Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 10 February 2022
Sec. Cognition

Local Prefrontal Cortex TMS-Induced Reactivity Is Related to Working Memory and Reasoning in Middle-Aged Adults

  • 1Institut Guttmann, Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitació adscrit a la Universitat Autónoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 2Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 3Fundació Institut d'Investigació en Ciències de la Salut Germans Trias i Pujol, Barcelona, Spain
  • 4Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain
  • 5Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina i Ciències de la Salut, i Institut de Neurociències, Universitat de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 6Departamento de Ciencias del Comportamiento, Escuela de Psicología, Universidad Metropolitana, Caracas, Venezuela
  • 7Center for Cognitive and Brain Health, Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States
  • 8Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, the Sahlgrenska Academy at the University of Gothenburg, Mölndal, Sweden
  • 9Clinical Neurochemistry Laboratory, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Mölndal, Sweden
  • 10Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, University College London Institute of Neurology, London, United Kingdom
  • 11UK Dementia Research Institute, University College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 12Hong Kong Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Hong Kong, China
  • 13Hinda and Arthur Marcus Institute for Aging Research and Deanna and Sidney Wolk Center for Memory Health, Hebrew SeniorLife, Boston, MA, United States
  • 14Department of Neurology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States

Introduction: The prefrontal cortex (PFC) plays a crucial role in cognition, particularly in executive functions. Cortical reactivity measured with Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation combined with Electroencephalography (TMS-EEG) is altered in pathological conditions, and it may also be a marker of cognitive status in middle-aged adults. In this study, we investigated the associations between cognitive measures and TMS evoked EEG reactivity and explored whether the effects of this relationship were related to neurofilament light chain levels (NfL), a marker of neuroaxonal damage.

Methods: Fifty two healthy middle-aged adults (41–65 years) from the Barcelona Brain Health Initiative cohort underwent TMS-EEG, a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment, and a blood test for NfL levels. Global and Local Mean-Field Power (GMFP/LMFP), two measures of cortical reactivity, were quantified after left prefrontal cortex (L-PFC) stimulation, and cognition was set as the outcome of the regression analysis. The left inferior parietal lobe (L-IPL) was used as a control stimulation condition.

Results: Local reactivity was significantly associated with working memory and reasoning only after L-PFC stimulation. No associations were found between NfL and cognition. These specific associations were independent of the status of neuroaxonal damage indexed by the NfL biomarker and remained after adjusting for age, biological sex, and education.

Conclusion: Our results demonstrate that TMS evoked EEG reactivity at the L-PFC, but not the L-IPL, is related to the cognitive status of middle-aged individuals and independent of NfL levels, and may become a valuable biomarker of frontal lobe-associated cognitive function.

Introduction

Cognitive functioning refers to a set of multiple mental abilities that involve the overall dynamics of information processing of stimuli (acquisition, coding, storage, retrieval, thinking, and decision making) to generate an adequate response (motor or verbal) to the environment (Lezak et al., 2004). The characterization of cognitive performance and cognitive profiles is typically carried out through specific domains referring to different processes and abilities within the global term “cognition” (Harvey, 2019), such as attention, episodic memory, working memory, reasoning, fluid intelligence, language, cognitive flexibility, visuospatial skills, and processing speed.

These cognitive domains have been related to specific anatomic areas and brain networks (Wu et al., 2020), and amongst them, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) plays a central role. The PFC is connected to almost all sensory, motor, neocortical and subcortical structures and is often implicated in “top-down” modulation of cognitive functions (Miller, 2000). In a hierarchical model of the neurophysiology of the cortex, the PFC constitutes the highest area of cortical representations (as opposed to lower cortical structures such as sensory and motor areas) dedicated to the integration and execution of higher-order executive functions (Fuster, 2001; Breukelaar et al., 2018). As these functions are significantly affected during aging, the role of the PFC has been extensively studied, and it has been shown that greater PFC activity is associated with better cognition (Eyler et al., 2011; Fernandez-Ruiz et al., 2018) and that preservation of PFC activity contributes to the maintenance of cognitive abilities (Morcom and Henson, 2018; Vidal-Piñeiro et al., 2019).

Beyond correlational evidence from brain-behavior investigations, direct experimental data using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to modulate older adults' PFC function demonstrated that high-frequency rTMS delivered over the bilateral PFC can enhance cognitive functioning (e.g. Solé-Padullés et al., 2006; Cui et al., 2020). If interpreted in the framework of theoretical models of cognitive aging, this enhancement may be due to the promotion of compensatory mechanisms by rTMS (Cabeza et al., 2018).

Given the role of the PFC in cognition and its relevance in healthy aging, the characterization of its neurophysiological activity in middle-aged adults, often understudied as compared with other age brackets (Willis et al., 2010; Lachman, 2015), could represent a valuable biomarker predictive of cognitive decline in older adults (McGinnis et al., 2011). Furthermore, research in middle-aged populations is relevant because changes in brain function can occur decades prior to the onset of clinically measurable symptoms (Beason-Held et al., 2013).

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation combined with Electroencephalography (TMS-EEG) is a non-invasive approach that allows the study of cortical reactivity via the perturbation of a cortical site and registration of the activity spread throughout the brain (Ilmoniemi and Kičić, 2010; Hallett et al., 2017). The spatiotemporal analysis of this reactivity and propagation allowed previous studies to explore functional network integrity in healthy and clinical populations (Pascual-Leone et al., 2011; Tremblay et al., 2019; Ozdemir et al., 2020).

Cortical reactivity has been defined as the relationship between the strength of the stimulus and the subsequent response (Komssi and Kähkönen, 2006). It is relevant because optimal excitatory and inhibitory cortical balance is needed for the correct functioning of the brain, connectivity between cortical regions, and cognitive functioning (Dehghani et al., 2016). Local and Global Mean-Field Power (GMFP/LMFP) reflect TMS-evoked brain reactivity on a specific subgroup of electrodes or throughout the entire brain, respectively (Lehmann and Skrandies, 1980; Komssi and Kähkönen, 2006; Romero Lauro et al., 2014). Both measures can reflect the electrical field distribution on the scalp (Skrandies, 1990). They could be valuable for measuring local reactivity directly on the stimulated region and reflecting its distribution to other brain areas (Lehmann and Skrandies, 1980). The neurophysiological effect of non-invasive protocols (repetitive TMS or Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation) has been studied through these measures in healthy adults (Casarotto et al., 2010; Romero Lauro et al., 2014; Pisoni et al., 2018; Ozdemir et al., 2021) and earlier research showed how these measures were directly related to pathological conditions, like depression (Voineskos et al., 2019). However, the relation between GMFP/LMFP and cognition in a healthy middle-aged population has not been studied before.

The goal of this study was to explore the relationship between cognition and local and global cortical reactivity after PFC stimulation using TMS-EEG in healthy middle-aged adults. Moreover, we investigated whether the effects are independent of a general measure of neuroaxonal damage and neurodegeneration as derived from plasma neurofilament light chain (NfL; Gisslén et al., 2016), a biomarker that has been associated with cognitive decline amongst elderly adults in the preclinical phase of Alzheimer's disease (AD) (Hu et al., 2019) and potentially in healthy middle-aged adults (Beydoun et al., 2021).

Materials and Methods

Subjects and Study Design

Fifty two middle-aged adults (36 male) between 41 and 65 years (M = 54, SD = 6.85) were recruited as part of the Barcelona Brain Health Initiative (Cattaneo et al., 2018). They underwent a TMS-EEG session, neuropsychological testing, a medical assessment with blood sample collection, and structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI). Participants were excluded during the medical visit if they had any neurological or neuropsychiatric disorders or used medications that could affect brain excitability or cognitive functions. Further exclusion criteria included contraindications for TMS (Rossini et al., 2015; Rossi et al., 2021) or MRI. All participants gave written informed consent, and the local ethics committee (Comité d'Ètica i Investigació Clínica de la Unió Catalana d'Hospitals) approved the study protocol, which conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki for research involving human subjects.

Neuropsychological Assessment

The neuropsychological assessment consisted of paper and pencil tests administered by two licensed neuropsychologists (VA, CP). The testing session lasted ~90 min and included 14 validated gold-standard instruments (see Cattaneo et al., 2018): Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) (Schmidt, 1996), Digit-Span Forward and Backward, Corsi block tapping test, Letter-Number Sequencing test (Peña-Casanova et al., 2012), Trail Making Test A and B (TMT) (Reitan and Wolfson, 1985; Peña-Casanova et al., 2012), Matrix Reasoning and Block design, the Digit symbol task, and the Cancellation test (Wechsler, 2012).

Raw scores of each test were transformed into z-scores and, similar to our previous reports (Vidal-Piñeiro et al., 2014; España-Irla et al., 2021), were grouped into five cognitive domains: episodic memory (RAVLT immediate recall, delayed recall, and recognition; Digit-span forward; Corsi block tapping), working memory (Digit-span backward; Letter-number sequencing), reasoning (Matrix and Block design WAIS-IV), flexibility (TMT B and B-A), and processing speed (TMT A; Digit symbol test; Cancellation test).

TMS Protocol

Participants were asked to sit in a comfortable armchair, look at a fixation cross placed at a 1.5 m distance, stay still, and keep their eyes open. The coil was placed tangentially over the scalp roughly at a 45-degree angle (relative to the mid-sagittal plane), resulting in a posterior-to-anterior current flow. A frameless stereotactic neuronavigation system (Brainsight, Rogue Research Inc., Montreal, QC Canada) was used with the subject's T1 weighted structural MRI (obtained from a 3T Siemens Magnetom Prisma) to ensure accurate targeting of the stimulation sites throughout the session.

Participant's Resting Motor Threshold (RMT) was assessed at the motor hotspot (M1) of the dominant hemisphere following the recommendations from the International Federation for Clinical Neurophysiology (Rossini et al., 2015; Rossi et al., 2021). Briefly, RMT was defined as the lowest stimulation intensity required to produce motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) of ≥ 50 μV in the relaxed first dorsal interosseous muscle (FDI) in five out of 10 trials. MEPs were measured using surface electromyography (EMG) with electrodes placed in a belly-tendon montage and the ground electrode on the ulnar styloid process and connected to a Biopac EMG100C amplifier (BIOPAC Systems Inc., California, USA). Handedness was assessed by the Edinburgh handedness questionnaire (Oldfield, 1971; Veale, 2014).

TMS was applied using a Medtronic Magpro X100 stimulator through a Cool-B65 figure-of-eight coil. One hundred and twenty biphasic single pulses were applied at 120% of RMT at random intervals between 3 and 6 s. Also, stimulation was applied over two target locations: the left prefrontal cortex (L-PFC) and a control target, the Inferior Parietal Lobule (L-IPL). The cohort had two different target procedures, based on the anatomy of each subject or using a cortical functional parcellation (Yeo et al., 2011) (See Supplementary Material for details on targeting procedure).

Due to time constraints during the experimental sessions, 77% of the individuals completed L-PFC stimulation (a total of 40 participants) and 67% L-IPL (35 participants), with 44% of them completing both conditions (23 participants). The complete TMS procedure lasted 2 h.

EEG Recordings

The EEG equipment used to record EEG responses to TMS was made up of a TMS-compatible EEG amplifier (ActiChamp system, Brain Products, GmbH, Munich, Germany) attached to 64 active electrodes (ActiCAP slim, Brain Products, GmbH, Munich, Germany), following the 10–20 international system for electrode montage. The ground was placed at the Fpz electrode site, and the signal was referenced to the AFz electrode. Electrode impedances were kept below 5 kΩ during the recording, and a continuous signal was collected, filtered DC to 500 Hz, and digitized at a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz. Besides wearing earplugs to protect from the “click” of the TMS pulse, subjects listened to white noise during stimulation to dampen the auditory evoked potential. The volume of the white noise was individually adjusted to each subjects' tolerance, and it was played through an active noise-canceling inserted earphone (Beoplay E4, Bang&Olufsen, Denmark).

EEG Preprocessing

The EEG signal was first preprocessed offline with custom MATLAB scripts (R2020b, The MathWorks Inc., Natick, Massachusetts) that incorporate function from the EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) and TESA plugin (Rogasch et al., 2017). The EEG signal was epoched around the TMS pulse (−1,000 to +2,000 ms) and baseline corrected (−900 to −100 ms). Excessively noisy channels were removed, but no more than three channels had to be discarded for any subject. Data was zero-padded between −2 and +14 ms around the TMS pulse to remove the early TMS pulse artifact. Epochs were inspected visually, and excessively noisy epochs were removed (M = 19, SD = 7). A two-step fast Independent Component Analysis (fICA) was conducted. The first fICA, was performed with Principal Component Analysis dimension reduced to 40 to minimize overfitting and was used to remove the decay artifact, typically 2 components were removed for each subject. Before the second round of fICA, the zero-padded TMS pulse was linearly interpolated, Butterworth band-pass (1 and 100 Hz) and notch (48 and 52 Hz) filters were applied, and data were re-referenced to the average reference. The second round of fICA was used to remove any remaining artifacts, including eye blinks, lateral eye movement, muscle, TMS-evoked muscle, electrode noise, and auditory evoked potentials (M = 28, SD = 3; a range of 21–31 out of 38). Finally, initially discarded channels were spline interpolated.

Cortical Reactivity TMS-EEG Measures

Global Mean-Field Power (GMFP) and Local Mean-Field Power (LMFP) were used to quantify overall and local brain reactivity measures, respectively.

Mean Field-Power (MFP) was calculated for both measures using the following formula:

MFP(t)=[ik(Vi(t)Vmean(t))2]K

where “t” is time, “V” is the voltage in the channel “I,” “K” is the number of channels, and “Vmean” the mean of the voltage across electrodes (Lehmann and Skrandies, 1980; Esser et al., 2006).

All EEG electrodes were used to compute GMFP, whereas, for LMFP, a subset of electrodes was chosen for L-PFC (FC1, FC3, FC5, F1, F3, F5) and L-IPL (CP1, CP3, CP5, P1, P3, P5) (Ozdemir et al., 2020). LMFP was used to calculate the local reactivity of the stimulation target region.

For GMFP and LMFP, the area under the curve was calculated using trapezoidal integration within two-time windows, before and after the pulse (Baseline and Post-stimulation). Baseline refers to the activity before each pulse (−500 to −3 ms), while Post-stimulation activity includes data between 15 and 400 ms after the TMS pulse. This time window has been selected to minimize the TMS artifact's impact and capture the entirety of the TMS evoked brain response (Fuggetta et al., 2005; Van Der Werf et al., 2006). This time window has been used in recent and similar research (Ozdemir et al., 2020; Rocchi et al., 2020; Vallesi et al., 2021). Furthermore, baseline data was used to normalize the activity post-stimulation, subtracting it from the activity post-TMS.

NfL Measurement

We collected blood samples using EDTA tubes during the medical assessment, and plasma was aliquoted and stored in a refrigerator at −80°C in a biobank facility following standard procedures usually employed for clinical purposes. Plasma NfL concentration was measured using the Single-molecule array (Simoa) NF-light Advantage Kit on an HD-X instrument as described by the kit manufacturer (Quanterix, Billerica, MA). The limit of quantification was 2.7 pg/mL, and the limit of detection was 0.3 pg/mL. For the quality control (QC) sample with an 11.2 pg/mL concentration, repeatability was 3.6%, and intermediate precision was 5.0%. For a QC sample with a 115 pg/mL concentration, repeatability was 5.3%, and intermediate precision was 6.8%. The measurements were performed at the Clinical Neurochemistry Laboratory at the University of Gothenburg by board-certified laboratory technicians who were blinded to clinical data.

Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were performed in SPSS version 22.0 (Statistical Package for Social Sciences, Chicago, IL, USA).

First, to explore changes between local and global reactivity before and after stimulation, we ran a repeated-measures ANOVA using the variable “Time” (Baseline and Post-stimulation) and “Mean-Field Type” (LMFP and GMFP) as within-subject factors.

Then, to investigate the relation between TMS reactivity, at local and global levels, and cognition, we ran multivariate multiple regressions for each stimulation site (PFC, IPL). L-IPL was used as a control condition to validate if our associations with cognition in L-PFC results were specific to this target. Models were run using cognitive composite scores as dependent variables (episodic memory, working memory, reasoning, flexibility, and processing speed) and Mean-Field Type, targeting method, NfL levels, age, biological sex, and years of education as predictors. Also, we run the same models without using covariates (targeting method, NfL levels, age, biological sex, and years of education).

Finally, to specifically explore the associations between NfL level and cognition, we first performed a bivariate correlation, and then to see the effect of age, we ran a partial correlation controlled by this variable.

Results

All subjects were right-handed and tolerated well the experimental procedures, and no adverse events were reported. Descriptive statistics of age, educational level, RMT, and plasma NfL levels are presented in Table 1, while cognitive scores are in the Supplementary Material (Supplementary Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic Variables, RMT and NfL plasma values (n = 52).

TMS Cortical Reactivity Changes

L-PFC

Repeated measures ANOVA showed a main effect of Time [F(1, 39) = 54.05, p < 0.001], indicating difference between pre and post stimulation, and a main effect of the Mean-Field Type [F(1, 39) = 69.02, p < 0.001], indicating that local reactivity was greater (M = 262.32; SD = 13.90) than global cortical reactivity (M = 206.91; SD = 9.54). The significant interaction between Time and Mean-Field Type [F(1, 39) = 145.97, p < 0.001] indicated a difference in the effects of stimulation between “local” and “global” conditions. While LMFP showed significant differences between baseline and post-TMS [t(39) = 13.56, p < 0.001, d = 2.14] GMFP did not [t(39) = 1.16, p = 0.253, d = 0.18; see Figure 1].

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. L-PFC. Graphical representation of local (LMFP) and global (GMFP) reactivity, at baseline and after stimulation.

L-IPL

Repeated measures ANOVA also showed a main effect of Time [F(1, 34) = 20.01, p < 0.001], indicating difference between pre and post stimulation, and a main effect of the Mean-Field Type [F(1, 34) = 89.38, p < 0.001] showing that local reactivity was greater (M = 260.10; SD = 15.78) than global cortical reactivity (M = 200.63; SD = 12.02). The significant interaction between Time and Mean-Field Type [F(1, 34) =143.96, p < 0.001] suggested difference in the effects of stimulation between “local” and “global” conditions. Similarly to the L-PFC analysis, while LMFP showed significative differences between baseline and post-TMS [t(34) = 8.55, p < 0.001, d = 1.44] GMFP did not [t(34) = 0.311, p = 0.758, d = 0.05; see Figure 2]. Also, an example of TMS-EEG responses after a PFC and IPL pulse is presented in Figure 3.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. L-IPL. Graphical representation of LFMP and GFMP at baseline and after stimulation.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Butterfly plots of a subject's TMS-EEG responses after PFC (A) and IPL (B) stimulation. Each figure time-series are plotted −100 to +400 ms around the TMS pulse.

Cortical Reactivity and Cognitive Functions Associations

L-PFC

Age and local reactivity resulted in statistically significant results for the multivariate regression analysis [respectively F(5, 28) = 3.28, p < 0.019, Wilks' Λ = 0.631, partial η2 = 0.369; F(5, 28) = 2.91, p = 0.031, Wilks' Λ = 0.658, partial η2 = 0.342; see Supplementary Table 2]. The analysis revealed a significant positive association between working memory and local reactivity to stimulation of the L-PFC [F(1, 32) = 5.01, p = 0.032, partial η2 = 0.135], as well as an association between reasoning and both age [F(1, 32) = 6.76, p = 0.014, partial η2= 0.174] and local reactivity to L-PFC stimulation [F(1, 32) = 4.70, p = 0.038, partial η2= 0.128] (see Figure 4). Episodic memory, processing speed, and flexibility were unrelated to the independent variables introduced in the model. Full model results including covariates can be found in Supplementary Table 3.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Multiple regression scatterplots between local cortical reactivity after PFC stimulation and working memory (A) and reasoning (B) after controlling for targeting method, NfL levels, age, biological sex, and years of education. Z-scores were used on the Y-axis and unstandardized Predicted Values on the X-axis (μV).

Conversely, no significant results were seen between cognition and cortical reactivity in models without covariates (see Supplementary Tables 4, 5).

L-IPL

For stimulation to left IPL, no statistically significant relations were found between any of the cognitive functions and either global or local TMS-EEG induced reactivity measures at baseline or post-stimulation. Model results including covariates are presented in Supplementary Tables 6, 7. Also, no significant results were seen between cognition and cortical reactivity in models without covariates (see Supplementary Tables 8, 9).

NfL Levels and Cognitive Functions

We first ran a bivariate correlation between NfL levels and cognitive variables and found that it was significantly and inversely correlated to participants' cognitive performance in reasoning (r = −0.373, p = 0.006), processing speed (r = −0.338, p = 0.014), and cognitive flexibility (r = −0.293, p = 0.035; see Supplementary Table 10). However, when controlled for age, such associations disappeared, indicating that the age of participants largely drove the correlations. In addition, and as revealed by the multivariate regression analyses, NfL concentration was not significantly associated with cognitive status (see Supplementary Tables 3, 7).

Discussion

In the present study, we explored the relationship between EEG reactivity to TMS of the PFC and IPL and cognition in healthy middle-aged adults, and the possible role of neuroaxonal damage measured by plasma NfL. Results indicate that local TMS-EEG reactivity after PFC stimulation is positively associated with executive functions, specifically working memory and reasoning. However, after IPL stimulation, cortical reactivity was not related to cognitive function. Finally, it was shown that neuroaxonal damage measured by NfL did not play a role in these associations. Despite that, a significant effect was seen when we correlated directly NfL level and cognitive functions, which disappeared when we controlled by age.

The fact that the relation between TMS-EEG reactivity and cognitive were limited to the local response following PFC stimulation, and not globally or when stimulating the L-IPL, is consistent with the specific PFC role in the top-down regulation of higher-order cognitive control (Miller, 2000). These results indicate that individual differences in local cortical reactivity to TMS of the PFC could be a practical, specific, sensitive, and simple biomarker to assess cognitive functioning, independently of the global brain and axonal degeneration.

Cortical reactivity has been previously related to factors such as alcohol or medication intake (Kähkönen et al., 2003; Khedr et al., 2020) and various TMS parameters (Casula et al., 2018). A rich literature has demonstrated that activity and connectivity between PFC and IPL are associated with cognition (Lückmann et al., 2014; Friedman and Robbins, 2021). In particular, whereas PFC has been more related to executive function and cognitive control (Friedman and Robbins, 2021), IPL has been associated with language and social cognition (Numssen et al., 2021). In line with our results, Ngetich et al. (2020) showed that after a continuous theta burst stimulation over L-PFC, there was a change in executive functions like working memory and decision making (Ngetich et al., 2020). Similarly, it has been shown that abnormal higher cortical excitability in the PFC in patients with AD than healthy controls was inversely associated with global cognition/executive functions (Joseph et al., 2021), confirming the relation between cognitive functions performance and PFC evoked activity.

The role of PFC in working memory has been extensively studied in the past decade with animals and humans, suggesting that PFC is strongly related to the cognitive process of maintaining available and select information for delayed responses (see Curtis and D'Esposito, 2003 for a review). It has been proposed that while the PFC is crucial to manipulate and select relevant information, a more posterior part of PFC (e.g., Brodmann area 8) is involved in mechanisms of maintenance (Rowe et al., 2000; Glahn et al., 2002). Also, complex reasoning tasks have been consistently associated with PFC activity and integrity. It has been proposed that PFC is strongly involved in logic processing (Santarnecchi et al., 2013), and specifically, its rostrolateral part is essential for relational integration and associations (Christoff et al., 2001; Krawczyk et al., 2011).

The association between cognition and PFC activity and connectivity is especially important in studying the maintenance of cognitive functioning in aging. Indeed it has been proposed that PFC activity could be related to the recruitment of compensatory mechanisms (Solé-Padullés et al., 2006; Höller-Wallscheid et al., 2017; Abellaneda-Pérez et al., 2019) that allow individuals to maintain cognition in the face of age-related brain changes.

Furthermore, NfL was shown to be negatively related to cognition, but this effect disappeared if controlled by the individual's age, and NfL level didn't have a significant influence on the identified relation between cortical reactivity and cognitive functions. NfL level is a marker of neuro-axonal damage in diseases such as Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease, multiple sclerosis, or amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, where high concentrations of NfL have been reported (Gaetani et al., 2019; Dhiman et al., 2020). Recent studies (Khalil et al., 2020; Beydoun et al., 2021; Rübsamen et al., 2021) have explored in healthy individuals the relation between NfL levels, brain structures, and cognitive scores, suggesting that higher NfL levels could be associated with brain atrophy, and in consequence worse cognition. Our study sample was limited to middle-aged, cognitively-unimpaired adults, and the fact that the level of NfL didn't influence the reactivity/cognition prediction could be because of collinearity between age and NfL level, or that most past results have focused on older adults or various patient populations. More studies in healthy middle-aged adults are needed to determine the significance and potential clinical utility of NfL plasma levels (Beydoun et al., 2021).

To conclude, our results indicate that cortical reactivity of L-PFC as characterized by TMS-EEG is related to cognition in middle-aged adults regardless of neuroaxonal damage (indicated by NfL), age, biological sex, and education. This TMS-EEG metric may represent a valuable and independent biomarker for cognition.

As with all studies, the design of the current study is subject to limitations. First, we acknowledge that our sample size was small, but still, it was in line with other studies whose objective was to associate TMS measures with cognition. Second, our sample was characterized by highly educated individuals, and there was a high prevalence of men. Furthermore, our statistical analysis was done for each stimulation site separately because of missing data. Given the small sample sizes, multiple comparisons corrections were not applied to maintain statistical power and avoid strongly increasing the probability of type II errors. Hence, further studies are needed, including more participants with both PFC and IPL stimulation data to confirm these results. Finally, a layer of foam between the coil and the electrodes was not used in this research, and despite this preventive measure could add some distance, increasing the resting motor threshold and the effect varies between subjects (ter Braack et al., 2015), it could have been beneficial for the reduction of auditory evoked potentials in the EEG analysis. Future investigations with larger and more heterogeneous samples are necessary to validate the conclusions of our study, and it would be valuable to explore if changes in cortical reactivity, measured longitudinally, may be predictive of age-related changes in cognition.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Comité d'Ètica i Investigació Clínica de la Unió Catalana d'Hospitals. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

AP-L, DB-F, and JT-M participated in the initial conception of the design of the BBHI project. MR-C, DB-F, AP-L, GC, and RP-A contributed to conception and design of the present study. MR-C, SD-G, GE-I, VA-S, CP-G, SA, JS-S, and TM contributed to the acquisition of data. MR-C, GC, TM, HZ, and RP-A analyzed the data. MR-C, GC, and DB-F contributed to the draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

DB-F and RP-A were funded by a Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities (MICIU/FEDER; RTI2018-095181-B-C21) and an ICREA Academia 2019 Research Grants, and also supported by an ICREA Academia 2019 Grand Award, RP-A was supported by a fellowship from la Caixa Foundation (ID 100010434, Fellowship code: LCF/BQ/DI19/11730050), JT-M was partly supported Fundació Joan Ribas (Araquistain_FJRA), AGAUR, Agència de Gestió d'Ajuts Universitaris i de Recerca. Convocatòria 2018 d'Indústria del Coneixement (modalitat PRODUCTE) and FEDER, Fons Europeu de Desenvolupament Regional (2018 PROD 00172), Fundació La Marató De TV3 (201735.10), and European Commission - H2020/Call H2020-SC1-2016-2017 (RIA) (Grant Agreement No. 777107), AP-L was partly supported by the National Institutes of Health (R24AG06142, and P01 AG031720), the National Science Foundation, DARPA, and the Barcelona Brain Health Initiative funded primarily by La Caixa (LCF/PR/PR16/11110004), and HZ is a Wallenberg Scholar supported by grants from the Swedish Research Council (#2018-02532), the European Research Council (#681712), Swedish State Support for Clinical Research (#ALFGBG-720931), the Alzheimer Drug Discovery Foundation (ADDF), USA (#201809-2016862), the AD Strategic Fund and the Alzheimer's Association (#ADSF-21-831376-C, #ADSF-21-831381-C and #ADSF-21-831377-C), the Olav Thon Foundation, the Erling-Persson Family Foundation, Stiftelsen för Gamla Tjänarinnor, Hjärnfonden, Sweden (#FO2019-0228), the European Union's Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Programme under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No 860197 (MIRIADE), and the UK Dementia Research Institute at UCL.

Conflict of Interest

AP-L is a co-founder of Linus Health and TI Solutions AG; serves on the scientific advisory boards for Starlab Neuroscience, Magstim Inc., Radiant Hearts, and MedRhythms; is listed as an inventor on several issued and pending patents on the real-time integration of non-invasive brain stimulation with electroencephalography and magnetic resonance imaging and is an Associate Editor for Annals of Neurology. HZ has served at scientific advisory boards and/or as a consultant for Abbvie, Alector, Annexon, Artery Therapeutics, AZTherapies, CogRx, Denali, Eisai, Nervgen, Pinteon Therapeutics, Red Abbey Labs, Passage Bio, Roche, Samumed, Siemens Healthineers, Triplet Therapeutics, and Wave, has given lectures in symposia sponsored by Cellectricon, Fujirebio, Alzecure and Biogen, and is a co-founder of Brain Biomarker Solutions in Gothenburg AB (BBS), which is a part of the GU Ventures Incubator Program (outside submitted work).

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

A special thanks is extended to all BBHI participants, the project team and research assistants, and other partners (Ad-Salutem Institute, Sodexo, I.C.A. Informàtica y Comunicaciones Avanzadas, Neuroelectrics, Corporació Catalana de Mitjans Audiovisuals, Club Metropolitan, Casa Ametller, and Agència de Qualitat i Avaluació Sanitàries de Catalunya-AQuAS) for their collaboration.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.813444/full#supplementary-material

References

Abellaneda-Pérez, K., Vaqué-Alcázar, L., Solé-Padullés, C., and Bartrés-Faz, D. (2019). Combining non-invasive brain stimulation with functional magnetic resonance imaging to investigate the neural substrates of cognitive aging. J. Neurosci. Res. doi: 10.1002/jnr.24514 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beason-Held, L. L., Goh, J. O., An, Y., Kraut, M. A., O'Brien, R. J., Ferrucci, L., et al. (2013). Changes in brain function occur years before the onset of cognitive impairment. J. Neurosci. 33, 18008–18014. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1402-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beydoun, M. A., Noren Hooten, N., Beydoun, H. A., Maldonado, A. I., Weiss, J., Evans, M. K., et al. (2021). Plasma neurofilament light as a potential biomarker for cognitive decline in a longitudinal study of middle-aged urban adults. Transl. Psychiatry 11, 1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01563-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breukelaar, I. A., Williams, L. M., Antees, C., Grieve, S. M., Foster, S. L., Gomes, L., et al. (2018). Cognitive ability is associated with changes in the functional organization of the cognitive control brain network. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 5028–5038. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Albert, M., Belleville, S., Craik, F. I. M., Duarte, A., Grady, C. L., et al. (2018). Maintenance, reserve and compensation: the cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 701–710. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casarotto, S., Lauro, L. J. R., Bellina, V., Casali, A. G., Rosanova, M., Pigorini, A., et al. (2010). EEG responses to TMS are sensitive to changes in the perturbation parameters and repeatable over time. PLoS One 5, e10281. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casula, E. P., Rocchi, L., Hannah, R., and Rothwell, J. C. (2018). Effects of pulse width, waveform and current direction in the cortex: A combined cTMS-EEG study. Brain Stimul. 11, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2018.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cattaneo, G., Bartrés-Faz, D., Morris, T. P., Sánchez, J. S., Macià, D., Tarrero, C., et al. (2018). The Barcelona brain health initiative: a cohort study to define and promote determinants of brain health. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10, 32‘. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christoff, K., Prabhakaran, V., Dorfman, J., Zhao, Z., Kroger, J. K., Holyoak, K. J., et al. (2001). Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in relational integration during reasoning. Neuroimage 14, 1136–1149. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, X., Ren, W., Zheng, Z., and Li, J. (2020). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation improved source memory and modulated recollection-based retrieval in healthy older adults. Front. Psychol. 11, 1137. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtis, C. E., and D'Esposito, M. (2003). Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 415–423. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00197-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehghani, N., Peyrache, A., Telenczuk, B., Le Van Quyen, M., Halgren, E., Cash, S. S., et al. (2016). Dynamic balance of excitation and inhibition in human and monkey neocortex. Sci. Rep. 6, 23176. doi: 10.1038/srep23176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhiman, K., Gupta, V. B., Villemagne, V. L., Eratne, D., Graham, P. L., Fowler, C., et al. (2020). Cerebrospinal fluid neurofilament light concentration predicts brain atrophy and cognition in Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's Dement. 12, e12005. doi: 10.1002/dad2.12005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

España-Irla, G., Gomes-Osman, J., Cattaneo, G., Albu, S., Cabello-Toscano, M., Solana-Sanchéz, J., et al. (2021). Associations between cardiorespiratory fitness, cardiovascular risk, and cognition are mediated by structural brain health in midlife. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 10, e020688. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.120.020688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esser, S. K., Huber, R., Massimini, M., Peterson, M. J., Ferrarelli, F., and Tononi, G. (2006). A direct demonstration of cortical LTP in humans: A combined TMS/EEG study. Brain Res. Bull. 69, 86–94. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eyler, L. T., Sherzai, A., Kaup, A. R., and Jeste, D. V. (2011). A review of functional brain imaging correlates of successful cognitive aging. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 115–122. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.12.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez-Ruiz, J., Peltsch, A., Alahyane, N., Brien, D. C., Coe, B. C., Garcia, A., et al. (2018). Age related prefrontal compensatory mechanisms for inhibitory control in the antisaccade task. Neuroimage 165, 92–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., and Robbins, T. W. (2021). The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology, 47, 72–89. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01132-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuggetta, G., Fiaschi, A., and Manganotti, P. (2005). Modulation of cortical oscillatory activities induced by varying single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation intensity over the left primary motor area: a combined EEG and TMS study. Neuroimage 27, 896–908. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuster, J. M (2001). The prefrontal cortex - an update: time is of the essence. Neuron 30, 319–333. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00285-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaetani, L., Blennow, K., Calabresi, P., Di Filippo, M., Parnetti, L., and Zetterberg, H. (2019). Neurofilament light chain as a biomarker in neurological disorders. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 870–881. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2018-320106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gisslén, M., Price, R. W., Andreasson, U., Norgren, N., Nilsson, S., Hagberg, L., et al. (2016). Plasma concentration of the Neurofilament Light Protein (NFL) is a biomarker of CNS Injury in HIV Infection: a cross-sectional study. EBioMedicine 3, 135–140. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2015.11.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glahn, D. C., Kim, J., Cohen, M. S., Poutanen, V. P., Therman, S., Bava, S., et al. (2002). Maintenance and manipulation in spatial working memory: dissociations in the prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 17, 201–213. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallett, M., Di Iorio, R., Rossini, P. M., Park, J. E., Chen, R., Celnik, P., et al. (2017). Contribution of transcranial magnetic stimulation to assessment of brain connectivity and networks. Clin. Neurophysiol. 128, 2125–2139. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, P. D (2019). Domains of cognition and their assessment. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 21, 227–237. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2019.21.3/pharvey

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Höller-Wallscheid, M. S., Thier, P., Pomper, J. K., and Lindner, A. (2017). Bilateral recruitment of prefrontal cortex in working memory is associated with task demand but not with age. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 114, E830–E839. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1601983114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, H., Chen, K. L., Ou, Y. N., Cao, X. P., Chen, S. D., Cui, M., et al. (2019). Neurofilament light chain plasma concentration predicts neurodegeneration and clinical progression in nondemented elderly adults. Aging (Albany. NY). 11, 6904–6914. doi: 10.18632/aging.102220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ilmoniemi, R. J., and Kičić, D. (2010). Methodology for combined TMS and EEG. Brain Topogr. 22, 233–248. doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0123-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joseph, S., Patterson, R., Wang, W., Blumberger, D. M., Rajji, T., and Kumar, S. (2021). Quantitative assessment of cortical excitability in alzheimer's dementia and its association with clinical symptoms: a systematic review and meta-analyses. J. Alzheimer's Dis. doi: 10.3233/JAD-210311 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kähkönen, S., Wilenius, J., Nikulin, V. V., Ollikainen, M., and Ilmoniemi, R. J. (2003). Alcohol reduces prefrontal cortical excitability in humans: A combined TMS and EEG study. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 747–754. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalil, M., Pirpamer, L., Hofer, E., Voortman, M. M., Barro, C., Leppert, D., et al. (2020). Serum neurofilament light levels in normal aging and their association with morphologic brain changes. Nat. Commun. 11:812. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-14612-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khedr, E. M., Tony, A. A., Abdelwarith, A., and Safwat, M. (2020). Effect of chronic nicotine consumption on motor cortical excitability: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neurophysiol. Clin. 50, 33–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2019.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Komssi, S., and Kähkönen, S. (2006). The novelty value of the combined use of electroencephalography and transcranial magnetic stimulation for neuroscience research. Brain Res. Rev. 52, 183–192. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krawczyk, D. C., Michelle McClelland, M., and Donovan, C. M. (2011). A hierarchy for relational reasoning in the prefrontal cortex. Cortex 47, 588–597. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lachman, M. E (2015). Mind the gap in the middle: a call to study midlife. Res. Hum. Dev. 12, 327–334. doi: 10.1080/15427609.2015.1068048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmann, D., and Skrandies, W. (1980). Reference-free identification of components of checkerboard-evoked multichannel potential fields. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 48, 609–621. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(80)90419-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lezak, M., Howieson, D., and Loring, D. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment, 4th Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Lückmann, H. C., Jacobs, H. I. L., and Sack, A. T. (2014). The cross-functional role of frontoparietal regions in cognition: internal attention as the overarching mechanism. Prog. Neurobiol. 116, 66–86. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGinnis, S. M., Brickhouse, M., Pascual, B., and Dickerson, B. C. (2011). Age-related changes in the thickness of cortical zones in humans. Brain Topogr. 24, 279–291. doi: 10.1007/s10548-011-0198-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K (2000). The prefontral cortex and cognitive control. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 59–65. doi: 10.1038/35036228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morcom, A. M., and Henson, R. N. A. (2018). Increased prefrontal activity with aging reflects nonspecific neural responses rather than compensation. J. Neurosci. 38, 7303–7313. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1701-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ngetich, R., Zhou, J., Zhang, J., Jin, Z., and Li, L. (2020). Assessing the effects of continuous theta burst stimulation over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on human cognition: a systematic review. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 14, 35. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2020.00035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Numssen, O., Bzdok, D., and Hartwigsen, G. (2021). Functional specialization within the inferior parietal lobes across cognitive domains. Elife 10. doi: 10.7554/eLife.63591.sa2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozdemir, R. A., Boucher, P., Fried, P. J., Momi, D., Jannati, A., Pascual-Leone, A., et al. (2021). Reproducibility of cortical response modulation induced by intermittent and continuous theta-burst stimulation of the human motor cortex. Brain Stimul. 14, 949–964. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2021.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozdemir, R. A., Tadayon, E., Boucher, P., Momi, D., Karakhanyan, K. A., Fox, M. D., et al. (2020). Individualized perturbation of the human connectome reveals reproducible biomarkers of network dynamics relevant to cognition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 117, 8115–8125. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1911240117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascual-Leone, A., Freitas, C., Oberman, L., Horvath, J. C., Halko, M., Edaief, M., et al. (2011). Characterizing brain cortical plasticity and network dynamics across the age-span in health and disease with TMS-EEG and TMS-fMRI. Brain Topogr. 23, 302–315. doi: 10.1007/s10548-011-0196-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peña-Casanova, J., Casals-Coll, M., Quintana, M., Sánchez-Benavides, G., Rognoni, T., Calvo, L., et al. (2012). Estudios normativos españoles en población adulta joven (Proyecto NEURONORMA jóvenes): métodos y características de la muestra. Neurologia 27, 253–260. doi: 10.1016/j.nrl.2011.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pisoni, A., Mattavelli, G., Papagno, C., Rosanova, M., Casali, A. G., and Lauro, L. J. R. (2018). Cognitive enhancement induced by Anodal tDCS drives circuit-specific cortical plasticity. Cereb. Cortex 28, 1132–1140. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M., and Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Interpretattion. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.

Google Scholar

Rocchi, L., Di Santo, A., Brown, K., Ibáñez, J., Casula, E., Rawji, V., et al. (2020). Disentangling EEG responses to TMS due to cortical and peripheral activations. Brain Stimul. 14, 4–18. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2020.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogasch, N. C., Sullivan, C., Thomson, R. H., Rose, N. S., Bailey, N. W., Fitzgerald, P. B., et al. (2017). Analysing concurrent transcranial magnetic stimulation and electroencephalographic data: a review and introduction to the open-source TESA software. Neuroimage 147, 934–951. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romero Lauro, L. J., Rosanova, M., Mattavelli, G., Convento, S., Pisoni, A., Opitz, A., et al. (2014). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) increases cortical excitability: direct evidence from TMS-EEG. Cortex 58, 99–111. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, S., Antal, A., Bestmann, S., Bikson, M., Brewer, C., Brockmöller, J., et al. (2021). Safety and recommendations for TMS use in healthy subjects and patient populations, with updates on training, ethical and regulatory issues: expert Guidelines. Clin. Neurophysiol. 132, 269–306. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2020.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossini, P. M., Burke, D., Chen, R., Cohen, L. G., Daskalakis, Z., Di Iorio, R., et al. (2015). Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and peripheral nerves: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research application: an updated report from an I.F.C.N. Committee. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 1071–1107. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, J. B., Toni, I., Josephs, O., Frackowiak, R. S. J., and Passingham, R. E. (2000). The prefrontal cortex: response selection or maintenance within working memory? Science (80-.) 288, 1656–1660. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5471.1656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rübsamen, N., Maceski, A., Leppert, D., Benkert, P., Kuhle, J., Wiendl, H., et al. (2021). Serum neurofilament light and tau as prognostic markers for all-cause mortality in the elderly general population—an analysis from the MEMO study. BMC Med. 19, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12916-021-01915-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santarnecchi, E., Polizzotto, N. R., Godone, M., Giovannelli, F., Feurra, M., Matzen, L., et al. (2013). Frequency-dependent enhancement of fluid intelligence induced by transcranial oscillatory potentials. Curr. Biol. 23, 1449–1453. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, M (1996). Rey Auditory and Verbal Learning Test: A Handbook. Los Ángeles: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Skrandies, W (1990). Global field power and topographic similarity. Brain Topogr. 3, 137–141. doi: 10.1007/BF01128870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solé-Padullés, C., Bartrés-Faz, D., Junqué, C., Clemente, I. C., Molinuevo, J. L., Bargalló, N., et al. (2006). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation effects on brain function and cognition among elders with memory dysfunction. A randomized sham-controlled study. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1487–1493. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ter Braack, E. M., de Vos, C. C., and van Putten, M. J. A. M. (2015). Masking the auditory evoked potential in TMS–EEG: a comparison of various methods. Brain Topogr. 28, 520–528. doi: 10.1007/s10548-013-0312-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, S., Rogasch, N. C., Premoli, I., Blumberger, D. M., Casarotto, S., Chen, R., et al. (2019). Clinical utility and prospective of TMS–EEG. Clin. Neurophysiol. 130, 802–844. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallesi, A., Del Felice, A., Capizzi, M., Tafuro, A., Formaggio, E., Bisiacchi, P., et al. (2021). Natural oscillation frequencies in the two lateral prefrontal cortices induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation: prefrontal natural frequencies. Neuroimage 227, 117655. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Der Werf, Y. D., Sadikot, A. F., Strafella, A. P., and Paus, T. (2006). The neural response to transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human motor cortex. II. Thalamocortical contributions. Exp. Brain Res. 175, 246–255. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0548-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veale, J. F (2014). Edinburgh Handedness Inventory - Short Form: A revised version based on confirmatory factor analysis. Laterality 19, 164–177. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2013.783045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidal-Piñeiro, D., Sneve, M. H., Nyberg, L. H., Mowinckel, A. M., Sederevicius, D., Walhovd, K. B., et al. (2019). Maintained frontal activity underlies high memory function over 8 years in aging. Cereb. Cortex 29, 3111–3123. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidal-Piñeiro, D., Valls-Pedret, C., Fernández-Cabello, S., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Sala-Llonch, R., Solana, E., et al. (2014). Decreased default mode network connectivity correlates with age-associated structural and cognitive changes. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6, 256. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voineskos, D., Blumberger, D. M., Zomorrodi, R., Rogasch, N. C., Farzan, F., Foussias, G., et al. (2019). Altered transcranial magnetic stimulation–electroencephalographic markers of inhibition and excitation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in major depressive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 85, 477–486. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.09.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D (2012). WAIS-IV. Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para adultos-IV. Manual De Aplicación Y Corrección. Madrid: NCS Pearson, Inc.

Google Scholar

Willis, S. L., Martin, M., and Rocke, C. (2010). Longitudinal perspectives on midlife development: Stability and change. Eur. J. Ageing 7, 131–134. doi: 10.1007/s10433-010-0162-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, T., Chen, C., Spagna, A., Wu, X., Mackie, M. A., Russell-Giller, S., et al. (2020). The functional anatomy of cognitive control: a domain-general brain network for uncertainty processing. J. Comp. Neurol. 528, 1265–1292. doi: 10.1002/cne.24804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeo, B. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari, D., Hollinshead, M., et al. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), Electroencephalography, TMS-EEG, cortical reactivity, prefrontal cortex (PFC), cognition

Citation: Redondo-Camós M, Cattaneo G, Perellón-Alfonso R, Alviarez-Schulze V, Morris TP, Solana-Sanchez J, España-Irla G, Delgado-Gallén S, Pachón-García C, Albu S, Zetterberg H, Tormos JM, Pascual-Leone A and Bartres-Faz D (2022) Local Prefrontal Cortex TMS-Induced Reactivity Is Related to Working Memory and Reasoning in Middle-Aged Adults. Front. Psychol. 13:813444. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.813444

Received: 11 November 2021; Accepted: 18 January 2022;
Published: 10 February 2022.

Edited by:

Valerio Santangelo, University of Perugia, Italy

Reviewed by:

Sanjeev Kumar, University of Toronto, Canada
Robert Blumenfeld, California State Polytechnic University, Pomona, United States
Aron T. Hill, Deakin University, Australia

Copyright © 2022 Redondo-Camós, Cattaneo, Perellón-Alfonso, Alviarez-Schulze, Morris, Solana-Sanchez, España-Irla, Delgado-Gallén, Pachón-García, Albu, Zetterberg, Tormos, Pascual-Leone and Bartres-Faz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: David Bartres-Faz, dbartres@ub.edu; Alvaro Pascual-Leone, apleone@hsl.harvard.edu

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.