in the Stroop-Task. Proc. Annu. Conf. Meet. Sci. Soc. 31.

Google Scholar

Reis Coimbra, D., Bevilacqua, G., Pereira, F., and Andrade, A. (2021). Effect of Mindfulness Training on Fatigue and Recovery in Elite Volleyball Athletes: A Randomized Controlled Follow-Up Study. J. Sports Sci. Med. 20, 1–8. doi: 10.52082/jssm.2021.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scanlan, A., Humphries, B., Tucker, P. S., and Dalbo, V. (2014). The influence of physical and cognitive factors on reactive agility performance in men basketball players. J. Sports Sci. 32, 367–374. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2013.825730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaabani, F., Naderi, A., Borella, E., and Calmeiro, L. (2020). Does a brief mindfulness intervention counteract the detrimental effects of ego depletion in basketball free throw under pressure? Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 9, 197–215. doi: 10.1037/spy0000201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sierpina, V. S (2005). Coming to our senses: healing ourselves and the world through mindfulness. Explore. 1:320. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2005.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sighinolfi, L (2020). Sport psychology in basketball: performance under pressure. Basketball Sports Med. Sci. 2020, 983–994. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-61070-1_78

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silverman, M. N., Heim, C. M., Nater, U. M., Marques, A. H., and Sternberg, E. M. (2010). Neuroendocrine and immune contributors to fatigue. PMR 2, 338–346. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2010.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slimani, M., Znazen, H., Bragazzi, N. L., Zguira, M. S., and Tod, D. (2018). The effect of mental fatigue on cognitive and aerobic performance in adolescent active endurance athletes: insights from a randomized counterbalanced, cross-over trial. J. Clin. Med. 7:510. doi: 10.3390/jcm7120510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, M., Coutts, A., Merlini, M., Deprez, D., Lenoir, M., and Marcora, S. (2015a). Mental fatigue impairs soccer-specific physical and technical performance. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 267–276. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, M., Marcora, S., and Coutts, A. (2015b). Mental fatigue impairs intermittent running performance. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 47, 1682–1690. doi: 10.1249/mss.0000000000000592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweller, J (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: effects on learning. Cogn. Sci. 12, 257–285. doi: 10.1016/0364-0213(88)90023-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, R., Lam, W. C., Yao, L., Wang, X., Cheng, C., Liu, F., et al. (2020). PRISMA (Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses) extension for Chinese herbal medicines 2020 (PRISMA-CHM 2020). Am. J. Chin. Med. 48, 1–35. doi: 10.1142/S0192415X20500639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, M., Mizuno, K., Tajima, S., Sasabe, T., and Watanabe, Y. (2009). Central nervous system fatigue alters autonomic nerve activity. Life Sci. 84, 235–239. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2008.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Cutsem, J., Marcora, S., De Pauw, K., Bailey, S., Meeusen, R., and Roelands, B. (2017). The effects of mental fatigue on physical performance: a systematic review. Sports Med. 47, 1569–1588. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0672-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Gemmert, A., and Galen, G. (1997). Stress, neuromotor noise, and human performance: a theoretical perspective. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 23, 1299–1313. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.23.5.1299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wandtner, B., Schumacher, M., and Schmidt, E. (2016). The role of self-regulation in the context of driver distraction: a simulator study. Traffic Inj. Prev. 17, 1–8. doi: 10.1080/15389588.2015.1102231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weerakkody, N. S., Taylor, C. J., Bulmer, C. L., Hamilton, D. B., Gloury, J., O'Brien, N. J., et al. (2020). The effect of mental fatigue on the performance of Australian football specific skills amongst amateur athletes. J. Sci. Med. Sport 24, 592–596. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2020.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickens, C (1980). “The structure of attentional resources,” in Attention and Performance VIII, eds R. S. Nickerson (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 239–257

Google Scholar

Wilson, M., Vine, S., and Wood, G. (2009). The influence of anxiety on visual attentional control in basketball free throw shooting. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 31, 152–168. doi: 10.1123/jsep.31.2.152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, S., Wang, Z., Wang, L., Shi, B., and Peng, S. (2021). Research on the influence of mental fatigue on information resources allocation of working memory. Sheng Wu Yi Xue Gong Cheng Xue Za Zhi 38, 671–677. doi: 10.7507/1001-5515.202007050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yarrow, K., Brown, P., and Krakauer, J. (2009). Inside the brain of an elite athlete: the neural processes that support high achievement in sports. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 585–596. doi: 10.1038/nrn2672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zarić, I., Milošević, M., Kukić, F., Dopsaj, M., Aminova, A. S., and Komkova, I. A. (2021). Association of mental toughness with competitive success of young female basketball players. Human Sport Med. 21, 86–93. doi: 10.14529/hsm210111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mental fatigue, sports, athletic performance, recovery, basketball

Citation: Cao S, Geok SK, Roslan S, Sun H, Lam SK and Qian S (2022) Mental Fatigue and Basketball Performance: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 12:819081. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.819081

Received: 22 November 2021; Accepted: 16 December 2021;
Published: 10 January 2022.

Edited by:

Donatella Di Corrado, Kore University of Enna, Italy

Reviewed by:

Teresa Bento, Polytechnic Institute of Santarém, Portugal
Masoud Mirmoezzi, Islamic Azad University Central Tehran Branch, Iran
Milos Milosevic, Singidunum University, Serbia

Copyright © 2022 Cao, Geok, Roslan, Sun, Lam and Qian. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shudian Cao, gs58918@student.upm.edu.my; Soh Kim Geok, kims@upm.edu.my

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.