Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 22 June 2022
Sec. Consciousness Research
This article is part of the Research Topic Rising Stars In: Consciousness Research 2021 View all 9 articles

Personal Memories and Bodily-Cues Influence Our Sense of Self

  • 1Department of Basic Neurosciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
  • 2Department of Neurology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States

How do our bodies influence who we are? Recent research in cognitive neuroscience has examined consciousness associated with the self and related multisensory processing of bodily signals, the so-called bodily self-consciousness. A parallel line of research has highlighted the concept of the autobiographical self and the associated autonoetic consciousness, which enables us to mentally travel in time. The subjective re-experiencing of past episodes is described as re-living them from within or outside one’s body. In this brief perspective, I aim to explore the underlying characteristics of self-consciousness and its relation to bodily signals and episodic memory. I will outline some recent behavioral and neuroimaging evidence indicating that bodily cues play a fundamental role in autobiographical memory. Finally, I will discuss these emerging concepts regarding the current understanding of bodily-self, autobiographical-self, their links to self-consciousness, and suggest directions for future research.

Introduction

The conscious experience of self-related events is perceived as embodied, i.e., our physical body is a central object in the world. We naturally experience and make sense of the world from an inside viewpoint of our physical body (Mach, 1897), yet we can retrieve memories from both within the body (i.e., own eyes/first-person perspective) or outside the body (i.e., observer/third-person perspective) (Nigro and Neisser, 1983; Rubin and Umanath, 2015). An essential aspect of consciousness is its link with a self, which is the subject of conscious experience. Self-consciousness originates at different levels, from a simple (i.e., the minimal level of unconscious experience) to a complex phenomenon (i.e., the core self and the autobiographical self) (Damasio, 1999). The core consciousness occurs when the brain continuously builds a mental representation of the minimal sense of self caused by an interaction with internal or external stimuli. This minimal sense of self is centered on integrating multisensory, interoceptive and exteroceptive bodily processing (e.g., vision, touch, proprioception, vestibular, and visceral signals) in the brain (Blanke et al., 2015; Park and Blanke, 2019). Manipulating the experience of one’s own body experimentally has been challenging. As James (1890), the pioneering psychologist and philosopher, pointed out in the nineteenth century: “The body is always there.” However, the recent advances in virtual reality technology enabled the experimental investigation of the global aspects of bodily self-consciousness, including the self-identification, self-location, and first-person perspective (Ehrsson, 2007; Lenggenhager et al., 2007). The bodily-self, which is the most fundamental aspect of self-consciousness, may influence higher cognitive aspects of self-representation, such as the autobiographical-self (Bergouignan et al., 2014; Bréchet et al., 2019, 2020; Tacikowski et al., 2020; Iriye and St. Jacques, 2021).

Conscious experiences, whether occurring from within or outside the body, are not always bound to here and now. What did you eat for breakfast yesterday? Where do you have to go today? When is your meeting tomorrow? The human mind can detach itself from the present moment and mentally travel to the past or imagine the future (Schacter et al., 2012). As Antonio Damasio (1999) defined, the autobiographical self represents a mental state derived from retrieving self-relevant memories. No scene captures the human ability to re-experience past events better than the well-known Marcel Proust’s “madeleine moment.” Struggling to remember details of his childhood, Marcel tastes a petite madeleine soaked in lime-blossom tea, “and suddenly the memory revealed itself. The taste was that of the little piece of madeleinehad recalled nothing to my mind before I tasted it. And all from my cup of tea.” Tulving (1985) associated the subjective possibility to mentally travel in time with autonoetic consciousness, i.e., the sense of self we experience when we subjectively re-experience an event and mentally travel in time. Mental time travel relies on episodic autobiographical memory, which allows humans to mentally detach themselves from a current self-location and consciously identify themselves at another particular place and time (Hassabis et al., 2007; Schacter et al., 2012; Zaman and Russell, 2021).

The relation between the self and episodic autobiographical memory lies at the core of our understanding of consciousness. One’s autobiographical memories and sense of self are closely related. The self, i.e., the subjective feeling that defines us as unique human beings, is a critical component of consciousness (Damasio, 2003). Memory is necessary for everything we do and provides continuity from one moment to another. Memory creates our conscious sense of identity, which we construct based on self-relevant past events. As James (1890) suggested: “I enter a friend’s room and see on the wall a painting.” At first, I have the strange, wondering consciousness, “surely I have seen that before,” but when or how does not become clear. There only clings to the picture a sort of penumbra of familiarity—when suddenly I exclaim: “I have it, it is a copy of part of one of the Fra Angelico in the Florentine Academy—I recollect it there!”

Bodily-Self Influences the Autobiographical-Self

Episodic autobiographical memory studies have been recently conducted using new approaches to control memory encoding and reflect accurate life-like testing outside the laboratory setting. For example, St Jacques and Schacter (2013); Nielson et al. (2015), and Vogel and Schwabe (2016) developed paradigms in which participants encoded real-life events while wearing a camera that automatically took photos. Still, these studies did not integrate the natural occurrence of participants’ physical bodies during memory retrieval. Perceiving one’s own physical body as part of a visual scene, such as seeing one’s hand pointing at a painting during a museum tour or an animal in a zoo, relies on a multisensory integration of proprioceptive, visual, and tactile cues (Blanke et al., 2015). Subjective experiences create a link between episodic autobiographical memory and bodily self-consciousness, which suggests that the multisensory bodily signals may also be relevant to the conscious re-experiencing of self-relevant, past events.

Using virtual reality (VR) technology, Bergouignan et al. (2014) tested the encoding of real-life events from within one’s own body/first-person perspective compared to outside one’s own body/third-person perspective. Interestingly, the results showed episodic recollection deficits, specific to the events encoded in the outside body condition, associated with a diminished hippocampal activity (see Table 1). In a follow-up study (Bergouignan et al., 2021), the authors showed that encoding the real-life events from outside one’s own body led to more third-person perspective during the retrieval (see Table 1). Recently, we tested whether the congruent multisensory bodily cues, i.e., the presence or absence of one’s own physical body seen from a first-person perspective, would impact episodic autobiographical memory performance (Figure 1A). We used VR technology to create well-controlled, real life-like scenes into which participants, including their physical bodies, were immersed during the initial stage of encoding and later retrieval (Bréchet et al., 2019). We established that the presence of one’s own physical body during encoding enhanced memory recognition and that this effect was body-specific (see Table 1). In a follow-up study by Gauthier et al. (2020), we demonstrated that seeing one’s own body during encoding impacts the brain mechanisms responsible for episodic autobiographical memory formation by modulating the connectivity between the right hippocampal formation and the neocortical regions involved in the process of multisensory bodily signals and self-consciousness (see Table 1). In line with the previous work, Tacikowski et al. (2020) created an illusion of swapping participant’s own body with a friend’s body and then asked participants to perform personality rating and memory recognition tasks. The authors hypothesized that the perception of one’s own body (bodily-self) would influence beliefs about one’s own personality (conceptual-self) and that a coherent self-representation would lead to normal memory encoding. Indeed, they found that the experience of illusory ownership of a friend’s body changed participants’ beliefs about their own personality and made them more similar to the friend’s personality. Interestingly, they further showed that adjusting to the new bodily-self was beneficial for memory encoding, while incoherence between the bodily-self and conceptual-self leads to memory impairment (see Table 1). The findings from these recent studies point out that memory retrieval is impaired when (i) participants encode events from outside their own bodies, (ii) the body is absent during encoding, and (iii) ownership of one’s own body is reduced during encoding. These novel insights show that a coherent, multisensory representation of one’s own body leads to hippocampal binding mechanisms that are directed to the neocortical areas, which are involved in episodic autobiographical memory (Bergouignan et al., 2014; Gauthier et al., 2020; Iriye and St Jacques, 2020; Roehri et al., 2022).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Studies examining the relationship between bodily-self and autobiographical-self.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Study paradigm. First, participants incidentally learn the context of two different outside scenes (i.e., encoding session; 10 min). Participants were immersed back into the scenes with 1-h delay and were asked to perform a recognition task and subjective confidence ratings for each presented scene (i.e., retrieval session; 30 min). Before the actual study, subjects were seated in a chair, they were asked to put on the HMD and noise-canceling headphones to avoid external disturbances and familiarized themselves with the VR technology (10 min). (B) Visualization of the anatomical overlap between BSC fMRI analysis (i.e., self-location and first-person perspective) and EAM ALE analysis bilaterally in the parietal areas Label provided using the MRIcron. Within-cluster FEW-corrected p < 0.05 with p < 0.001 (uncorrected) as the cluster forming threshold.

Bodily-Self and Autobiographical-Self Representations Anatomically Overlap

Until recently, it has been unknown whether bodily self-consciousness and autobiographical memories, either episodic or semantic, involve distinct or similar brain regions. We examined whether experimental manipulation of the self-location and first-person perspective aspects of bodily self-consciousness may anatomically overlap with the activations related to the subjective conscious experience of remembering self-relevant past events (Bréchet et al., 2018). We included results from patients suffering from out-of-body experiences with abnormal self-location and first-person perspective, whose brain damage was localized in the inferior parietal lobule (Ionta et al., 2011). Our systematic meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies revealed an anatomical overlap bilaterally in the angular gyrus, specific to bodily-self and episodic autobiographical-self, while there was no overlap with semantic autobiographical-self (see Figure 1B and Table 1). This finding is supported by the emerging evidence from studies in healthy participants that shows that the lateral parietal cortex is critical for the subjective, conscious experience of retrieving multisensory episodic memories (Bonnici et al., 2016, 2018; Sestieri et al., 2017; Humphreys et al., 2021). Related to this, recent series of papers (St Jacques et al., 2017, 2018; Marcotti and St Jacques, 2018; Iriye and St Jacques, 2020) showed that shifting visual perspective during retrieval shapes accuracy and subjective vividness of memories, which activated the posterior parietal cortex (see Table 1). Emerging evidence from patients with lateral parietal lesions shows that damage to this brain region causes reduced subjective confidence, vividness, and richness when re-experiencing self-relevant memories (Simons et al., 2010; Berryhill, 2012; Hower et al., 2014) as well as egocentric episodic memory deficits (Russell et al., 2019). Recently, Penaud et al. (2022) showed that familiarity and self-perspective (i.e., centered on one’s own interaction with the environment) improved recall of past events, their spatiotemporal context and one’s sense of remembering (see Table 1).

Bodily-Self Retroactively Strengthens Autobiographical-Self

Many seemingly irrelevant everyday life events may become significant only later (Kensinger, 2015). For example, that stranger who asked for directions becomes more relevant after realizing that your wallet is missing. Recently, two behavioral studies showed how memory for neutral images could be enhanced by future fearful (Dunsmoor et al., 2015) or rewarding (Patil et al., 2017), conceptually related events. More precisely, during the first stage of incidental encoding, the so-called “pre-conditioning classification task,” two neutral categories of images portraying animals and tools appeared to be of the same relevance. During a second phase of the incidental encoding, the so-called “conditioning classification task,” a prominent event, either fear conditioning or reward motivation, became purposefully associated with one of the two categories (animals or tools). A memory recognition task revealed that participants remembered better neutral images (for example, tools) associated with fear or reward during the conditioning phase. They also remembered better the conceptually related images (tools) from the pre-conditioning phase.

Studies on mental self-projections (Arzy et al., 2008; Dafni-Merom and Arzy, 2020) suggest that the experience of the self in the present moment is also involved with the ability to remember our past or imagine the future. Furthermore, this conscious self-awareness (i.e., a mental state in which the content of one’s consciousness refers to knowledge about oneself, for example, reflecting about one’s personality or identity) is intrinsically connected to the multisensory bodily processes (Blanke et al., 2015; Tacikowski et al., 2020). Therefore, we examined whether the retroactive and selective effect could be (a) triggered by the multisensory bodily signals, such as the presence or absence of one’s own physical body and (b) generalized to naturalistic scenes, such as inside rooms or outdoor scenes, into which the participants would be immersed using VR technology (Bréchet et al., 2020). We showed that the presence of one’s own body can retroactively strengthen memory recognition and that this retroactively enhancing effect became selectively associated with a particular group of items (either from rooms or scenes) (see Table 1).

Summary and Future Directions

The behavioral and neuroimaging evidence reviewed here suggests that the fundamental aspects of self-consciousness, the bodily-self and autobiographical-self, critically interact and impact each other. We experience the world from an inside viewpoint of a body and from a physical location of a body, which we identify as our own. This sense of ownership that I am the one who currently experiences the world around me is also essential with respect to one’s past and future, therefore playing a significant role in constructing an autobiographical self. However, only a handful of recent studies (see Table 1) examined the relationship between bodily-self and autobiographical-self experimentally and showed that encoding events (i) from outside one’s own body, (ii) when one’s body is absent, (iii) when the sense of ownership of one’s body is reduced leads to impairment of episodic autobiographical memories. On the other hand, these studies also revealed the coherent multisensory bodily-self representations have beneficial, strengthening effects on autobiographical memory and may prevent memory loss.

Understanding the interactions between the core aspects of self-consciousness, bodily-self, and autobiographical-self has important clinical implications. Several studies have found that memory decline in aging is associated with a lack of vivid autobiographical memories and increased retrieval from a third-person perspective (Piolino et al., 2006, 2009). Both normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease in autobiographical memory are highly related to the self (Martinelli and Piolino, 2009; Martinelli et al., 2013). Alzheimer’s disease is a neurodegenerative, progressive disorder that distorts the autobiographical-self, which is tightly connected to the sense of agency (“I am the one who generates experiences”) and ownership (“I am the one who undergoes experiences”) in the world (Arzy and Schacter, 2019). Detecting these early bodily-self signs, which are often overlooked in Alzheimer’s disease, is of importance. One of the challenges for future research will be to reinstate a coherent sense of self to reduce autobiographical memory impairment.

Several open questions arise from these recent studies and their results. How can the neuroscientific methods ensure that people selectively remember what’s significant to them in real life rather than laboratory-based settings? How can we ensure that only some memories are enhanced while others are diminished? Would those mentioned above, experimental studies tested in healthy young participants, modify the bodily-self and autobiographical-self of Alzheimer’s patients in the same way? Is this the first step toward “a memory pacemaker”? More work is needed toward the long-term vision of restoring memory function. And perhaps more philosophically: What is more important: bodily-self or autobiographical-self? Quoting the words of Wiesel (2012): “Illness may diminish me, but it will not destroy me. The body is not eternal, but the idea of the soul is. The brain will be buried, but memory will survive it.”

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Funding

This work was supported by Synapsis Foundation – Alzheimer Research Switzerland ARS to LB (Career Development Award).

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The author thank Daniel Schacter for his pertinent comments and suggestions on this manuscript.

References

Arzy, S., Molnar-Szakacs, I., and Blanke, O. (2008). ‘Self in time: imagined self-location influences neural activity related to mental time travel’. J. Neurosci. 28, 6502–6507. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5712-07.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arzy, S., and Schacter, D. L. (2019). ‘Self-Agency and Self-Ownership in Cognitive Mapping’. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 476–487. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergouignan, L., Nyberg, L., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2014). ‘Out-of-body-induced hippocampal amnesia’. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 4421–4426. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1318801111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergouignan, L., Nyberg, L., and Henrik Ehrsson, H. (2021). Out-of-body memory encoding causes third-person perspective at recall. J. Cogn. Psychol. 34, 160–178.

Google Scholar

Berryhill, M. E. (2012). ‘Insights from neuropsychology: pinpointing the role of the posterior parietal cortex in episodic and working memory’. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:31. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanke, O., Slater, M., and Serino, A. (2015). ‘Behavioral, Neural, and Computational Principles of Bodily Self-Consciousness. Neuron 88, 145–166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonnici, H. M., Cheke, L. G., Green, D. A. E., FitzGerald, T., and Simons, J. S. (2018). Specifying a Causal Role for Angular Gyrus in Autobiographical Memory. J. Neurosci. 38, 10438–10443. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1239-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonnici, H. M., Richter, F. R., Yazar, Y., and Simons, J. S. (2016). ‘Multimodal Feature Integration in the Angular Gyrus during Episodic and Semantic Retrieval’. J. Neurosci. 36, 5462–5471. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4310-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bréchet, L., Grivaz, P., Gauthier, B., and Blanke, O. (2018). ‘Common Recruitment of Angular Gyrus in Episodic Autobiographical Memory and Bodily Self-Consciousness’. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:270. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00270

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bréchet, L., Hausmann, S. B., Mange, R., Herbelin, B., Blanke, O., and Serino, A. (2020). ‘Subjective feeling of re-experiencing past events using immersive virtual reality prevents a loss of episodic memory’. Brain Behav. 10:e01571. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bréchet, L., Mange, R., Herbelin, B., Theillaud, Q., Gauthier, B., Serino, A., et al. (2019). ‘First-person view of one’s body in immersive virtual reality: influence on episodic memory’. PLoS One 14:e0197763. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197763

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dafni-Merom, A., and Arzy, S. (2020). ‘The radiation of autonoetic consciousness in cognitive neuroscience: a functional neuroanatomy perspective’. Neuropsychologia 143:107477. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damasio, A. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens: Body, Emotion and the Making of Consciousness. San Diego: Harcourt Brace.

Google Scholar

Damasio, A. (2003). Feelings of emotion and the self. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1001, 253–261.

Google Scholar

Dunsmoor, J. E., Murty, V. P., Davachi, L., and Phelps, E. A. (2015). ‘Emotional learning selectively and retroactively strengthens memories for related events’. Nature 520, 345–348. doi: 10.1038/nature14106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrsson, H. H. (2007). ‘The experimental induction of out-of-body experiences’. Science 317:1048. doi: 10.1126/science.1142175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gauthier, B., Bréchet, L., Lance, F., Mange, R., Herbelin, B., Faivre, N., et al. (2020). ‘First-person body view modulates the neural substrates of episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness: a functional connectivity study’. Neuroimage 223:117370. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., Vann, S. D., and Maguire, E. A. (2007). ‘Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences’. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 1726–1731. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610561104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hower, K. H., Wixted, J., Berryhill, M. E., and Olson, I. R. (2014). ‘Impaired perception of mnemonic oldness, but not mnemonic newness, after parietal lobe damage’. Neuropsychologia 56, 409–417. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphreys, G. F., Lambon Ralph, M. A., and Simons, J. S. (2021). ‘A Unifying Account of Angular Gyrus Contributions to Episodic and Semantic Cognition’. Trends Neurosci. 44, 452–463. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2021.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ionta, S., Heydrich, L., Lenggenhager, B., Mouthon, M., Fornari, E., Chapuis, D., et al. (2011). ‘Multisensory mechanisms in temporo-parietal cortex support self-location and first-person perspective’. Neuron 70, 363–374. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iriye, H., and St Jacques, P. L. (2020). ‘How visual perspective influences the spatiotemporal dynamics of autobiographical memory retrieval’. Cortex 129, 464–475. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iriye, H., and St. Jacques, P. L. (2021). Memories for third-person experiences in immersive virtual reality. Sci. Rep. 11:4667. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-84047-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover Publications.

Google Scholar

Kensinger, E. A. (2015). ‘The future can shape memory for the present’. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 179–180. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenggenhager, B., Tadi, T., Metzinger, T., Blanke, O., Ehrsson, H. H., Wiech, K., et al. (2007). ‘Video ergo sum: manipulating bodily self-consciousness. Threatening a rubber hand that you feel is yours elicits a cortical anxiety response. Science 317, 1096–1099.

Google Scholar

Mach, E. (1897). The Analysis of Sensations and the Relation of the Physical to the Psychical. La Salle, IL: Open Court.

Google Scholar

Marcotti, P., and St Jacques, P. L. (2018). ‘Shifting visual perspective during memory retrieval reduces the accuracy of subsequent memories’. Memory 26, 330–341. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1329441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinelli, P., Anssens, A., Sperduti, M., and Piolino, P. (2013). ‘The influence of normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease in autobiographical memory highly related to the self’. Neuropsychology 27, 69–78. doi: 10.1037/a0030453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinelli, P., and Piolino, P. (2009). ‘[Self-defining memories: last episodic memories bastion in normal aging?]’. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 7, 151–167. doi: 10.1684/pnv.2009.0178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielson, D. M., Smith, T. A., Sreekumar, V., Dennis, S., and Sederberg, P. B. (2015). ‘Human hippocampus represents space and time during retrieval of real-world memories’. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 11078–11083. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1507104112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigro, G., and Neisser, U. (1983). ‘Point of view in personal memories’. Cogn. Psychol. 15, 467–482. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2015.1137323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, H. D., and Blanke, O. (2019). ‘Coupling Inner and Outer Body for Self-Consciousness’. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 377–388. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patil, A., Murty, V. P., Dunsmoor, J. E., Phelps, E. A., and Davachi, L. (2017). ‘Reward retroactively enhances memory consolidation for related items’. Learn Mem. 24, 65–69. doi: 10.1101/lm.042978.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penaud, S., Jebara, N., Zaoui, M., Orriols, E., Berthoz, A., and Piolino, P. (2022). ‘Episodic memory and self-reference in a naturalistic context: new insights based on a virtual walk in the Latin Quarter of Paris’. J. Environ. Psychol. 81:101801.

Google Scholar

Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., Clarys, D., Guillery-Girard, B., Taconnat, L., Isingrini, M., et al. (2006). ‘Autobiographical memory, autonoetic consciousness, and self-perspective in aging’. Psychol. Aging 21, 510–525. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., and Eustache, F. (2009). ‘Episodic autobiographical memories over the course of time: cognitive, neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings’. Neuropsychologia 47, 2314–2329. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roehri, N., Bréchet, L., Seeber, M., Pascual-Leone, A., and Michel, C. M. (2022). ‘Phase-Amplitude Coupling and Phase Synchronization Between Medial Temporal, Frontal and Posterior Brain Regions Support Episodic Autobiographical Memory Recall’. Brain Topogr. 35, 191–206. doi: 10.1007/s10548-022-00890-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, D. C., and Umanath, S. (2015). ‘Event memory: a theory of memory for laboratory, autobiographical, and fictional events’. Psychol. Rev. 122, 1–23. doi: 10.1037/a0037907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, C., Davies, S., Li, K., Musil, A. S., Malhotra, P. A., and Williams, A. L. (2019). ‘Self-perspective in episodic memory after parietal damage and in healthy ageing’. Neuropsychologia 124, 171–181. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., Martin, V. C., Spreng, R. N., and Szpunar, K. K. (2012). ‘The future of memory: remembering, imagining, and the brain’. Neuron 76, 677–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sestieri, C., Shulman, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2017). ‘The contribution of the human posterior parietal cortex to episodic memory’. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 183–192. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simons, J. S., Peers, P. V., Mazuz, Y. S., Berryhill, M. E., and Olson, I. R. (2010). ‘Dissociation between memory accuracy and memory confidence following bilateral parietal lesions’. Cereb. Cortex 20, 479–485. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

St Jacques, P. L., Carpenter, A. C., Szpunar, K. K., and Schacter, D. L. (2018). Remembering and imagining alternative versions of the personal past. Neuropsychologia 110, 170–179. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

St Jacques, P. L., and Schacter, D. L. (2013). Modifying memory: selectively enhancing and updating personal memories for a museum tour by reactivating them. Psychol. Sci. 24, 537–543. doi: 10.1177/0956797612457377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

St Jacques, P. L., Szpunar, K. K., and Schacter, D. L. (2017). Shifting visual perspective during retrieval shapes autobiographical memories. Neuroimage 148, 103–114. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tacikowski, P., Weijs, M. L., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2020). ‘Perception of Our Own Body Influences Self-Concept and Self-Incoherence Impairs Episodic Memory. iScience 23:101429. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2020.101429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulving, E. (1985). ‘Memory and consciousness’. Can. Psychol. 26, 1–12.

Google Scholar

Vogel, S., and Schwabe, L. (2016). ‘Stress in the zoo: tracking the impact of stress on memory formation over time’. Psychoneuroendocrinology 71, 64–72. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.04.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiesel, E. (2012). Open Heart. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.

Google Scholar

Zaman, A., and Russell, C. (2021). Does autonoetic consciousness in episodic memory rely on recall from a first-person perspective? J. Cogn. Psychol. 34, 9–23.

Google Scholar

Keywords: bodily-self, autobiographical-self, out-of-body experiences, view-points, autonoetic consciousness, fMRI, VR

Citation: Bréchet L (2022) Personal Memories and Bodily-Cues Influence Our Sense of Self. Front. Psychol. 13:855450. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.855450

Received: 15 January 2022; Accepted: 02 June 2022;
Published: 22 June 2022.

Edited by:

Antonino Raffone, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Reviewed by:

Jane Elizabeth Aspell, Anglia Ruskin University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Bréchet. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lucie Bréchet, lucie.brechet@unige.ch

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.