Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 11 October 2022
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Intersubjectivity: Recent Advances in Theory, Research, and Practice View all 14 articles

Skin-to-skin SDF positioning: The key to intersubjective intimacy between mother and very preterm newborn—A pilot matched-pair case-control study

  • 1Laboratoire de Psychopathologie et Processus de Santé (LPPS UR 4057), Université Paris Cité, Paris, France
  • 2NICU Service de réanimation néonatale, Hospital Center Intercommunal De Créteil, Créteil, France
  • 3Faculté de santé - Université Paris Est Créteil, Créteil, France
  • 4Université Paris Nanterre, Nanterre, France
  • 5Service de pédopsychiatrie universitaire, Hospital Group Du Havre, Le Havre, France

Background: Skin-to-skin contact (SSC) has been widely studied in NICU and several meta-analyses have looked at its benefits, for both the baby and the parent. However, very few studies have investigated SSC’ benefits for communication, in particular in the very-preterm newborn immediately after birth.

Aims: To investigate the immediate benefits of Supported Diagonal Flexion (SDF) positioning during SSC on the quality of mother—very-preterm newborn communication and to examine the coordination of the timing of communicative behaviors, just a few days after birth.

Subjects and study design: Monocentric prospective matched-pair case-control study. Thirty-four mothers and their very preterm infants (27 to 31 + 6 weeks GA, mean age at birth: 30 weeks GA) were assigned to one of the two SSC positioning, either the Vertical Control positioning (n = 17) or the SDF Intervention positioning (n = 17). Mother and newborn were filmed during the first 5 min of their first SSC.

Outcome measures: Infants’ states of consciousness according to the Assessment of Preterm Infants’ Behavior scale (APIB). Onset and duration of newborns’ and mothers’ vocalizations and their temporal proximity within a 1-s time-window.

Results: In comparison with the Vertical group, very preterm newborns in the SDF Intervention Group spent less time in a drowsy state and more in deep sleep. At 3.5 days of life, newborns’ vocal production in SSC did not differ significantly between the two groups. Mothers offered a denser vocal envelope in the SDF group than in the Vertical group and their vocalizations were on average significantly longer. Moreover, in a one-second time-frame, temporal proximity of mother-very preterm newborn behaviors was greater in the SDF Intervention Group.

Conclusion: Although conducted on a limited number of dyads, our study shows that SDF positioning fosters mother-very preterm newborn intimate encounter during the very first skin to skin contact after delivery. Our pioneer data sheds light on the way a mother and her very preterm vocally meet, and constitutes a pilot step in the exploration of innate intersubjectivity in the context of very preterm birth.

Introduction

In the 1970s, scientists brought evidence that, before they actually begin to speak, newborns, as well as young infants, experience intersubjective awareness. Their thoughts develop through the sympathetic and timely engagement with the expressive behaviors of a sensitive parent (Bruner, 1977; Trevarthen, 1977; Bateson, 1979; Brazelton, 1979; Brazelton et al., 1979). The centrality of interpersonal synchrony was highlighted through pioneer studies using videotape recordings of mother–infant interactions (Trevarthen, 1977; Tronick et al., 1977; Feldman et al., 1999; Jaffe et al., 2001). The present study falls within the psychobiological theory of intersubjectivity (Trevarthen, 2012, 2016). The theory of innate intersubjectivity claims that “a child is born with motives to find and use the motives of other persons in ‘conversational’ negotiation of purposes, emotions, experiences, and meaning. The efficiency of sympathetic engagement between persons signals the ability of each to ‘model’ or ‘mirror’ the motivations and purposes of companions, immediately” [Trevarthen, 1998 (in Braten) p. 16]. In this perspective, achieving a conversation at birth rests on an efficient mobilization of the neonate’s internal resources (self-regulation), as well as on the efficient sympathetic regulation of a partner. The theory of innate intersubjectivity considers newborn and partner as an intrinsic unit, the dyad. It is especially activated during intimate dyadic free play. In such face-to-face, eye-to-eye intersubjective emotional communications, infant and mother intently look at and listen to each other. Thereby, they synchronize and regulate each other’s emotional states. During these protoconversations, the partners’ emotions are expressed and actively perceived in spontaneous, reciprocal, and rhythmic-turn-taking interactions (Trevarthen, 1993). In this perspective, the dyad becomes the place where they meet, as well as where they miss each other. Every time they meet or miss each other results from an intelligent combination of their ability to perceive each other’s motives. In her tribute to Stern, Beebe (2017, p. 234) stated that “the meaning of the behavior is co-created,” recalling that the way communication is bidirectionally regulated is specific to a particular dyad. Feldman et al. (2011) developed a similar neurophysiological model, including physiological synchrony from birth: when mother and child are physically close, they synchronize their breathing and heart rate. This provides a basis for later “social synchrony,” in which partners meet and miss one another and respond during shared interactions. But sometimes birth comes earlier than expected and requires hospitalization, thereby separating mother and child and medicalizing their first relations. The present study investigates the essential question of how and to what extent the quality of communication can be supported from the start in the context of very preterm birth.

Before term birth

For the fetus, pregnancy is a time for chemosensory learning, which helps him adapt to his new after-birth environment (Lickliter, 2000). The uterine environment provides plentiful multisensory stimulations. The fetus can experience vestibular, somaesthetic, auditory, chemical and olfactory stimulations, simultaneously or sequentially (Pinéda et al., 2017a).

In utero, the fetus moves intentionally, with controlled timing and sequencing, anticipating sensory confirmation of self-related effects (Piontelli, 2010). The fetus is bathed in an environment of vestibular, somatosensory, tactile, and auditory rhythmic stimuli, through the mother’s breathing, heartbeats, walking, dancing, running, speaking, singing, etc. (Provasi et al., 2021). After 20 weeks, some fetuses make self-touching gestures with their left hand, which may signal sympathetic emotional attitudes with a mother’s feelings of stress, and respond positively to her voice (Reissland and Kisilevsky, 2015). Various sounds are present in the uterus, with frequencies and intensities that are low yet detectable by the fetus (Philbin, 2017). When the mother speaks or sings, sound travels through the inside but also through the air, enabling the fetus to perceive her voice from both internal and external sources (Pino, 2016). Fetuses are able to clearly identify the mother’s voice, and could shape mouth movements when they hear someone speaking (Marx and Nagy, 2015). Toward the end of pregnancy, fetuses are particularly attuned to maternal acoustic cues (Ferrari et al., 2016) and they are able to detect, recognize, respond, and remember some characteristics of her voice (Kisilevsky et al., 2003; Voegtline et al., 2013).

Term birth

Babies are equipped to perceive the world they are born into. Newborns have a holistic perception of others: they perceive faces, voices, body movements, tactile stimuli, smells, and shapes. Evidence suggests that the mother’s voice is one of the major drivers of the intricate intersensory and intermodal connections that are formed and retained in memory in the days following birth (Gratier and Devouche, 2017). The mother’s voice is a salient, consistent and frequent stimulus that provides before-after birth continuity, regardless of the drastic changes linked to the exit of the uterine environment (Lickliter, 2000). From that perspective, the full-term newborn experiences a normal biological continuum between intra- and extra-uterine lives. At birth, the newborn is exposed to multimodal stimulations that form intermodal redundancies (joint and repeated apprehension of the environment with different sensory modalities; Provasi et al., 2014). This gives meaning to the newborn’s environment, making his/her ecological niche. Significant biological stimulations originating from the mother, notably on the olfactory (Varendi et al., 1997) and auditory levels (Doheny et al., 2012), lead to an improvement of physiological and behavioral stability and better adapted responses.

A parent, or any caring elder, can pick up on the infant’s skills to regulate him- or herself as well as the partner, thus enabling the development of a new and unique relationship of intimate familiarity and responsiveness (Brazelton et al., 1979; Trevarthen, 2011; Devouche and Gratier, 2019). In this context, Dominguez et al. (2016) and Boiteau et al. (2021) highlighted a tight coordination in the timing of vocalization between 2 to 4-day-old neonates and their parent. In both studies, neonates demonstrated control over the timing of vocalizations produced with an attentive and affectionate partner, with a 1-s temporal window explaining most baby-parent vocal contingencies. However, to date, no study has explored temporal coordination between the newborn and a partner in the context of preterm birth.

Preterm birth

Preterms have been denied part of their prenatal chemosensory learning time, and deprived of the typical biological continuum between pre- and postnatal life. In addition, after birth, they have to face the incubator’s harsh environment that combines sensory deprivations, over-stimulations and inappropriate stimulations (Lickliter, 2000), which do not match their pre-organized sensory expectations. Preterm babies receive many sensory stimulations that are not adapted to their sensory maturation level, including more and higher levels of auditory stimulations (machine noises and rings, voices that are transformed and amplified by the incubator’s walls) and reduced vestibular stimulation, at a time when they should be benefiting from filtered auditory stimulations, and regular vestibular stimulations provided by the mother’s movements (Lickliter, 2000). They are also likely to be subjected to visual over-stimulations (bright lights) sometimes necessary in highly technical care. Furthermore, they are confronted to many unpleasant (antiseptic agents) but very few familiar or maternal smells (Marlier and Schaal, 2005). The preterm newborn is thus exposed to early sensory experiences that are atypical, both in quality and quantity, and that occur during the most critical period for the nervous system’s development (Pinéda et al., 2017b; Montagna and Nosarti, 2016).

At birth, preterms’ motor and interactional skills are fragile, which makes it is more difficult for them than for healthy term babies, to initiate an exchange and respond actively (Muller-Nix et al., 2004). However, they can discriminate between two stimuli, associate events, and become habituated to external stimulations. The preterm infant does receive environmental input and plays an active part in the interactive system (Provasi, 2019). Nevertheless, interactions between mothers and preterm children often involve poorer maternal adaptation to the infant’s signals, leading to less maternal touch, as well as fewer vocalizations and gazes (Feldman, 2006; Forcada-Guex et al., 2011).

Preterm birth is associated with high stress and anxiety levels in parents (Segre et al., 2014), including post-traumatic stress (Forcada-Guex et al., 2011; Anderson and Cacola, 2017). A correlation has been evidenced between maternal and paternal stress, as well as between parental stress and the quality of father–mother–infant interactions in premature (Gatta et al., 2017). In the meta-analysis of Grote et al. (2010), 39% of mothers with a preterm baby presented postpartum depressive (PPD) symptoms. De Paula Eduardo et al.’s (2019) systematic review analyses the studies that explored preterm birth as a risk factor for postpartum depression in the last 10 years, and provides evidence of higher risk for PPD among mothers of preterm infants up to 24 weeks after childbirth. Stressful or traumatizing events have been shown to slow down oxytocin release, which inhibits the mother’s empathetic abilities (Feldman, 2015). Indeed, mothers of preterm infants provide a less responsive and stimulating environment than mothers of full-term infants (Muller-Nix et al., 2004). Preterm birth could permanently disturb the interactional sphere (Zelkowitz et al., 2009). The analysis of preterms’ neurological and behavioral disturbances highlights the entanglement between biological vulnerability (brain alteration) and environmental factors, such as stress, perinatal pain, and break of the emotional connexion with the mother (Montagna and Nosarti, 2016).

Pierrat et al. (2021, the EPIPAGE 2 study) showed that at age 5, behavioral disturbances were parents’ most frequent preoccupation in France. This study also showed that the level of prematurity is crucial to neurodevelopment: moderate or severe developmental difficulties observed in 27% of extreme preterm children, 19% of very preterm children, and 12% of moderately preterm children. Irrespective of the degree of prematurity, 35% of preterms require adapted care to prevent difficulties from impacting the child’s daily life and learning. More precisely: 50% of extreme preterms, 1/3 of very preterm children and ¼ of moderately preterm children received support in their development (speech therapy, psychomotor education, psychological support …). In the OLIMPE study, an ancillary study to EPIPAGE 2, Cambonie et al. (2017) recorded disorganized interactive behaviors on discharge from the maternity ward (50% for mothers, 33% for preterm babies), which persisted at 6 months (32% for mothers and 26% for preterms).

Skin-to-skin contact

Skin-to-skin contact is a consistent and reciprocal interaction, which is entirely dedicated to the parent-infant relationship. It is performed routinely around the world, starting from birth, as part of intensive care in neonatal units (Nyqvist et al., 2010). Skin-to-skin contact is offered during hospitalization, as part of family-centered care programs, as a support of the subjective experience of parenthood (Roué et al., 2017). This natural relational opportunity helps parents develop a sense of responsibility that is often on hold while the infant is in medical care. Skin-to-skin contact, along with infant-directed singing, allows mother and child to synchronize rhythmically and provides an envelope that enables the tuning of different rhythmic stimulations (Markova et al., 2019). Supporting mutual synchrony through SCC is in line with Thomson et al.’s (2013) recommendation to include some sense of coherence into the neonatal environment, by giving parents a central role in the decision-making process and supporting their engagement to care for their child.

In the specific context of a preterm birth, SSC has immediate physiological and neurobehavioral benefits (Feldman and Eidelman, 2003): it facilitates the vulnerable newborn’s adaptation to extra-uterine life (Kristoffersen et al., 2016). Longer-term benefits include better psychomotor (Feldman et al., 2002) and cognitive development (Charpak et al., 2005; Feldman et al., 2014). Mothers and fathers of preterm newborns experience an increase of oxytocin release and a reduction in cortisol and stress responses during SSC, as well as facilitated breastfeeding (Cong et al., 2015; Mörelius et al., 2015). Mothers’ psychological benefits include reduced stress and postpartum depression risk (Athanasopoulou and Fox, 2014; Zhao and Zhang, 2020). Additional benefits have been evidenced on parents’ affective and interactive behavior at corrected term, as well as on the quality of their attachment (Tallandini and Scalembra, 2006; Moore et al., 2016; Feldman et al., 2014). Furthermore, SSC may promote parental presence, even in poor reception conditions (Raiskila et al., 2017).

Indeed, SSC has been widely studied (Charpak et al., 2005) and several meta-analyses or meta-syntheses have investigated its implementation (Seidman et al., 2015; Chan et al., 2016) or its benefits (Moore et al., 2016; Lawn et al., 2010; Mori et al., 2010; Anderzen-Carlsson et al., 2014; Johnston et al., 2017; Conde-Agudelo et al., 2011).

There has been a real paradigm shift toward family-centered care (Franck and O'Brien, 2019; Skene et al., 2019). In their paper presenting eight principles for patient-centered and family-centered care for newborns in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU), Roué et al. (2017) listed the main benefits of SSC, however omitting to mention potential benefits for communication. Studies investigating this aspect are scarce. In their systematic review on the effects of early communication intervention on the speech and communication skills of preterm infants in the NICU, Harding et al. (2019) identified five studies, all conducted on very or extremely preterm babies, with outcome measures collected 1 month after birth (one study) and at 3 months CA or more (four other studies). Importantly, four of these studies correlated infant and mother measures but none actually focused on communication during SSC. Only Caskey et al. (2014) investigated the timing and coordination of parent-preemie communications, through a turn-taking measure computed by the LENA system, with a 5-s time window of temporal contingency. Authors considered any block that contains both infant and adult speech as conversational and found that infants responded preferentially and more frequently to their mothers compared with their fathers. However, their measure of coordination timing remained less precise than the one used by Dominguez et al. (2016) and Boiteau et al. (2021).

The search terms used for the Harding et al. (2019) review excluded several studies, such as Tallandini and Scalembra (2006), Feldman et al. (2014), or Buil et al. (2016) that nonetheless focused on mother-preemie communication. The study conducted by Buil et al. (2020) precisely aimed to investigate how communication could be improved during SSC, by modifying SSC positioning. Considering that mothers often complained about the impossibility to look at their baby’s face, because of the vertical positioning of their child on their chest (leading them to sometimes prefer an arm-holding cuddle), authors investigated how an innovative kangaroo Supported Diagonal Flexion (SDF) positioning influenced mother and infant well-being and communication during SSC, while maintaining a safe and preventive preterm posture (Buil et al., 2016). This positioning was developed in a high-tech NICU according to recent experts’ recommendations for promoting a “semi reclined positioning” (Ludington-Hoe et al., 2008; Nyqvist et al., 2010). SDF positioning was also found to reduce the risk of postnatal maternal depression and to promote and prolong SSC sessions (Buil et al., 2019). Moreover, Buil et al. (2020) tested the kangaroo SDF positioning 18 days after a very preterm birth and showed that SDF positioning improves the mother’s ability to perceive her infant’s behaviors such as vocalizations, smiles or eyes openings, as well as to respond in a timely manner. Since its creation, other teams have investigated potential benefits of skin to skin in SDF position, either looking at “the influence of a skin-to-skin lullaby on the stability of maternal behavior and on the tonic emotional manifestations of the preterm infant” (Provasi et al., 2021) or at the “vocal responsiveness of preterm infants to maternal infant-directed speaking and singing during skin-to-skin contact (Kangaroo Care) in the NICU” (Carvalho et al., 2019). However, in these studies, observations were not made immediately after birth. Therefore, the essential question of how and to what extent SDF positioning enhances the quality of communication from the start is yet to be examined. In this direction, our team has previously explored touch and maternal vocal behavior during the first skin to skin in a sample of our population. In this position, mothers display more active, securing and affectionate touch, favoring a quality early reunion, free from over-stimulations, as reflected by drowsiness and less agitation in the baby (Buil et al., 2017a). Moreover, from the very first minutes of skin to skin, mothers in SDF provide a denser and more musical sound envelope (Buil et al., 2017b).

Given our current knowledge on the effects of temporal coordination during communication on infant development, particularly in the neonatal period and infancy, the question of mother-preterm communication is decisive. The first minutes of the very first skin to skin are the physical and emotional reunions of mother and child after birth in a potential traumatic context. Nonetheless, current literature regarding the first skin to skin in the NICU focuses mainly on its secure feasibility (Linnér et al., 2020), the moment of and impact on its implementation during hospitalization (Mörelius et al., 2012; Blomqvist et al., 2013), its physiological benefits for the preterm (Cadwell et al., 2018; Gere et al., 2021; Pandya et al., 2021) or parents’ feelings (Maastrup et al., 2018). The data collected in this pioneering study shed light on what actually happens between mothers and their babies born prematurely during their first skin to skin.

The present study aimed to better characterize the vocal meeting of the mother and her very preterm newborn, during the first minutes of their first ever skin to skin. We also further examined the immediate benefits of SDF positioning during the first SSC on the quality of mother—very-preterm infant communication. We hypothesized that, compared to Vertical positioning and as early as 3–4 days after birth, SDF positioning would, by increasing opportunities for eye contact, improve the mothers’ ability to recognize her infant’s signals, thus enabling a more timely feedback. Finally, we aimed to examine how the communicative behaviors of mothers and very-preterm infants were coordinated in time during SSC, just a few days after birth.

Materials and methods

Participants

The study was conducted between May 2015 and June 2016, in a level III NICU at the Centre Hospitalier Intercommunal de Créteil (France). Among the 90 very preterm babies (27 to 31 + 6 weeks’ gestation) admitted during the inclusion period, 53 met parent and child’s inclusion criteria [living in the geographical area considering the longitudinal follow-up, no multiple birth > 2, no neurological complication due to several vascular hemorrhage (IVH grade III, or IV)], no severe medical conditions, no incapacitated physical or psychological illness in the mother. Seven mothers refused to participate, and two inclusions were missed. Among the remaining 44 births, two were lost during follow-up and eight were multiple births which were not included in the present report. The final sample included 34 very preterm infants and their mothers. The first 17 dyads were offered SSC positioning, as usually practiced in the participating NICU (Vertical Control Group). The following 17 dyads were offered the Supported Diagonal Flexion (SDF) Intervention positioning and these were matched to the first 17 dyads on newborns’ gestational age at birth and weight at birth (see Buil et al., 2020 for the detailed method). Participants’ socio-demographic, Ob/Gyn, delivery and birth data were obtained from medical files.

The present study is part of a longitudinal follow-up from very preterm birth until 3 months corrected age. The data presented constituted the first data collection point of this prospective monocentric matched-pair case-control study. Some data from our study of mother-very preterm communication at 18 days post-partum have already been published in a previous paper (Buil et al., 2020). Measures of the mothers’ risk of depression (made before the first SSC session), were comparable for the SDF positioning group and the Vertical positioning group, with EPDS mean scores of 13.8 and 12.9, respectively (Buil et al., 2019).

Skin-to-skin positioning in each group

In the Vertical Control Group, preemies were positioned chest to chest between the mother’s breasts, at the center and on the median line of the mother’s torso, in an upright position, with a breastfeeding nursing pillow (see Figures 1A,B). According to Kangaroo Mother Care guidelines, the head is turned to one side and in a slightly extended position which keeps the airway open. Moreover, the hips should be flexed and extended in a “frog” position; the arms should also be flexed (World Health Organization, 2003).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Vertical control positioning (A,B) and Supported Diagonal Flexion (SDF) intervention positioning (C,D) during the very first SSC after very preterm birth.

In the SDF Intervention positioning (see Figures 1C,D), the baby is naked, off-center and semi-reclined on the mother’s chest, chest to chest (Buil et al., 2016). In this intermittent kangaroo mother care, the choice of on which side of the body (right or left) the baby’s head is positioned is up to the mother. The SDF position is characterized by a slightly flexed body axis, with the limbs retracted in a preventive posture and the head in line with the body axis to prevent side-to-side toppling and to free respiratory permeability (Nyqvist et al., 2010), moderately externally rotated hips in flexion-abduction (Vaivre-Douret et al., 2004), with adducted shoulders (Ferrari et al., 2007). The baby’s head is located between the mother’s nipple and clavicle and oriented toward the mother’s face. His/her arms and legs are flexed, in a naturally adopted asymmetrical tonic neck posture (Casaer, 1979), according to the baby’s gestational age and comfort. The baby is positioned on the mother’s naked chest, a baby wrap adjusted around the two is used to support and help maintain the baby’s posture and to relieve the mother. For the study purpose, we chose The Little-Wrap-Without-A-Knot is a knitted fabric, specially made for Baby Wearing. On one end of the cloth are sewn two metal rings. The Fabric has a special density of 300 gr/m2. It is made of a two-faced Viscose certified Oeko-Tex 100. The special mechanism of this knitted fabric, allows it to slightly stretch, and to ensure a soft touch that respects the preterm baby’s skin. The Little-Wrap-Without-A-Knot is put as a loop around the mother’s body. It goes from shoulder to opposite hip and back up to the shoulder, in an asymmetric diagonal. The end of the fabric passes through the rings that creates a buckle effect. This adjustment system ensures a precise fit between the two morphologies and to maintain the desired position of the baby.

In both positionings, the mother was comfortably seated in an adjustable armchair, her back inclined 40°, with a toe-clip, dim light and a quiet atmosphere; she had the choice to wear clothes or not. In SDF Intervention positioning, the baby wrap was placed over the mother’s clothes or on her naked chest.

Apparatus

Mother and neonate were filmed during their very first SSC, on average 3.7 days after very preterm birth, i.e., on average 29.4 weeks GA (see Table 1). For each dyad, vocal interaction was analyzed only during the first 5 min. Indeed, mothers were particularly active during the first minutes of SSC care, before giving way to mutual relaxation. Thus, 5 min recordings allowed sufficient data collection, but left plenty of intimacy time to the dyad. Mother-neonate dyads were filmed using a single camcorder mounted on a tripod and sound recordings were obtained using a shotgun microphone. Video and audio recordings were synchronized. The camcorder was placed in front of the dyad, in order to frame the mother’s chest and the baby’s entire body, as well as their faces.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Infant’s’ characteristics at birth and at first skin to skin, according to type of SSC positioning.

Video and acoustic analyses

Video analysis was performed using The Observer XT with a precision of 40 ms (25 images per second). Vocal data were analyzed with the software Audacity.

Coding of newborns’ states of alertness

Newborns’ state of alertness was coded with frame-by-frame video microanalysis using the software The Observer XT1 and following APIB’s state configurations (Als et al., 1982): (1) deep sleep state, (2) active sleep state, (3) drowsiness state, (4AL) low awake state, (4AH) high awake state, (4B) quiet awake state, (5) active awake state, and (6) crying state. We included an additional “undefined” category, when the infant’s state of alertness could not be coded because the newborn’s face and body could not be viewed in the video. Coding was carried out by a perinatal professional with NIDCAP certification (Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program). We recorded the time of onset and duration of each of these states of alertness.

Coding of newborn and maternal vocalizations

Maternal and newborn vocalizations were coded using the software Audacity.2 Based on the visualization of sonograms and audio guidance, a segmentation into two types of events was made: maternal vocalization and newborn vocalization, (Gratier et al., 2015; Dominguez et al., 2016), both defined as the production of vocal sound that was either continuous or included unvoiced segments of <300 ms. If a pause following an audible vocal sound was >300 ms, two successive vocalizations were coded. Vegetative sounds produced by infants such as burps, growls or hiccups, noise from the environment and vegetative sounds produced by mothers, such as coughs, were not coded. Our criterion was more flexible than that used with full term babies, in order to include more of the preemies’ voiced demonstrations.

Coding of turn-taking sequences

A turn-taking sequence was defined as a sequence of vocalizations involving at least one alternation between interactive partners, which is expressed as follows. When more than one alternation occurred, with intervening pauses between the two partners, the number of pauses coincided with the number of turns in the sequence (e.g., two turns: “newborn vocalisation-pause-mother vocalisation-pause-newborn vocalisation”; three turns: “newborn vocalisation-pause-mother vocalisation-pause-newborn vocalization-pause-mother vocalisation” and so on). A turn-taking sequence ended when the same partner produced at least two successive vocalizations, or when the pause following a vocalization exceeded 3,000 ms in accordance with previous studies (Stern, 1985; Gratier et al., 2015; Dominguez et al., 2016; Boiteau et al., 2021).

Training and reliability

A single coder was responsible for coding states of consciousness with the Observer software and two coders for coding the vocal exchange with the Audacity software. None of the coders were aware of the aim and hypotheses under investigation. For this reason, training was performed on randomly chosen dyads both from the SDF and Vertical positioning groups. The training of coders consisted in three steps. First, coders were trained to use the coding template on 4 dyads under the supervision of the researchers. During the second step, each trainee was invited to code 4 additional dyads alone. At the end of this step, the coding was checked and discussed with the supervisor. The third step consisted in reapplying the second step.

Twenty five percent of the data set, chosen randomly, was double-coded. Inter-coder reliability (Pearson product–moment correlations) regarding the number of behaviors ranged from 0.82 to 0.92 depending on the behavior. Onset positions were considered identical if they occurred within 80 ms (i.e., two frames); thus, measures of behavior duration had an error tolerance of up to 160 ms. Both coders correctly identified 77.2% of all onset positions within the subset of double-coded sequences.

Ethical considerations

All mothers were offered to participate to the research study on a voluntary basis, within the first 2 days postpartum, and in all cases before the first SSC session. Every mother was informed of the research by a letter in the NICU. Mothers gave a written informed consent before participating. Written informed consent was obtained from the mothers for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article. An initial information-based meeting was organized prior to data collection. This research was approved by the French Local Ethics Consulting Committee for the Protection of Persons (IRB n°2015120001072).

Statistics

All analyses were performed using Stata for Windows (version 14; StataCorp). Sociodemographic data, previous obstetrical and delivery data and infants’ characteristics at birth and at first skin to skin contact were compared in both groups, with either a t-test or a chi-square test, depending on the measure. The number of vocalizations and their duration were analyzed through a general linear model with an adjustment on age and weight at birth. Parametric tests were not chosen considering these measures did not differ significantly from normality.

Results

For 11 dyads, either the baby or the mother did not vocalize during the recorded session of SSC. Analysis of the vocal exchange was therefore conducted on 23 dyads, 11 dyads in the Vertical control group and 12 in the SDF Intervention group. Table 2 presents sociodemographic characteristics, obstetrical and delivery data, and Table 1 infant’s data at birth, at first skin to skin, according to SSC positioning. No significant difference was found between the two groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Sociodemographic, previous obstetrical, and delivery data according to SSC positioning.

State of consciousness

Infants were mainly in a state of drowsiness (state 3), even more so in the Vertical group (58%) than in the SDF group (35% of time). Active sleep state (state 2) represented around 35% of the time in both groups. However, infants in the SDF group also spent 19% of the time in deep sleep (state 1), while those in the Vertical group spent only 3%. In SDF group, infants spent 58% of the time sleeping (states 1 and 2) compared to 36% in the Vertical group. Distributions were significantly different [Chi-square (5) = 116, p < 0.0001].

Newborns’ vocal production

We collected 167 vocalizations: 84 in the SDF group (i.e., on average 7.0 per newborn and 1.4 per minute) and 83 in the Vertical group (i.e., on average 7.5 per newborn and 1.5 per minute, adj p = 0.90). Vocalizations’ duration was on average longer in the Vertical group (Vertical: 817 ms, SDF: 537 ms), but not significantly after adjustment on birth age and weight (adj p = 0.78). Consequently, newborns’ vocalizations occupied less dialogue space in the SDF group than in the Vertical group (1.3% vs. 1.9% of the 5 min analyzed).

Mothers’ vocal production

Overall, mothers vocalized twice more in the SDF group (604, i.e., on average 45.3 per mother and 9.1 per minute) than in the Vertical group (269, i.e., on average 24.5 per mother and 4.9 per minute), but the difference was not significant (adj p = 0.068). Their vocalizations were on average significantly longer (SDF: 1166 ms, Vertical: 1068 ms, adj p = 0.002). Hence, Mothers’ vocalizations occupied more dialogue space in the SDF group than in the Vertical group (20.1% vs. 8.0% of the 5 min analyzed, p = 0.007).

Temporal proximity of mother and newborn vocalizations

Based on a 3-s criterion, 3 preterm newborn vocalizations out of 4 (74%) were at a temporal proximity from a maternal vocalization in the SDF intervention group against 1 out of 2 (47%) in the Vertical control group (OR = 3.2, p < 0.0001). The difference was still significant when using a 1-s criterion (57% vs. 39%, OR = 2.1, p = 0.017).

Turn-taking coordination

Based on a 3-s criterion, 73 mother-newborn turn-taking sequences (TTS) were identified: 44 in the SDF group and 29 in the Vertical group. In all, 75 newborn vocalizations were involved in these sequences, i.e., 45% of the total number of vocalizations collected. Almost all sequences were either one turn “baby–mother” or two turns “mother–baby–mother.” One third of these were two-turns sequences, sensitively but not significantly more in the SDF group (OR = 1.4, p = 0.56). When using a 1-s criterion, the number of TTS sequences fell to 65: 36 in the SDF group and 29 in the Vertical group: the odd ratio indicates a 3.8 times higher chance to observe a two-turns sequence rather than a one-turn sequence in the SDF intervention group than in the Vertical control group, but it does not reach significance (OR = 3.8, p = 0.059).

Discussion

The present study brings pioneer data on the very first skin to skin between mother and her very premature baby. Although novel, analyses conducted in the paper are pilot, the first of their kind.

First meeting: Not necessarily vocal

Our study focused on the very first vocal exchanges, therefore we selected dyads in which both the mother and the baby vocalized (23 out of 34 dyads). It is important to note that during the first 5 min of the first meeting after giving birth, some mothers may have not wished to speak. This very first skin-to-skin is also the very first opportunity for mothers to hold the newborn, with a place for spontaneity as it is a non-medical, non-nursing, and non-guided moment. It is a moment for intimacy, for being together, and not necessarily for speaking. Indeed, during this moment, deciding to speak or not to speak to the newborn was the mother’s choice. It seems that this choice was the expression of the mother’s preferred way of communicating, the relationship being potentially expressed through other sensory modalities: tactile (Buil et al., 2017a), kinaesthetic, visual.

Better sleep quality

Our results showed that within the first 5 min of the very first skin to skin, very preterm newborns in the “SDF” group spent less time in a drowsy state (state 3), and more in deep sleep (state 1), than those in the Vertical control group. This result suggests that the “SDF” positioning helps preterms stabilize in restorative sleep during skin to skin, rather than stay in a state of drowsiness, considered by some authors as a transitional state, costly in energy (Als, 1982; Brazelton and Nugent, 1995; Bullinger and Goubet, 1999; Foreman et al., 2008; Devouche and Buil, 2019). This stabilization of preterm newborns’ state could rely on both an improved postural support thanks to the SDF positioning that fosters the axial winding posture with support on the neck and retroverted pelvis (Vaivre-Douret et al., 2004), but also the active behavioral support of mothers installed in SDF positioning. These mothers would intuitively help their child into deep sleep, switching rapidly from state 2 to 1, or appease him/her. From the first minutes of this renewed closeness, the SDF positioning could increase mothers’ sensitivity to their child’s signals of disorganization (such as growling, frowning, wriggling, spreading their fingers, etc.,), allowing them to respond by speaking, nursing or caressing them before they reach full disorganization.

Premature newborn vocal presence

During this moment, that occurred on average at 3.5 days of life, newborns’ vocal production did not differ significantly between the two groups, even though vocalizations were on average longer in the Vertical group (before statistical adjustment). Our vocalizations sample was not large enough to allow us to investigate the links between length of vocalizations and states of awareness. However, we believe that the lengthier vocalizations recorded in the Vertical group could be linked to the state 3 that is found in the synactive theory of development (Als, 1982) and characterized by more grunting, which are also probably longer than other kinds of vocalizations. It would be interesting to investigate this further, by characterizing the quality of this vocalization.

One aim of this study was to better characterize the vocal meeting between mother and very preterm newborn, during the first minutes of their first ever skin to skin. Our study provides pioneering data regarding the vocal presence of the very preterm newborn during the first SSC, which represents 1.4 to 1.5 vocalizations per minute for the 23 dyads in which vocalizations did occur, regardless of the positioning. The observed frequency was much higher than that recorded by Caskey et al. (2011), because data was collected at a time dedicated to the relationship, whereas Caskey and colleagues made their recording during several hours in a crib. Vocal displays were scarcer in our very preterm newborns than in healthy term newborns installed for a cuddle in the parent’s arms or close to each other [3/min in Boiteau et al., 2021 and 2.7/min in Dominguez et al., 2016], but they were present. Indeed, it is hard to know whether these vocalizations were signs of discomfort or communication, whether they were voluntary or not, and whether they were addressed or not. Their mere existence, despite infants’ immaturity, parents’ feeling of unease and the hypermedicalization of these first moments, provides an opportunity to establish communication.

Maternal vocal envelope

Our results showed a denser vocal presence in the SDF group than in the Vertical group (20% vs. 8%), with longer vocal interventions. This longer exposure to the mother’s voice might be important according to a recent literature review highlighting its positive impact on parenting skills (Filippa et al., 2019). This result supports the hypothesis that SSC in SDF positioning enhances and supports early vocal contact. This strategy is now recognized as a new and important Infant- and Family-Centered Developmental Care (IFCDC) strategy for the benefit of preterm infant brain development (Filippa et al., 2017; Monaci et al., 2021).

This clearly denser maternal vocal envelope could be perceived as over-stimulating for a 3–4-day preterm newborn. The SDF positioning was not conceived to promote “more” (as in “too much”) but rather “better,” closer to what would be the first meeting with a healthy term newborn. Here, judging by the analysis of the newborn’s states of consciousness, it seems that these behaviors were not perceived as over-stimulations. This question could be further explored by investigating the links between maternal voice and states of consciousness, as it was done by Filippa et al. (2018) or Saliba et al. (2020).

Temporal coordination

In our pilot data collection, we tried to investigate the vocal temporal coordination between the mother and her very preterm newborn as Dominguez et al. (2016) and Boiteau et al. (2021) did with term-newborns. However, the few vocalizations recorded only allowed a timed analysis. Although no difference was found between both positionings regarding the amount of newborn vocalizations involved in turn-taking, descriptive results suggest more complex turn-taking in the SDF intervention group. Although not significant, this result is consistent with the denser vocal presence of mothers. We hypothesized that being more able to see the face and perceiving their newborn as an available partner, mothers in the SDF group were more likely to vocalize in a timely manner and thus create the very first vocal exchange with their baby.

Furthermore, based on a 1-s criterion, 57% of the preterm newborn vocalizations were at a temporal proximity of a mother’s vocalization in the SDF intervention group against 39% in the Vertical control group. Hence, our results show that mothers in the SDF intervention group provided a more proximal vocal envelope, but far from the 95% observed by Dominguez et al. (2016) in mothers of healthy term babies. By nature, full-term infants require the intervention of competent adults, and in this sense, it is not relevant to oppose the “natural” lives of term infants with the “unnatural” lives of hospitalized infants (Gratier and Devouche, 2017). Environments are constructed around infants but more importantly, infants, from the earliest moments of their lives, participate in this construction. The present study adds to the current knowledge and understanding that preterm infants respond to their environment too. Our results plead for more reflection on how to better adapt the environment, so that in turn, it responds to the premature baby.

Developmental perspectives

Dyads observed in the present study were followed during the first months of life and data collected at 18 days of life, i.e., 15 days after the very first SSC have already been published in a previous paper (Buil et al., 2020). At birth, preemies in both groups did not differ in their vocal production, the main results being that mothers provide a more proximal and denser vocal envelope. Two weeks later, in the SDF Intervention Group, very preterm infants vocalized three times more, mothers vocalized, gazed at their baby’s face, and smiled more than in the Vertical Control Group, and temporal proximity of mother-infant behaviors in a one-second time frame was greater in the SDF Intervention Group (Buil et al., 2020).

The present study and the previous one plead in favor of a positive impact of a more comfortable positioning, that allows visual contact for mother-preemie interaction from the very beginning. Indeed, when offered more opportunities to be connected, mothers tended to engage more in the communication. Being able to perceive key behaviors such as open eyes, mouth movements, or being able to understand a vocal manifestation because they have a better perception of the whole story, mothers are more responsive from the very beginning. Thus, because primary intersubjectivity had the possibility to develop and so had the dyadic space (Trevarthen, 1998), mother and preemie were more able to meet and communicate 2 weeks later.

SDF positioning not only influences positively infants’ states of consciousness and mothers’ availability, thus fostering communication. Indeed, what seems to matter the most is the mother’s responsiveness, her ability to perceive the availability of her very preterm newborn to interact (Ainsworth et al., 2015) and to adjust her behavior at the level of body tonus and emotion (Ajuriaguerra, 1986).

Long-term perspective: Supporting the preemie, the mother and the father, and a timely adjusted communication during SSC

Given the persistence of difficulties in parent-infant synchrony at 3 (Feldman and Eidelman, 2007) and 6 months GA (Forcada-Guex et al., 2011) in preterm contexts, offering SSC to support communication between the preemie and his parent as soon as possible after birth is essential. Feldman et al. (2014) pointed out that any attempt to encourage mother-infant proximity could help fragile babies and their parents to develop optimal synchrony before the end of the sensitive period. Practicing SSC with SDF positioning, rather than SSC as it is widely practiced today (Vertical positioning), offsets the paucity of parent-infant communication related to preterm birth, by enhancing both partners’ production and detection of multimodal signals, as well as their temporal coordination. Our results highlight the possibility of mother—very preterm baby vocal exchanges during skin-to-skin, especially with SDF positioning, as early as 3/4 days after birth.

The literature shows that mothers present a left side bias for carrying their baby (Donnot and Vauclair, 2005; Malatesta et al., 2019), which seems particularly stable during the first 3 months of the child’s life, regardless of culture and time (Malatesta et al., 2019). These two studies noted correlations between carrying bias and maternal empathy as well as mother’s capacity to engage emotionally in a relationship (Donnot and Vauclair, 2005; Malatesta et al., 2019). In the present study, five mothers out of 17 spontaneously chose to carry their newborn on the left side of their body during the first SSC. Hence, by allowing mothers to choose on which side their wish to carry their child each time a skin-to-skin care is proposed, SSC in SDF positioning is indeed a natural precocious way to support intuitive parenting (Papoušek and Papoušek, 2002).

In their article on the eight principles to follow in NICU, Roué et al. (2017) highlighted the need for a free 24 h/24 parental access and a psychological parental support. In their conclusion they recalled the need for future researches in this direction. More recently, Filippa et al. (2021) published a systematic review on the benefits of involving the fathers of preterm infants in early interventions in neonatal intensive care units. Their conclusion was in line with Roué et al.’s recommendations by stressing the need to develop new, multimodal and interactive interventions that provide fathers with positive contact with their preterm infants. Indeed, few studies were conducted on early father-preterm communication. However, as Boiteau et al. (2021) showed, the father and the baby meet as of the birth, why not the father and the (very) premature newborn? Benefits of SDF positioning could thus be appreciated through daily practice with fathers in NICU.

SDF positioning could promote brain-to-brain synchrony during naturalistic social interactions (Kinreich et al., 2017). SSC is described as the only developmental care dedicated to dyadic interaction (Buil, 2019), but this appears all the more obvious when both partners manage to meet (even briefly), by making eye contact, smiling and vocalizing. Even though long-term benefits of SDF positioning are yet to be investigated, our results, although based on pilot data, pleads in favor of a modification of current skin-to-skin contact practices with very preterm babies.

Limitations

This pilot study presents some limitations. Additionally, to the number of dyads included (N = 34) which in itself limits the range of our results, our study was conducted in one unique level 3 NICU. A random allocation of dyads in SSC positioning would have been more optimal and would have strengthened our study design, but it would have created a risk of practice contamination. The safe implementation of SDF positioning in NICU needs to be further examined by conducting a multicentric study based on randomized sampling and including a larger group of extremely preterm infants and/or unstable very preterm infants requiring endotracheal ventilation.

Conclusion

Very little is known about what happens between the very preterm and the parent during skin-to-skin contact, despite it being the only care entirely dedicated to the parent–infant relationship in NICUs. Seeing very preterm newborns open their eyes, attempt a smile or hearing their vocalizations are rare and short-lived moments. Yet they are essential moments, because they reveal the preterm as a fully-fledged individual, who communicates, despite immaturity and uncertain vital and developmental prognoses. They are also essential when infant and parents meet for they contribute to the creation of the first bonds.

Our study showed that whatever the positioning considered, and although we only analyzed the first 5 min of SSC in a sample of 23 dyads, very preterm newborns were able to vocalize. Indeed, during SSC, i.e., in a moment dedicated to intimacy, the very preterm newborn was vocally present and provided opportunities to establish communication. Moreover, SDF positioning supported mothers’ vocal responsiveness to the vocal presence of their newborn, thus fostering the beginning of adapted and coordinated vocal responses, and feeding intersubjective intimacy.

Providing early communicational pathways for parents and very preterm infants, as early as possible, enables initial interactive experiences, providing a less atypical, less dysregulated, and more consistent environment. Our pioneer data sheds light on the way a mother and her very preterm vocally meet, and constitutes a pilot step in the exploration of innate intersubjectivity in the context of very preterm birth. Future studies are needed to explore other ways by which the very preterm connects to his/her partner, thus creating a dyadic intersubjective space, i.e., vision, tonic postural dialogue, rhythm, touch, and olfaction.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because it contains sociodemographic and medical information that cannot be shared consistently with GDPR requirements. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to devouche7@gmail.com.

Ethics statement

All mothers were offered to participate to the research study on a voluntary basis, within the first two days postpartum, and in all cases before the first SSC session. Every mother was informed of the research by a letter in the NICU. Mothers gave a written informed consent before participating. Written informed consent was obtained from the mothers for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article. An initial information-based meeting was organised prior to data collection. This research was approved by the French Local Ethics Consulting Committee for the Protection of Persons (IRB n°2015120001072).

Author contributions

AB and ED contributed to the conception and design of the study and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. AB collected the data. ED performed the statistical analysis. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by four research grants by Institut Universitaire de France, Fondation Mustela, Fondation pour la Recherche en Psychomotricité et Maladies de Civilisation, and Fondation des Treilles. Baby wraps were provided by L’école à porter—JPMBB.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the Chairs and staff of the Neonatal Medicine Department who facilitated access to the participants in this study. We extend our special thanks to all the families who agreed to take part in this research project.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Abbreviations

APIB, Assessment of preterm infants’ behavior scale; IFCDC, Infant- and Family-Centered Developmental Care; GA, Gestational age; NICU, Neonatal intensive care unit; SDF, Supported diagonal flexion; SSC, Skin-to-skin contact.

Footnotes

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S. N. Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the strange Situation. London: PsychologyPress; (2015).

Google Scholar

Ajuriaguerra, J. D. (1986). Organización neuropsicológica de algunas funciones: de los movimientos espontáneos al diálogo tónico-postural ya las formas precoces de comunicación. Revista Psicomotricidad. 23, 17–34.

Google Scholar

Als, H. (1982). Toward a synactive theory of development: promise for the assessment and support of infant individuality. Infant Ment. Health J. 3, 229–243.

Google Scholar

Als, H., Lester, B. M., Tronick, E. Z., and Brazelton, T. B. (1982). “Toward a research instrument for the assessment of preterm infants’ behavior (APIB)” in Theory and Research in Behavioral pediatrics (Springer, Boston, MA), 35–132.

Google Scholar

Anderson, C., and Cacola, P. (2017). Implications of preterm birth for maternal mental health and infant development. MCN: The American journal of maternal/child. Nursing 42, 108–114. doi: 10.1097/NMC.0000000000000311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderzen-Carlsson, A., Lamy, Z. C., Tingvall, M., and Eriksson, M. (2014). Parental experiences of providing skin-to-skin care to their newborn infant—part 2: a qualitative meta-synthesis. Int. J. Qual. Stud. Health Well-Being 9:24907. doi: 10.3402/qhw.v9.24907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Athanasopoulou, E., and Fox, J. R. (2014). Effects of kangaroo mother care on maternal mood and interaction patterns between parents and their preterm, low birth weight infants: a systematic review. Infant Ment. Health J. 35, 245–262. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bateson, M. C. (1979). “The epigenesis of conversational interaction: A personal account of research development,” in Before speech: The Beginning of human Communication. ed. M. Bullowa (London: Cambridge University Press), 63–77.

Google Scholar

Beebe, B. (2017). Daniel Stern: microanalysis and the empirical infant research foundations. Psychoanal. Inq. 37, 228–241.

Google Scholar

Blomqvist, Y. T., Ewald, U., Gradin, M., Nyqvist, K. H., and Rubertsson, C. (2013). Initiation and extent of skin-to-skin care at two Swedish neonatal intensive care units. Acta Paediatr. 102, 22–28. doi: 10.1111/apa.12056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boiteau, C., Kokkinaki, T., Sankey, C., Buil, A., Gratier, M., and Devouche, E. (2021). Father–newborn vocal interaction: A contribution to the theory of innate intersubjectivity. Infant Child Dev. :e2259. doi: 10.1002/icd.2259

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brazelton, T. B., Yogman, M. W., Als, H., and Tronick, E. (1979). “The infant as a focus for family reciprocity,” in The child and his Family. eds. M. Lewis and L. A. Rosenblum (London: Plenum Press), 29–43.

Google Scholar

Brazelton, T. B. (1979). “Evidence of Communication during Neonatal Behavioral Assessment,” in Beforespeech: The beginning of human communication. ed. M. Bullowa (London: Cambridge University Press), 79–99.

Google Scholar

Brazelton, T. B., and Nugent, J. K. (1995). Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale. 3rd.

Google Scholar

Bruner, J. S. (1977). “Early social interaction and language acquisition,” in Studies in mother-infant Interaction: The Loch Lomond Symposium. ed. H. R. Schaffer (London: Academic Press), 271–289.

Google Scholar

Buil, A. (2019). “Early psychomotor therapy: support for vulnerable babies and their families,” in Early Interaction and Developmental Psychopathology. eds. G. Apter, E. Devouche, and M. Gratier (Springer)

Google Scholar

Buil, A., Caeymaex, L., Mero, S., Sankey, C., Apter, G., and Devouche, E. (2019). Kangaroo supported diagonal flexion positioning: positive impact on maternal stress and postpartum depression risk and on skin-to-skin practice with very preterm infants. J. Neonatal Nurs. 25, 86–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jnn.2018.10.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buil, A., Carchon, I., Apter, G., Laborne, F. X., Granier, M., and Devouche, E. (2016). Kangaroo supported diagonal flexion positioning: new insights into skin-to-skin contact for communication between mothers and very preterm infants. Arch. Pediatr. 23, 913–920. doi: 10.1016/j.arcped.2016.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buil, A., Da Costa, S. M., Granger, A., and Devouche, E. (2017a). Changer l’installation du soin peau-à-peau en néonatalogie pour une communication précoce de qualité. Enfance 4, 471–476. doi: 10.3917/enf1.174.0471

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buil, A., Fillon-Devys, D., Granger, A., Roger, K., Thomas, N., Apter, G., et al. (2017b). Impact de l’installation en Flexion diagonale soutenue sur le maternage tactile spontané lors de la première séance de peau-à-peau en réanimation néonatale. Neuropsychiatr. Enfance Adolesc. 65, 289–298.

Google Scholar

Buil, A., Sankey, C., Caeymaex, L., Apter, G., Gratier, M., and Devouche, E. (2020). Fostering mother-very preterm infant communication during skin-to-skin contact through a modified positioning. Early Hum. Dev. 141:104939. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2019.104939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullinger, A., and Goubet, N. (1999). Le bébé prématuré, acteur de son développement. Enfance 52, 27–32. doi: 10.3406/enfan.1999.3127

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cadwell, K., Brimdyr, K., and Phillips, R. (2018). Mapping, measuring, and analyzing the process of skin-to-skin contact and early breastfeeding in the first hour after birth. Breastfeed. Med. 13, 485–492. doi: 10.1089/bfm.2018.0048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cambonie, G., Muller, J., Ehlinger, V., Roy, J., Guédeney, A., Lebeaux, C., et al. (2017). OLIMPE study writing group. Mother-infant interaction assessment at discharge and at 6 months in a French cohort of infants born very preterm: The OLIMPE study. PLoS One 12:e0188942. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188942

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carvalho, M. E. S., Justo, J. M., Gratier, M., Tomé, T., Pereira, E., and Rodrigues, H. (2019). Vocal responsiveness of preterm infants to maternal infant-directed speaking and singing during skin-to-skin contact (kangaroo care) in the NICU. Infant Behav. Dev. 57:101332

Google Scholar

Casaer, P. Postural Behaviour in Newborn Infants Clinics in Developmental Medicine. London: W. Spastic International Medical Publications; (1979).

Google Scholar

Caskey, M., Stephen, B., Tucker, R., and Vohr, B. (2011). Importance of parent talk on the development of preterm infant vocalizations. Pediatrics 128, 910–916. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-0609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caskey, M., Stephens, B., Tucker, R., and Vohr, B. (2014). Adult talk in the NICU with preterm infants and developmental outcomes. Pediatrics 133, 578–584.

Google Scholar

Chan, G. J., Labar, A. S., Wall, S., and Atun, R. (2016). Kangaroo mother care: a systematic review of barriers and enablers. Bull. World Health Organ. 94:130.

Google Scholar

Charpak, N., Gabriel Ruiz, J., Zupan, J., Cattaneo, A., Figueroa, Z., Tessier, R., et al. (2005). Kangaroo mother care: 25 years after. Acta Paediatr. 94, 514–522.

Google Scholar

Conde-Agudelo, A., Belizán, J. M., and Díaz-Rossello, J. L. (2011). Kangaroo mother care to reduce morbidity and mortality in low birthweight infants. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 16. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD002771.pub4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cong, X., Ludington-Hoe, S. M., Hussain, N., Cusson, R. M., Walsh, S., Vazquez, V., et al. (2015). Parental oxytocin responses during skin-to-skin contact in pre-term infants. Early Hum. Dev. 91, 401–406. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Paula Eduardo, J. A. F., de Rezende, M. G., Menezes, P. R., and Del-Ben, C. M. (2019). Preterm birth as a risk factor for postpartum depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 259, 392–403. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.08.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devouche, E., and Buil, A. (2019). “De l’état éveillé au sommeil. Comprendre les états de conscience du bébé,” in J. Le développement du bébé de la vie fœtale à la marche: Sensoriel-Psychomoteur-Cognitif-Affectif-Social. eds. E. Devouche and Provasi (Elsevier Health Sciences)

Google Scholar

Devouche, E., and Gratier, M. (2019). “The beginning of parent-infant communication” in Early Interaction and Developmental Psychopathology. Volume I: Infancy. eds. G. Apter, E. Devouche, and M. Gratier (Cham: Springer), 21–34.

Google Scholar

Doheny, L., Morey, J. A., Ringer, S. A., and Lahav, A. (2012). Reduced frequency of apnea and bradycardia episodes caused by exposure to biological maternal sounds. Pediatr. Int. 54, e1–e3. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-200X.2012.03575.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dominguez, S., Devouche, E., Apter, G., and Gratier, M. (2016). The roots of turn-taking in the neonatal period. Infant Child Dev. 25, 240–255.

Google Scholar

Donnot, J., and Vauclair, J. (2005). Biais de latéralité dans la façon de porter un très jeune enfant: une revue de la question. Neuropsychiatr. Enfance Adolesc. 53, 413–425. doi: 10.1016/j.neurenf.2005.09.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2006). From biological rhythms to social rhythms: physiological precursors of mother-infant synchrony. Dev. Psychol. 42, 175–188.

Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2015). Mutual influences between child emotion regulation and parent–child reciprocity support development across the first 10 years of life: implications for developmental psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 27, 1007–1023. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R., and Eidelman, A. I. (2003). Skin-to-skin contact (kangaroo care) accelerates autonomic and neurobehavioural maturation in preterm infants. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 45, 274–281.

Google Scholar

Feldman, R., and Eidelman, A. (2007). Maternal postpartum behavior and the emergence of infant–mother and infant–father synchrony in preterm and full-term infants: The role of neonatal vagal tone. Dev. Psychobiol. 49, 290–302.

Google Scholar

Feldman, R., Eidelman, A. I., Sirota, L., and Weller, A. (2002). Comparaison of skin-to-skin (kangaroo) and traditional care: parenting outcomes et preterm infant developement. Pediatrics 110, 16–26.

Google Scholar

Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., and Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother-infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Dev. Psychol. 35, 223–231. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R., Magori-Cohen, R., Galili, G., Singer, M., and Louzon, Y. (2011). Mother and infant coordinate heart rhythms through episodes of interaction synchrony. Infant Behav. Dev. 34, 569–577. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R., Rosentha, Z., and Eidelman, A. I. (2014). Maternal-preterm skin-to-skin contact enhances child physiologic organization and cognitive control across the first 10 years of life. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 56–64. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, F., Bertoncelli, N., Gallo, C., Roversi, M. F., Guerra, M. P., Ranzi, A., et al. (2007). Posture and movement in healthy preterm infants in supine position in and outside the nest. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 92, 386–390.

Google Scholar

Ferrari, G. A., Nicolini, Y., Demuru, E., Tosato, C., Hussain, M., Scesa, E., et al. (2016). Ultrasonographic investigation ofhumanfetus responses to maternal communicative and non-communicative stimuli. Front. Psychol. 7:354. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filippa, M., Gratier, M., Devouche, E., and Grandjean, D. (2018). Changes in infant-directed speech and song are related to preterm infant facial expression in the neonatal intensive care unit. Interact. Stud. 19, 427–444.

Google Scholar

Filippa, M., Kuhn, P., and Westrup, B. Early Vocal Contact and Preterm Infant Brain Development. Cham: Springer; (2017).

Google Scholar

Filippa, M., Lordier, L., De Almeida, J. S., Monaci, M. G., Darque, A. A., Grandjean, D., et al. (2019). Early vocal contact and music in the NICU: new insights into preventive interventions. Pediatr. Res. 87, 249–264. doi: 10.1038/s41390-019-0490-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filippa, M., Saliba, S., Esseily, R., Gratier, M., Grandjean, D., and Kuhn, P. (2021). Systematic review shows the benefits of involving the fathers of preterm infants in early interventions in neonatal intensive care units. Acta Paediatr. 110, 2509–2520. doi: 10.1111/apa.15961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forcada-Guex, M., Borghini, A., Pierrehumbert, B., Ansermet, F., and Muller-Nix, C. (2011). Prematurity, maternal posttraumatic stress and consequences on the mother–infant relationship. Early Hum. Dev. 87, 21–26. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foreman, S. W., Thomas, K. A., and Blackburn, S. T. (2008). Individual and gender differences matter in preterm infant state development. J. Obstet. Gynecol. Neonatal Nurs. 37, 657–665. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.2008.00292.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franck, L. S., and O'Brien, K. (2019). The evolution of family-centered care: From supporting parent-delivered interventions to a model of family integrated care. Birth Defects Res. 111, 1044–1059. doi: 10.1002/bdr2.1521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gatta, M., Miscioscia, M., Svanellini, L., Peraro, C., and Simonelli, A. (2017). A psychological perspective on preterm children: the influence of contextual factors on quality of family interactions. Biomed. Res. Int. 2017. doi: 10.1155/2017/9152627

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gere, S., Berhane, Y., and Worku, A. (2021). Chest-to-Back skin-to-skin contact to regulate body temperature for low birth weight and/or preterm babies: A crossover randomized controlled clinical trial. Int. J. Pediatr. 2021

Google Scholar

Gratier, M., and Devouche, E. (2017). “The development of infant participation in communication,” in Early Vocal Contact and Preterm Infant Brain Development. eds. F. Manuela, P. Kuhn, and P. Westrup (Cham: Springer), 55–69.

Google Scholar

Gratier, M., Devouche, E., Guellai, B., Infanti, R., Yilmaz, E., and Parlato-Oliveira, E. (2015). Early development of turn-taking invocal interaction between mothers and infants. Front. Psychol. 6:1167. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grote, N. K., Bridge, J. A., Gavin, A. R., Melville, J. L., Iyengar, S., and Katon, W. J. (2010). A meta-analysis of depression during pregnancy and the risk of preterm birth, low birth weight, and intrauterine growth restriction. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 1012–1024. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harding, C., Levin, A., Crossley, S. L., Murphy, R., and Van den Engel-Hoek, L. (2019). Effects of early communication intervention on speech and communication skills of preterm infants in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU): A systematic review. J. Neonatal Nurs. 25, 177–188. doi: 10.1016/j.jnn.2019.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., Jasnow, M. D., Rochat, P., et al. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: coordinated timing in development. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 66, 1–7. doi: 10.1111/1540-5834.00137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, C., Campbell-Yeo, M., Disher, T., Benoit, B., Fernandes, A., Streiner, D., et al. (2017). Skin‐to‐skin care for procedural pain in neonates. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008435.pub2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinreich, S., Djalovski, A., Kraus, L., Louzoun, Y., and Feldman, R. (2017). Brain-to-brain synchrony during naturalistic social interactions. Sci. Rep. 7:17060. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-17339-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M. J., Lee, K., Xie, X., Huang, H., Ye, H.-H., et al. (2003). Effects of experience on fetalvoicerecognition. Psychol. Sci. 14, 220–224.

Google Scholar

Kristoffersen, L., Støen, R., Rygh, H., Sognnæs, M., Follestad, T., Mohn, H. S., et al. (2016). Early skin-to-skin contact or incubator for very preterm infants: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 17:593. doi: 10.1186/s13063-016-1730-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawn, J. E., Mwansa-Kambafwile, J., Horta, B. L., Barros, F. C., and Cousens, S. (2010). ‘Kangaroo mother care’ to prevent neonatal deaths due to preterm birth complications. Int. J. Epidemiol. 39, i144–i154. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lickliter, R. (2000). Atypical perinatal sensory stimulation and early perceptual development: insights from developmental psychobiology. J. Perinatol. 20, S45–S54. doi: 10.1038/sj.jp.7200450

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linnér, A., Klemming, S., Sundberg, B., Lilliesköld, S., Westrup, B., Jonas, W., et al. (2020). Immediate skin-to-skin contact is feasible for very preterm infants but thermal control remains a challenge. Acta Paediatr. 109, 697–704. doi: 10.1111/apa.15062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludington-Hoe, S. M., Morgan, K., and Abouelfettoh, A. (2008). A clinical guideline for implementation of kangaroo care with preterm infants of 30 or more weeks' postmenstrual age. Adv. Neonatal Care 8, S3–S23. doi: 10.1097/01.ANC.0000324330.25734.b6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maastrup, R., Weis, J., Engsig, A. B., Johannsen, K. L., and Zoffmann, V. (2018). Now she has become my daughter: parents’ early experiences of skin-to-skin contact with extremely preterm infants. Scand. J. Caring Sci. 32, 545–553. doi: 10.1111/scs.12478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malatesta, G., Marzoli, D., Rapino, M., and Tommasi, L. (2019). The left-cradling bias and its relationship with empathy and depression. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-42539-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markova, G., Nguyen, T., and Hoehl, S. (2019). Neurobehavioral interpersonal synchrony in early development: The role of interactional rhythms. Front. Psychol. 10:2078. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marlier, L., and Schaal, B. (2005). Human newborns prefer human milk: conspecific milk odor is attractive without postnatal exposure. Child Dev. 76, 155–168. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00836.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marx, V., and Nagy, E. (2015). Fetalbehavioral responses to maternal voice and touch. PLoS One 10:e0129118. doi: 10.1371/journal.Pone.0129118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monaci, M. G., Gratier, M., Trevarthen, C., Grandjean, D., Kuhn, P., and Filippa, M. (2021). Parental perception of vocal contact with preterm infants: communicative musicality in the neonatal intensive care unit. Children 8:513. doi: 10.3390/children8060513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montagna, A., and Nosarti, C. (2016). Socio-emotional development following very preterm birth: pathways to psychopathology. Front. Psychol. 7:80. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, E. R., Bergman, N., Anderson, G. C., and Medley, N. (2016). Early skin‐to‐skin contact for mothers and their healthy newborn infants. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003519.pub4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mörelius, E., Angelhoff, C., Eriksson, J., and Olhager, E. (2012). Time of initiation of skin-to-skin contact in extremely preterm infants in Sweden. Acta Paediatr. 101, 14–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02398.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mörelius, E., Örtenstrand, A., Theodorsson, E., and Frostell, A. (2015). A randomised trial of continuous skin-to-skin contact after preterm birth and the effects on salivary cortisol, parental stress, depression, and breastfeeding. Early Hum. Dev. 91, 63–70. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mori, R., Khanna, R., Pledge, D., and Nakayama, T. (2010). Meta-analysis of physiological effects of skin-to-skin contact for newborns and mothers. Pediatr. Int. 52, 161–170. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-200X.2009.02909.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller-Nix, C., Forcada-Guex, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Jaunin, L., Borghini, A., and Ansermet, F. (2004). Prematurity, maternal stress and mother–child interactions. Early Hum. Dev. 79, 145–158. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyqvist, K. H., Anderson, G. C., Bergman, N., Cattaneo, A., Charpak, N., Davanzo, R., et al. (2010). Towards universal kangaroo mother care: recommendations and report from the first European conference and seventh international workshop on kangaroo mother care. Acta Paediatr. 99, 820–826. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2010.01787.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pandya, D., Kartikeswar, G. A. P., Patwardhan, G., Kadam, S., Pandit, A., and Patole, S. (2021). Effect of early kangaroo mother care on time to full feeds in preterm infants-A prospective cohort study. Early Hum. Dev. 154:105312. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2021.105312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papoušek, H., and Papoušek, M. (2002). “Intuitive parenting,” in Handbook of Parenting Volume 2 Biology and Ecology of Parenting. ed. M. H. Bornstein (Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 183–203.

Google Scholar

Philbin, K. M. (2017). “The sound environments and auditory perceptions of the fetus and preterm newborn” in Early Vocal Contact and Preterm Infant Brain Development: Bridging the Gaps Between Research and Practice. eds. M. Filippa, B. Westrup, and P. Kuhn (Cham, Switzerland: Springer), 91–111.

Google Scholar

Pierrat, V., Marchand-Martin, L., Marret, S., Arnaud, C., Benhammou, V., Cambonie, G., et al. (2021). Neurodevelopmental outcomes at age 5 among children born preterm: EPIPAGE-2 cohort study. BMJ 373. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinéda, R., Durant, P., Mathur, A., Inder, T., Wallendorf, M., and Schlaggar, B. L. (2017a). Auditory exposure in the neonatal intensive care unit: room type and other predictors. J. Pediatr. 183, 56–66.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.12.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinéda, R., Guth, R., Herring, A., Reynolds, L., Oberle, S., and Smith, J. (2017b). Enhancing sensory experiences for very preterm infants in the NICU: an integrative review. J. Perinatol. 37, 323–332. doi: 10.1038/jp.2016.179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pino, O. (2016). Fetal memory: The effects of prenatal auditory experience on human development. BAOJ Med. Nurs. 2:20.

Google Scholar

Piontelli, A. (2010). Development of Normal fetal Movements: The first 25 weeks of Gestation. Milan: Springer.

Google Scholar

Provasi, J. (2019). “Parent-preterm interaction,” in Early Interaction and Developmental Psychopathology, Volume I: Infancy. eds. G. Apter, E. Devouche, and M. Gratier (Cham: Springer), 123–149.

Google Scholar

Provasi, J., Anderson, D. I., and Barbu-Roth, M. (2014). Rhythm perception, production, and synchronization during the perinatal period. Front. Psychol. 5:1048. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Provasi, J., Blanc, L., and Carchon, I. (2021). The importance of rhythmic stimulation for preterm infants in the NICU. Children 8:660. doi: 10.3390/children8080660Academic

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raiskila, S., Axelin, A., Toome, L., Caballero, S., Tandberg, B. S., Montirosso, R., et al. (2017). Parents' presence and parent–infant closeness in 11 neonatal intensive care units in six European countries vary between and within the countries. Acta Paediatr. 106, 878–888. doi: 10.1111/apa.13798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reissland, N., and Kisilevsky, B. (Eds.). (2015). Fetal Development: Research on brain and Behavior, Environmental Influences, Andemerging Technologies. London: Springer Verlag.

Google Scholar

Roué, J. M., Kuhn, P., Maestro, M. L., Maastrup, R. A., Mitanchez, D., Westrup, B., et al. (2017). Eight principles for patient-centred and family-centred care for newborns in the neonatal intensive care unit. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 102, F364–F368. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2016-312180

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saliba, S., Esseily, R., Filippa, M., Gratier, M., and Grandjean, D. (2020). Changes in the vocal qualities of mothers and fathers are related to preterm infant’s behavioural states. Acta Paediatr. 109, 2271–2277. doi: 10.1111/apa.15238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segre, L. S., McCabe, J. E., Chuffo-Siewert, R., and O’Hara, M. W. (2014). Depression and anxiety symptoms in mothers of newborns hospitalized on the neonatal intensive care unit. Nurs. Res. 63, 320–332. doi: 10.1097/NNR.0000000000000039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidman, G., Unnikrishnan, S., Kenny, E., Myslinski, S., Cairns-Smith, S., Mulligan, B., et al. (2015). Barriers and enablers of kangaroo mother care practice: a systematic review. PLoS One 10:e0125643. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0125643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skene, C., Gerrish, K., Price, F., Pilling, E., Bayliss, P., and Gillespie, S. (2019). Developing family-centred care in a neonatal intensive care unit: an action research study. Intensive Crit. Care Nurs. 50, 54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.iccn.2018.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, D. N.. The Interpersonal World of the infant: a view from Psychoanalysis and Developmental Psychology. New York: Basic Books; (1985).

Google Scholar

Tallandini, M. A., and Scalembra, C. (2006). Kangaroo mother care and mother-preterm infant dyadic interaction. Infant Ment. Health J. 27, 251–275. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomson, G., Moran, V. H., Axelin, A., Dykes, F., and Flacking, R. (2013). Integrating a sense of coherence into the neonatal environment. BMC Paediatr. 13:84. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-13-84

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (1977). “Descriptive analysis of infant communicative behavior,” in Studies in Mother-Infant Interaction. ed. H. Schaffer (New York: Academic Press), 227–270.

Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (1993). “The self born in Intersubjectivity: The psychology of an infant communicating” in The Perceived Self: Ecological and Interpersonal Sources of Self-Knowledge. ed. U. Neisser. ed. ed (New York: Cambridge University Press), 121–173.

Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (1998). “The concept of foundations of infant intersubjectivity,” in Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny. ed. S. Bråten (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 15–47.

Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2011). What is it like to be a person who knows nothing? Defining the active intersubjective mind of a newborn human being. Infant Child Dev. 20, 119–135. doi: 10.1002/icd.689

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2012). Embodied human intersubjectivity: imaginative agency, to share meaning. J. Cogn. Semiot. 4, 6–56. doi: 10.1515/cogsem.2012.4.1.6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2016). The spiritual nature of the infant self: An imaginative actor in relations of affection. J. Conscious. Stud. 23, 258–282.

Google Scholar

Tronick, E. D., Als, H., and Brazelton, T. B. (1977). Mutuality in mother infant interaction. J. Commun. 27, 74–79. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1977.tb01829.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaivre-Douret, L., Ennouri, K., Jrad, I., Garrec, C., and Papiernik, E. (2004). Effect of positioning on the incidence of abnormalities of muscle tone in low-risk, preterm infants. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 8, 21–34. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2003.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varendi, H., Porter, R. H., and Winberg, J. (1997). Natural odour preferences of newborn infants change over time. Acta Paediatr. 86, 985–990.

Google Scholar

Voegtline, K. M., Costigan, K. A., Pater, H., and DiPetro, J. A. (2013). Near-term fetal response to maternal spoken voice. Infant Behav. Dev. 36, 526–533.

Google Scholar

World Health Organization. Kangaroo Mother Care: A Practical Guide. Geneva: Departement of Reproductive Health and Research, WHO; (2003).

Google Scholar

Zelkowitz, P., Papageorgiou, A., Bardin, C., and Wang, T. (2009). Persistent maternal anxiety affects the interaction between mothers and their very low birthweight children at 24 months. Early Hum. Dev. 85, 51–58. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2008.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, X. H., and Zhang, Z. H. (2020). Risk factors for postpartum depression: An evidence-based systematic review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Asian J. Psychiatr. 53:102353.

Google Scholar

Keywords: first skin-to-skin contact, SDF positioning, mother-very preterm newborn interaction, innate intersubjectivity, Kangaroo Care, NICU, vocal production, very premature birth

Citation: Buil A, Sankey C, Caeymaex L, Gratier M, Apter G, Vitte L and Devouche E (2022) Skin-to-skin SDF positioning: The key to intersubjective intimacy between mother and very preterm newborn—A pilot matched-pair case-control study. Front. Psychol. 13:790313. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.790313

Received: 06 October 2021; Accepted: 22 August 2022;
Published: 11 October 2022.

Edited by:

Jonathan T. Delafield-Butt, University of Strathclyde, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Eystein Våpenstad, Inland Norway University of Applied Sciences, Norway
Mikael Heimann, Linköping University, Sweden

Copyright © 2022 Buil, Sankey, Caeymaex, Gratier, Apter, Vitte and Devouche. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Emmanuel Devouche, devouche7@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.